Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T15:31:25.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Eye movement patterns and visual attention during scene viewing in 3- to 12-month-olds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2016

ANDREA HELO*
Affiliation:
Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France Departamento de Fonoaudiología, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
PIA RÄMÄ
Affiliation:
Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France CNRS (UMR 8242), Paris, France
SEBASTIAN PANNASCH
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Engineering Psychology and Applied Cognitive Research, Technische Universitaet Dresden, Dresden, Germany
DAVID MEARY
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, Université Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, Grenoble, France
*
*Address correspondence to: Andrea Helo, 45 rue des Saint-Pères, 75006 Paris, France. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Recently, two attentional modes have been associated with specific eye movement patterns during scene processing. Ambient mode, characterized by short fixations and long saccades during early scene inspection, is associated with localization of objects. Focal mode, characterized by longer fixations, is associated with more detailed object feature processing during later inspection phase. The aim of the present study was to investigate the development of these attentional modes. More specifically, we examined whether indications of ambient and focal attention modes are similar in infants and adults. Therefore, we measured eye movements in 3- to 12-months-old infants while exploring visual scenes. Our results show that both adults and 12-month-olds had shorter fixation durations within the first 1.5 s of scene viewing compared with later time phases (>2.5 s); indicating that there was a transition from ambient to focal processing during image inspection. In younger infants, fixation durations between two viewing phases did not differ. Our results suggest that at the end of the first year of life, infants have developed an adult-like scene viewing behavior. The evidence for the existence of distinct attentional processing mechanisms during early infancy furthermore underlines the importance of the concept of the two modes.

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aslin, R.N. & Salapatek, P. (1975). Saccadic localization of visual targets by the very young human infant. Perception & Psychophysics 17(3), 293302. doi: 10.3758/BF03203214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, H. & Carchon, I. (1992). On the onset of eye-head coordination in infants. Behavioural Brain Research 49(1), 8590. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80197-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronson, G. (1990). Changes in infants’ visual scanning across the 2- to 14-week age period. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 49(1), 101125. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90051-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronson, G. (1991). Infant differences in rate of visual encoding. Child Development 62(1), 4454. doi: 10.2307/1130703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronson, G. (1994). Infants’ transitions toward adult-like scanning. Child Development 65(5), 12431261. doi: 10.2307/1131497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castelhano, M.S. & Henderson, J.M. (2007). Initial scene representations facilitate eye movement guidance in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33(4), 753763. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.753.Google Scholar
Castelhano, M.S. & Henderson, J.M. (2008). Stable individual differences across images in human saccadic eye movements. Canadian Journal Experimental Psychology 62(1), 114. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.62.1.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castelhano, M.S., Mack, M.L. & Henderson, J.M. (2009). Viewing task influences eye movement control during active scene perception. Journal of Vision 9(3), 115. doi: 10.1167/9.3.6.Introduction.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandna, A. (1991). Natural history of the development of visual acuity in infants. Eye 5, 2026. doi: 10.1038/eye.1991.4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infacy. Annual Review of Psychology 52, 337367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, J., Freeseman, L.J., Coldren, J.T. & Frick, J.E. (1995). Individual differences in infant fixation duration: Dominance of global versus local stimulus properties. Cognitive Development 10(2), 271285. doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(95)90012-8.Google Scholar
Colombo, J., Mitchell, D.W., Coldren, J.T. & Freeseman, L.J. (1991). Individual differences in infant visual attention: Are short lookers faster processors or feature processors? Child Development 62(6), 12471257. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01603.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courage, M.L., Reynolds, G.D. & Richards, J.E. (2006). Infants’ attention to patterned stimuli: Developmental change from 3 to 12 months of age. Child Development 77(3), 680695. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00897.x.Google Scholar
Daniel, B.M. & Lee, D.N. (1990). Development of looking with head and eyes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 50(2), 200216. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90039-B.Google Scholar
Follet, B., Le Meur, O. & Baccino, T. (2011). New insights into ambient and focal visual fixations using an automatic classification algorithm. I-Perception 2(6), 592610. doi: 10.1068/i0414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilmore, R. & Johnson, M.H. (1995). Working memory in infancy: Six-month-olds’ performance on two versions of the oculomotor delayed response task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 59, 397418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helo, A., Pannasch, S., Sirri, L. & Rämä, P. (2014). The maturation of eye movement behavior: Scene viewing characteristics in children and adults. Vision Research 103, 8391. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.006.Google Scholar
Henderson, J.M. (2007). Regarding scenes. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16(4), 219222. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00507.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hood, B.M. & Atkinson, J. (1993). Disengaging visual attention in the infant and adult. Infant Behavior and Development 16(4), 405422. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80001-O.Google Scholar
Ingle, D. (1967). Two visual mechanisms underlying the behavior of fish. Psychologische Forschung 31(1), 4451. doi: 10.1007/BF00422385.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.H. (1995). The inhibition of automatic saccades in early infancy. Developmental Psychobiology 28(5), 281291. doi: 10.1002/dev.420280504.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.H. (2002). The development of visual attention: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader (2nd ed.), ed. Johnson, M.H., Munakata, Y. & Gilmore, R.O., Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.H., Posner, M.I. & Rothbart, M.K. (1994). Facilitation of saccades toward a covertly attended location in early infancy. Psychological Science 5(2), 9092. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00636.x.Google Scholar
Luna, B., Velanova, K. & Geier, C.F. (2008). Development of eye-movement control. Brain and Cognition 68(3), 293308. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.019.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, M. & Shimojo, S. (1997). Infants’ fast saccades in the gap paradigm and development of visual attention. Infant Behavior and Development 20(4), 449455. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90035-7.Google Scholar
Mills, M., Hollingworth, A. & Dodd, M.D. (2011). Examining the influence of task set on eye movements and fixations. Journal of Vision 11(8), 115. doi: 10.1167/11.8.17.Introduction.Google Scholar
Nakagawa, A. & Sukigara, M. (2013). Variable coordination of eye and head movements during the early development of attention: A longitudinal study of infants aged 12–36 months. Infant Behavior and Development 36(4), 517525. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.04.002.Google Scholar
Pannasch, S., Helmert, J.R., Roth, K. & Walter, H. (2008). Visual fixation durations and saccade amplitudes: Shifting relationship in a variety of conditions. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2(2), 119.Google Scholar
Pannasch, S., Schulz, J. & Velichkovsky, B.M. (2011). On the control of visual fixation durations in free viewing of complex images. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 73(4), 11201132. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0090-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, J.O., Finocchio, D.V., Ong, L. & Fuchs, A.F. (1997). Smooth pursuit in 1-to 4-month-old human infants. Vision Research 37(21), 30093020.Google Scholar
Potter, M. (1976). Short-term conceptual memory for pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory 2(5), 509522. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.2.5.509.Google Scholar
Regal, D.M., Ashmead, D.H. & Salapatek, P. (1983). The coordiantion of eye and head movements during early infancy: A selective review. Behavioural Brain Research 10, 125132.Google Scholar
Rosander, K. & Von Hofsten, C. (2002). Development of gaze tracking of small and large objects. Experimental Brain Research 146(2), 257264. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1161-2.Google Scholar
Roucoux, A., Culee, C. & Roucoux, M. (1983). Development of fixation and pursuit eye movements in human infants. Behavioural Brain Research 10(1), 133139. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(83)90159-6.Google Scholar
Salapatek, P., Aslin, R.N., Simonson, J. & Pulos, E. (1980). Infant saccadic eye movements to visible and previously visible targets. Child Development 51(4), 10901094.Google Scholar
Shea, S.L. & Aslin, R.N. (1990). Oculomotor responses to step-ramp targets by young human infants. Vision Research 30(7), 10771092. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90116-3.Google Scholar
Scinto, L., Pillalamarri, R. & Karsh, R. (1986). Cognitive strategies for visual search. Acta Psychologica 62(3), 263292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(86)90091-0.Google Scholar
Schyns, P. & Oliva, A. (1994). FROM BLOBS TO BOUNDARY EDGES:. Evidence for Time- and Spatial-Scale-Dependent Scene Recognition. Psychological Science 5(4), 195200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00500.x.Google Scholar
Slater, A.M. & Findlay, J.M. (1975). Binocular Fixation in the newborn baby. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20, 248273.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C.B. (1968). Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung 31(4), 299337. doi: 10.1007/BF00422717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unema, P.J.A., Pannasch, S., Joos, M. & Velichkovsky, B.M. (2005). Time course of information processing during scene perception: The relationship between saccade amplitude and fixation duration. Visual Cognition 12(3), 473494. doi: 10.1080/13506280444000409.Google Scholar
Velichkovsky, B.M., Dornhoefer, S.M., Pannasch, S. & Unema, P.J.A. (2000). Visual fixations and level of attentional processing. In Duhowski, A. (Ed.), Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications (pp. 7985). Palm Beach Gardens, NY: ACM Press.Google Scholar
Velichkovsky, B., Rothert, A., Kopf, M., Dornhöfer, S. & Joos, M. (2002). Towards an express-diagnostics for level of processing and hazard perception. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 5(2), 145156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1369-8478(02)00013-x.Google Scholar
Von Hofsten, C. & Rosander, K. (1997). Development of smooth-pursuit tracking in young infants. Vision Research 37(13), 17991810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wass, S.V. & Smith, T.J. (2014). Individual differences in infant oculomotor behavior during the viewing of complex naturalistic scenes. Infancy 19(4), 352384. doi: 10.1111/infa.12049.Google Scholar