Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:29:55.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variants Near CCK Receptors are Associated With Electrophysiological Responses to Pre-pulse Startle Stimuli in a Mexican American Cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2015

Trina M. Norden-Krichmar
Affiliation:
Department of Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA Department of Epidemiology, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA
Ian R. Gizer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
Evelyn Phillips
Affiliation:
Department of Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA
Kirk C. Wilhelmsen
Affiliation:
Department of Genetics and Neurology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, N.C., USA
Nicholas J. Schork
Affiliation:
Department of Human Biology, J. Craig Venter Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA
Cindy L. Ehlers*
Affiliation:
Department of Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA
*
address for correspondence: Cindy L. Ehlers, Department of Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA 92037USA. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Neurophysiological measurements of the response to pre-pulse and startle stimuli have been suggested to represent an important endophenotype for both substance dependence and other select psychiatric disorders. We have previously shown, in young adult Mexican Americans (MA), that presentation of a short delay acoustic pre-pulse, prior to the startle stimuli can elicit a late negative component at about 400 msec (N4S), in the event-related potential (ERP), recorded from frontal cortical areas. In the present study, we investigated whether genetic factors associated with this endophenotype could be identified. The study included 420 (age 18–30 years) MA men (n = 170), and women (n = 250). DNA was genotyped using an Affymetrix Axiom Exome1A chip. An association analysis revealed that the CCKAR and CCKBR (cholecystokinin A and B receptor) genes each had a nearby variant that showed suggestive significance with the amplitude of the N4S component to pre-pulse stimuli. The neurotransmitter cholecystokinin (CCK), along with its receptors, CCKAR and CCKBR, have been previously associated with psychiatric disorders, suggesting that variants near these genes may play a role in the pre-pulse/startle response in this cohort.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2015 

The identification of neurophysiological endophenotypes associated with psychiatric disorders may help in determining the causal relationship between clinical phenomena associated with the disorder and basic molecular processes that are in large part determined by genetic factors. One psychophysiological measure that has been used as a potential endophenotype for a number of psychiatric disorders is the acoustic startle reflex (ASR) and pre-pulse inhibition of the startle (PPI). The startle reflex is a constellation of responses usually indexed by eye blink responses (Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Caine, Braff and Geyer1992), but also by electrophysiological recordings from cortical areas that may index cognitive responses to startle (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Phillips, Criado and Gilder2011; Ford et al., Reference Ford, White, Lim and Pfefferbaum1994, Reference Ford, Roth, Menon and Pfefferbaum1999; Putnam & Roth, Reference Putnam and Roth1990). PPI refers to the fact that if a weak stimulus is presented prior to the presentation of the startle stimuli (pre-pulse) the response to the startle is reduced in amplitude. It has been suggested that pre-pulse inhibition is an index of automatic sensorimotor gating (Geyer & Swerdlow, Reference Geyer and Swerdlow2001). In pre-pulse facilitation (PPF), the response to the startle is enhanced by short or long delay pre-pulses. PPF has been suggested to reflect a combination of alerting, attention and/ or arousal (Filion et al., Reference Filion, Dawson and Schell1998; Hsieh et al., Reference Hsieh, Swerdlow and Braff2006; Ludewig et al., Reference Ludewig, Ludewig, Seitz, Obrist, Geyer and Vollenweider2003).

The anatomical substrates of the neurobehavioral responses (ASR/PPI/PPF) to the presentation of the startle stimuli have been extensively investigated in clinical and pre-clinical studies (Braff et al., Reference Braff, Geyer, Light, Sprock, Perry, Cadenhead and Swerdlow2001a, Reference Braff, Geyer and Swerdlow2001b; Kumari et al., Reference Kumari, Antonova, Zachariah, Galea, Aasen, Ettinger and Sharma2005; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Braff, Taaid and Geyer1994). Startle responses involve a complex neural network extending from brainstem nuclei via the thalamus to higher order cortical areas that may regulate cognitive responses to startle (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Hughes, Budd, Cooper, Fulham, Karayanidis and Schall2007; Fendt et al., Reference Fendt, Li and Yeomans2001; Kumari et al., Reference Kumari, Antonova, Zachariah, Galea, Aasen, Ettinger and Sharma2005; Neuner et al., Reference Neuner, Stocker, Kellermann, Ermer, Wegener, Eickhoff and Shah2010; Schall et al., Reference Schall, Catts, Karayanidis and Ward1999). There is some evidence that the cognitive response to ASR/PPI may share a common underlying neurophysiology with some behavioral and clinical measures of cognition that require response inhibition (Filion et al., Reference Filion, Kelly, Hazlett, Dawson, Schell and Bohmelt1999). For instance, both performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task and PPI of startle have been suggested to reflect pre-frontal cortical function and dysfunction (Filion et al., Reference Filion, Kelly, Hazlett, Dawson, Schell and Bohmelt1999; Swerdlow & Geyer, Reference Swerdlow, Geyer, Dawson, Schell and Bohmelt1999). Impairments in frontal lobe function and associated behaviors, such as executive functioning, have been an important theoretical construct in the understanding a number of behavioral disorders, such as schizophrenia (Bagney et al., Reference Bagney, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Martinez-Gras, Sanchez-Morla, Santos, Jimenez-Arriero and Parg2013; Chan et al., Reference Chan, Chan, Hui, Wong, Chang, Lee and Chen2014; Eisenberg & Berman, Reference Eisenberg and Berman2010; Holmen et al., Reference Holmen, Juuhl-Langseth, Thormodsen, Ueland, Agartz, Sundet and Melle2012; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Johnstone, Miller, Macmillan and Crow2010), bipolar disorders (Erol et al., Reference Erol, Kosger, Putgul and Ersoy2014; Kulkarni et al., Reference Kulkarni, Jain, Reddy, Kumar and Kandavel2010; Yen et al., Reference Yen, Cheng, Huang, Ko, Yen, Chang and Chen2009; Zimmerman et al., Reference Zimmerman, DelBello, Getz, Shear and Strakowski2006) and substance use disorders (Fernandez-Serrano et al., Reference Fernandez-Serrano, Perez-Garcia, Schmidt Rio-Valle and Verdejo-Garcia2010; Gierski et al., Reference Gierski, Hubsch, Stefaniak, Benzerouk, Cuervo-Lombard, Bera-Potelle and Limosin2013; Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Vollstadt-Klein, von der Goltz, Flor, Mann and Kiefer2009; Maurage et al., Reference Maurage, de Timary, Billieux, Collignon and Heeren2014; van der Plas et al., Reference van der Plas, Crone, van den Wildenberg, Tranel and Bechara2009; Zorko et al., Reference Zorko, Marusic, Cebasek-Travnik and Bucik2004). If frontal cortical responses to the startle stimuli index some aspect of frontal lobe functioning involving higher cognition, then psychiatric disorders postulated to involve some aspects of frontal lobe dysfunction should also have startle deficits, such as anxiety disorders (De Pascalis et al., Reference De Pascalis, Cozzuto and Russo2013), schizophrenia (De Koning et al., Reference De Koning, Bloemen, Van Duin, Booij, Abel, De Haan and Van Amelsvoort2014; Swerdlow et al., Reference Swerdlow, Light, Sprock, Calkins, Green, Greenwood and Braff2014), post-traumatic stress disorder (Grillon et al., Reference Grillon, Morgan, Southwick, Davis and Charney1996), and alcoholism (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Phillips, Criado and Gilder2011; Marin et al., Reference Marin, Ponce, Martinez-Gras, Koeneke, Curivil, Jimenez-Arriero and Rubio2012).

The present investigation used a startle paradigm with short delay pre-pulse-plus-startle stimuli, which elicits a large frontal negative slow wave designated the N4S component (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Phillips, Criado and Gilder2011). In the present study, we used this endophenotype in an association analysis using an Affymetrix Axiom Exome1A array to explore potential genetic factors underlying the N4S component response to pre-pulse startle stimuli in a young adult MA cohort that has been previously well-characterized clinically (Criado & Ehlers, Reference Criado and Ehlers2007; Criado et al., Reference Criado, Gizer, Edenberg and Ehlers2014; Ehlers & Phillips, Reference Ehlers and Phillips2007; Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Gilder, Criado and Caetano2009, Reference Ehlers, Gilder, Criado and Caetano2010, Reference Ehlers, Phillips, Criado and Gilder2011, Reference Ehlers, Liang and Gizer2012, Reference Ehlers, Stouffer and Gilder2014).

Materials and Methods

Sample Ascertainment

To investigate risk and protective factors for the pre-pulse inhibition and startle response in a select population of MA young adults, we investigated a cohort of 420 (age 18–30 years) MA men (n = 170) and women (n = 250). Participants were recruited using a commercial mailing list that provided the addresses of individuals with Hispanic surnames in 11 zip codes in San Diego County. The mailed invitation stated that potential participants must be of MA heritage, between the ages of 18 and 30 years, residing in the United States legally, and able to read and write in English. Based on a phone interview, participants were excluded if they were pregnant, were nursing, or currently had a major medical or neurological disorder or head injury. All participants were identified as having over 20% Hispanic heritage, with 92% reporting over 50% Hispanic heritage. The Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA; Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, Cadoret, Cloninger, Dinwiddie, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger and Schuckit1994) was used to make lifetime substance use and other psychiatric disorder diagnoses, according to DSM-III-R and DSM-IV criteria. There have been several studies that have evaluated the concurrent diagnostic validity of the SSAGA across alcohol and drug dependencies, major depression, anxiety disorders, and antisocial personality disorder (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, Cadoret, Cloninger, Dinwiddie, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger and Schuckit1994; Hesselbrock et al., Reference Hesselbrock, Easton, Bucholz, Schuckit and Hesselbrock1999). These findings indicate that the SSAGA is a highly reliable and valid instrument for use in studies of psychiatric disorders, including substance dependence. The protocol for the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the Scripps Research Institute, and written consent was obtained for all participants. Participants were asked to refrain from alcohol and drug usage for 24 hours prior to the testing.

Startle ERP Collection and Analysis

Recordings were obtained from participants who were seated on a hospital bed in a sound-attenuated room. Acoustic startle stimuli were presented binaurally through headphones. The behavioral response to the startle (eye blink) is recorded using electrodes placed below and lateral to the eye as described (Braff et al., Reference Braff, Geyer and Swerdlow2001b). The auditory stimuli consist of 45 trials. These trials include randomly presented startle stimuli (115 dB white noise burst for 40 msec n = 30) and pre-pulse-startle stimuli (85 dB white noise burst for 20 msec-duration) immediately (<5 msec) followed by the startle (115 dB white noise burst for 40 msec n = 15). Each individual startle and/or pre-pulse startle trial is separated by an interval of 15 seconds. Background white noise was presented for the entire session at a level of 60 dB. The behavioral variables assessed included: ASR magnitude on startle trials and pre-pulse trials as determined by quantification of the eye blink response as described below.

Seven channels of ERP data (FZ, CZ, PZ, F3, F4, F7, and F8, referenced to linked ear lobes with a forehead ground, international 10–20 system) were obtained using gold-plated electrodes with impedance held below 5 KΩ. Frontal electrodes were emphasized in the montage as previous data had suggested that ERP decrements in frontal areas distinguished subjects with a risk for alcohol dependence (Bauer, Reference Bauer1997). An electrode placed left lateral infraorbitally and reference to the left earlobe was used to monitor both horizontal and vertical eye movements. ERP signals were amplified (time constant 0.3 s, 35 Hz low pass) using a Nihon Kohden EEG machine and were transferred online to a PC. The combined gain of the EEG amplifiers and the analog-to-digital multiplexer amplifier was 50 K.

The eye blink and ERP trials were simultaneously digitized at a rate of 256 Hz (bandwidth 0.5–35 Hz). Individual trials where the EEG or eye blink exceeded ±250 microvolts (<5% of the trials) were eliminated before averaging. The N4S component of the ERP was quantified using a computerized peak detection routine that identifies baseline-to-peak amplitudes (in μV) within the specified latency window (350–500 msec). The eye blink amplitude was also assessed using this routine. The latency window for the eye blink was 50–120 msec. The baseline was determined by averaging the 150 ms of pre-stimulus activity obtained for each trial. The routine is user-driven, and each peak detection must be verified by the user. All peaks were quantified by one investigator, and verified by a second investigator, both of whom were blind to participants’ characteristics.

The N4S component of the ERP was also evaluated to determine if it was altered as a function of alcohol dependence, antisocial personality disorder/conduct disorder (ASPD/CD), affective/anxiety disorders (ANYAXAF), and any other drug dependence (AnyDrugDep). In this analysis, regionally averaged N4S component responses to startle and pre-pulse/startle were compared between those participants with and without the psychiatric disorders using ANCOVA (co-varying for gender).

Sample Preparation and Genotyping

For all subjects, DNA was extracted from blood samples, followed by genotyping using an Affymetrix Exome1A chip. The DNA samples were prepared and the exome chip genotyping was performed on the Affymetrix Axiom Exome 1A Array according to the Affymetrix Axiom 2.0 Assay Manual Workflow documentation. The Affymetrix Exome 1A chip contains 247,222 markers. Variant quality from the exome chip genotyping was initially assessed according to Affymetrix best practices (Affymetrix, 2011). Plink version 1.07 (Purcell et al., Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira, Bender and Sham2007) was used to calculate Hardy-Weinberg (HWE) p values on the set of unrelated samples, followed by the removal of 653 variants with an HWE p < 10−10.

Association Analysis

PLINK was used to test for genome-wide association for the N4S component of the ERP in response to acoustic startle stimuli following the presentation of a pre-pulse. PLINK was run with linear regression model parameters and with one million permutations. Gender and age were included as covariates. To determine the effect of extreme outliers in the phenotypic values, custom R code was written to generate winsorized phenotype values at 5% and 95% cut-offs, which were then used as the phenotype values in PLINK. Manhattan plots were generated using Manhattan R library (Stephen Turner, http://gettinggeneticsdone.blogspot.com/2011/04/annotated-manhattan-plots-and-qq-plots.html). Annotations of the variants were obtained from the Affymetrix Exome 1A chip description file. Multiple test correction p-value thresholds were calculated for the Affymetrix Exome1A chip using the Genetic Type 1 Error Calculator (GEC) software (Li et al., Reference Li, Yeung, Cherny and Sham2012). The UCSC Genome Browser (Kent et al., Reference Kent, Sugnet, Furey, Roskin, Pringle, Zahler and Haussler2002) was also utilized to visualize the genomic region containing the significant SNPs.

In order to determine if the SNPs that were significantly associated with the pre-pulse response phenotype were shared with the alcohol dependence trait, we used the multivariate version of the PLINK software (Ferreira & Purcell, Reference Ferreira and Purcell2009) with covariates age and gender.

LD Analysis

In order to check for linkage disequilibrium (LD) across the CCKAR and CCKBR gene regions, PLINK was utilized to extract a subset of variants for analysis based on the physical position of these genes on chromosomes 4 and 11, respectively. In particular, a region of chromosome 4 from genomic location 25,450,000–28,000,000 and chromosome 11 from genomic location 6,100,000–6,500,000, were extracted from the data set. Haploview (Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Fry, Maller and Daly2005) was used to calculate the LD statistics and visualize the haplotype block structure of the gene regions.

Results

Demographics of the Mexican American Population

The demographics for the full sample of individuals (N = 420) that were included in the association analysis are shown in Table 1. The subjects were a mean age of 23.6 (range 18–30) years at the time of interview, with 40% of the sample being male and 60% of the sample being female. Participants had a mean of 13.3 years of education (SD = 1.8), and a mean income of $30,000–$49,000. Using self-reported ancestry based on grandparent origin, 92% of the participants reported at least 50% Hispanic heritage. The mean BMI was 27 (SD = 7, range 17–64). Approximately 29% (n = 123) of the participants were diagnosed with alcohol dependence according to DSM-III-R guidelines, indicating that these subjects had symptoms from three or more symptom groups, out of nine possible symptom groups. Eleven percent (n = 45) of the participants were diagnosed with conduct disorder or ASPD/CD. Thirty-one percent (n = 132) of the participants has a lifetime diagnosis of affective and/or anxiety disorder. Twenty-seven percent (n = 115) of the participants had a diagnosis of another drug disorder (nicotine, cannabis, hallucinogens, stimulants, sedatives, opioids).

TABLE 1 Demographics for Mexican American Study Participants

The mean N4S amplitude was evaluated using ANCOVA (co-varying with gender) as a function of alcohol dependence, ASPD/CD, any affective and/or anxiety disorder, and any other drug dependency. Participants with a lifetime diagnosis of alcohol dependence had significantly increased amplitude N4S responses to pre-pulse/startle stimuli as compared to participants with no alcohol dependence diagnoses (F = 6.535; p = .011). There were no significant associations between the N4S amplitude to pre-pulse stimuli phenotypic trait and ASPD/CD, ANYAXAF, or AnyDrugDep diagnoses.

Association Analysis

Figure 1 contains the Manhattan plot for the amplitude of the N4S response to pre-pulse stimuli phenotypic trait tested in the association analysis across the entire genome, using covariates age and gender, and applying a minor allele cut-off of 0.01. Although there were five total variants which exhibited p values ≤ E-05 after association, as shown in Table 2, there were only two variants (rs2171755 and rs58905541) that showed suggestive significance and possessed plausible gene functions for this phenotype. One protective variant (rs2171755; NC_000004.12:g.26502338T>C) is located 12 kb upstream from the CCKAR gene in a MIRb class SINE repetitive element, and is common in our sample, with an allele frequency of 0.36. The rs58905541 variant is a risk variant (NC_000011.10:g.6296157C>T) located 24 kb downstream from the CCKBR gene in a DNaseI hypersensitivity cluster, with an allele frequency of 0.018. The variant retained its significance through permutation and winsorization and the exclusion of covariates age and gender. The association values for these two variants, along with the allele and genotype frequencies, are shown in Table 3. The minor allele frequencies of these variants in the 1,000 Genomes project was obtained from the dbSNP website (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/SNP/).

TABLE 2 Significant Variants for Pre-pulse Phenotype

TABLE 3 Allele Frequencies, Genotype Frequencies, and Association Analysis Results

The minor allele frequencies from the 1000 Genomes project, 1000GENOMES AF (ALL = total subjects and AMR = Admixed American subjects) were obtained from the 1000Genomes website (http://www.1000genomes.org/). Abbreviations: MAF = minor allele frequency, HWE = Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, SE = standard error.

Note: Manhattan plot across all chromosomes, using covariates age and gender. Minor allele frequency cut-off of 0.01 applied to the plot. Suggestive significance line calculated from GEC software. Green rectangles in plot highlight SNPs rs2171755 and rs58905541.

FIGURE 1 Manhattan plot for pre-pulse phenotype.

From the multivariate association analysis of the N4S response to pre-pulse stimuli phenotypic trait and alcohol dependence diagnosis phenotype, we found that the inclusion of the alcohol dependence phenotype did not significantly alter the association of the SNPs with the pre-pulse phenotype. That is, the p values were significant whether we used univariate PLINK (rs2171755 p value = 1.79E-05; rs58905541 p value = 1.11E-06), or multivariate PLINK (rs2171755 p value = 9.36E-05; rs58905541 p value = 5.77E-06). Additionally, the weights determined by multivariate PLINK demonstrated that the pre-pulse phenotype had a strong correlation to the SNPs (rs2171755 weight for pre-pulse = 0.995; rs58905541 weight for pre-pulse = 0.999), while the alcohol dependence phenotype had a very weak correlation (rs2171755 weight for alcohol dependence = 0.026; rs58905541 weight for alcohol dependence = 0.109). These results suggest that the pre-pulse phenotype is driving the significance of the association for these two SNPs.

Multiple Test Correction

Multiple test correction p value thresholds were calculated for the Affymetrix Axiom Exome1A chip using the GEC software (Li et al., Reference Li, Yeung, Cherny and Sham2012), and the thresholds generated were thereby used to determine that the variants could be characterized to possess suggestive significance. In particular, for the Affymetrix Axiom Exome1A chip at a minor allele frequency of 0.01 or greater, the threshold for a suggestive p value was calculated as 2.53E-05, significant p value as 1.27E-06, and highly significant p value as 2.53E-08.

LD Analysis

The LD was calculated across the CCKAR and CCKBR gene regions. The average D’ value across the SNP pairs in the CCKAR region was 0.77 in this data set. No other SNPs on the Affymetrix chip were found to be in high LD with the variant near the CCKAR gene. The average D’ value across the SNP pairs in the CCKBR region was 0.85 in this data set. While no SNP was found to be in complete LD with rs58905541, the SNP with highest LD was rs1462983 (Dʹ = 0.837, r2 = 0.023, LOD = 2.19, CI = 0.37–0.95). This SNP was located in OR56B4 (olfactory receptor, family 56, subfamily B, member 4) and possessed a positive beta value, suggesting it is a risk factor. However, rs1462932 was not significantly associated with the pre-pulse inhibition response phenotype in our sample when using PLINK for the analysis.

Discussion

The present study confirmed previous observations in this population of an increase in PPF of the N4S ERP component to the acoustic pre-pulse stimuli (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Phillips, Criado and Gilder2011). In the present study, we used this endophenotype in an association analysis using an Affymetrix Axiom Exome1A array to explore potential genetic factors underlying the N4S component response to pre-pulse startle stimuli in this young adult MA cohort. The results of the present study suggest that variants located near CCKAR and CCKBR (cholecystokinin A and B receptor) genes are suggestive to be associated with the N4S ERP response to pre-pulse startle stimuli in this MA cohort. The rs2171755 variant (NC_000004.12:g.26502338T>C) is located 12 kb upstream from the CCKAR gene in a MIRb class SINE repetitive element. Interestingly, a variant in a MIRb class SINE repetitive element has been reported to be associated with human cognition (Gosso et al., Reference Gosso, de Geus, Polderman, Boomsma, Posthuma and Heutink2007). The rs58905541 variant (NC_000011.10:g.6296157C>T) is located 24 kb downstream from the CCKBR gene in a DNaseI hypersensitivity cluster. Variants in regulatory regions of the genome, such as DNase I hypersensitive sites, have been found to be associated with a number of diseases (Encode Project Consortium, 2012; Maurano et al., Reference Maurano, Humbert, Rynes, Thurman, Haugen, Wang and Stamatoyannopoulos2012). Additionally, both SNPs show phylogenetic conservation in the UCSC Genome Browser alignment tracks in Figure 2. Since, it has been reported that phenotype-associated variants occur more frequently in evolutionarily constrained regions of the genome (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Hansen, Abaan, Tullius and Margulies2009), it suggests that these SNPs are likely to be real and functional. Therefore, it is feasible that the non-coding variants found in this present study could potentially play a role in the N4S ERP response to pre-pulse stimuli.

Note: This figure shows the genomic location of the SNPs rs2171755 and rs58905541.

FIGURE 2 UCSC Genome browser view of the SNP locations.

The neurotransmitter cholecystokinin (CCK) is widely present in the human body and in the central nervous system, where it modulates the dopaminergic system (Crawley & Corwin, Reference Crawley and Corwin1994). Because of its potential modulation of the dopaminergic system and associated reward pathways, CCK has been investigated in a number of studies as a candidate gene for substance dependence and other behavioral disorders. CCK, along with its two receptors, CCKAR and CCKBR, have been previously associated with anxiety and panic disorders (Maron et al., Reference Maron, Toru, Tasa, Must, Toover, Lang and Shlik2008; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Markie and Fitches2012), schizophrenia (Christoforou et al., Reference Christoforou, Le Hellard, Thomson, Morris, Tenesa, Pickard and Evans2007; Sanjuan et al., Reference Sanjuan, Toirac, Gonzalez, Leal, Molto, Najera and De Frutos2004), nicotine dependence (Takimoto et al., Reference Takimoto, Terayama, Waga, Okayama, Ikeda, Fukunishi and Iwahashi2005), and alcohol dependence (Miyasaka et al., Reference Miyasaka, Yoshida, Matsushita, Higuchi, Maruyama, Niino and Funakoshi2004; Okubo & Harada, Reference Okubo and Harada2001; Okubo et al., Reference Okubo, Harada, Higuchi and Matsushita2002). Although one study reported no association between CCK and CCKBR with alcohol dependence (Vanakoski et al., Reference Vanakoski, Virkkunen, Naukkarinen and Goldman2001), the study was performed using a Finnish population and did not examine the same significant SNPs that were determined in our study. Besides possible neurological influences of CCK on alcohol addiction, it has also been found that CCK through activation of CCKA receptors protects the gastric mucosa against ethanol-induced gastric damage in rats (Konturek et al., Reference Konturek, Brzozowski, Pytko-Polonczyk and Drozdowicz1995). In the context of alcoholism, variation in CCK activity in the gut could allow for the ingestion of greater or lesser amounts of alcohol, which in turn could influence liability towards alcoholism.

Further evidence of the role of CCK in the regulation of startle responses is supported by studies where the administration of CCK-related peptides has been found to influence the startle response in rats and humans. In particular, infusion of CCK has been found to enhance the acoustic startle response in rats (Feifel & Swerdlow, Reference Feifel and Swerdlow1997; Feifel et al., Reference Feifel, Priebe and Shilling2001; Fendt et al., Reference Fendt, Koch, Kungel and Schnitzler1995), and some CCK antagonists attenuate startle responses (Feifel et al., Reference Feifel, Reza, Wustrow and Davis1999; Josselyn et al., Reference Josselyn, Frankland, Petrisano, Bush, Yeomans and Vaccarino1995). In human subjects, CCK infusion has also been demonstrated to increase eye-blink startle as well as produce a mild increase in anxiety and heart rate as well as increases in plasma concentrations of ACTH, cortisol, prolactin, and growth hormone (Shlik et al., Reference Shlik, Zhou, Koszycki, Vaccarino and Bradwejn1999). Since, CCK peptides have been demonstrated to enhance anxiety, several studies have investigated the role of the CCK system in anxiety and panic disorders, using CCK-related peptide administration as a challenge method (Koszycki et al., Reference Koszycki, Prichard, Fiocco, Shlik, Kennedy and Bradwejn2012; Maron et al., Reference Maron, Toru, Tasa, Must, Toover, Lang and Shlik2008). These CCK challenge studies were able to explore genetic factors in panic disorders by performing candidate gene analyses to find alleles that may be associated with greater sensitivity to the CCK peptides. One study found an association of the tryptophan hydroxylase gene isomer 2 (TPH2) with subjects experiencing panic attacks after CCK infusion (Maron et al., Reference Maron, Toru, Tasa, Must, Toover, Lang and Shlik2008). Another study found an association to the CCKBR gene in subjects with greater pre-challenge anxiety (Koszycki et al., Reference Koszycki, Prichard, Fiocco, Shlik, Kennedy and Bradwejn2012). Additional studies have utilized CCK-peptide infusion to determine its effects on EEG and ERP measures in healthy human subjects (Knott et al., Reference Knott, Mahoney, Gunnarsson, Bradwejn and Shlik2002, Reference Knott, Mahoney, Bradwejn, Shlik and Gunnarsson2003). CCK-4 infusion was found to delay the latencies of N100 and P200 components of the ERP that was elicited during an auditory oddball task (Knott et al., Reference Knott, Mahoney, Gunnarsson, Bradwejn and Shlik2002). During EEG recording of resting subjects, CCK-4 infusion was also found to increase asymmetry and reduce coherence of the slow-wave activity at mid-temporal recording sites (Knott et al., Reference Knott, Mahoney, Bradwejn, Shlik and Gunnarsson2003). Taken together, these studies suggest a plausible role for CCK variants in the regulation of brain activity and behavior.

Impairments in frontal lobe function and associated behaviors such as executive functioning have been suggested to underlie anxiety disorders (Castaneda et al., Reference Castaneda, Tuulio-Henriksson, Marttunen, Suvisaari and Lonnqvist2008), as well as a number of other behavioral disorders such as schizophrenia (Bagney et al., Reference Bagney, Rodriguez-Jimenez, Martinez-Gras, Sanchez-Morla, Santos, Jimenez-Arriero and Parg2013; Chan et al., Reference Chan, Chan, Hui, Wong, Chang, Lee and Chen2014; Eisenberg & Berman, Reference Eisenberg and Berman2010; Holmen et al., Reference Holmen, Juuhl-Langseth, Thormodsen, Ueland, Agartz, Sundet and Melle2012; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Johnstone, Miller, Macmillan and Crow2010), bipolar disorders (Erol et al., Reference Erol, Kosger, Putgul and Ersoy2014; Kulkarni et al., Reference Kulkarni, Jain, Reddy, Kumar and Kandavel2010; Yen et al., Reference Yen, Cheng, Huang, Ko, Yen, Chang and Chen2009; Zimmerman et al., Reference Zimmerman, DelBello, Getz, Shear and Strakowski2006), and substance use disorders (Fernandez-Serrano et al., Reference Fernandez-Serrano, Perez-Garcia, Schmidt Rio-Valle and Verdejo-Garcia2010; Gierski et al., Reference Gierski, Hubsch, Stefaniak, Benzerouk, Cuervo-Lombard, Bera-Potelle and Limosin2013; Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Vollstadt-Klein, von der Goltz, Flor, Mann and Kiefer2009; Maurage et al., Reference Maurage, de Timary, Billieux, Collignon and Heeren2014; van der Plas et al., Reference van der Plas, Crone, van den Wildenberg, Tranel and Bechara2009; Zorko et al., Reference Zorko, Marusic, Cebasek-Travnik and Bucik2004). Interestingly, schizophrenia and bipolar disorders also demonstrate significant clinical comorbidity with substance use disorders (Regier et al., Reference Regier, Farmer, Rae, Locke, Keith, Judd and Goodwin1990) and may also share genetic susceptibility factors (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014; Schuckit et al., Reference Schuckit, Kelsoe, Braff and Wilhelmsen2003). There is also some data to suggest that they may share common endophenotypes such as deficits in some aspects of responses to startle (Kohl et al., Reference Kohl, Heekeren, Klosterkotter and Kuhn2013), although this hypothesis requires further testing and confirmation.

In summary, in the present study, associations between the N4S ERP responses to acoustic pre-pulse startle stimuli were determined using association analyses. Our results suggest that variants located in regulatory non-coding regions near the cholecystokinin A and B receptors may play a role in the pre-pulse/startle response. However, the results of this study should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. First, the findings may not generalize to the general American population of mixed heritage or all MAs, or all Hispanic young adult Americans. Over half of the participants in the present were women and thus findings many not generalize to previous studies that have focused on samples of entirely male participants. Second, the study was limited to young adults between the ages of 18 and 30 years, and the sample size may limit the interpretation of the results. Despite these limitations, this report represents an important step in an ongoing investigation to determine risk and protective factors associated with development of substance use disorders in this select MA population.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank and acknowledge the following people for their role in (1) the genotyping effort: Chris Bizon, Scott Chasse, Piotr Mieczkowski, Ewa Patrycja Malc, Joshua Sailsbery, and Phil Owens; and (2) for recruiting participants and collecting the clinical data: David Gilder, Susan Lopez, and Linda Corey. Funding for this study was provided by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH); from the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse (NIAAA) and the National Center on Minority Health and Health Disparities (NCMHD) 5R37 AA010201 and P60 AA006420 (CLE) and the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) 5 R01 DA030976 (CLE, IRG, KCW, & NJS). NIAAA, NCMHD and NIDA had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication. NJS and his lab are supported in part by NIH grants 5 UL1 RR025774, R21 AI085374, 5 U01 DA024417, 5 R01 HL089655, 5 R01 AG035020, 1 R01 MH093500, 2 U19 AI063603, 2 U19 AG023122, 5 P01 AG027734, 1 R21 DA033813.

Conflict of Interest

NJS is a founder and stock holder in Cypher Genomics and paid consultant for the following companies: Human Longevity, Inc., MD Revolution, and Click Therapeutics. All of the authors declare that they have no conflicts of interests.

References

Affymetrix. (2011). Affymetrix: Best practice supplement to axiom genotyping solution data analysis user guide rev. 1 (vol. 1, pp. 133). Santa Clara, CA: Author.Google Scholar
Bagney, A., Rodriguez-Jimenez, R., Martinez-Gras, I., Sanchez-Morla, E. M., Santos, J. L., Jimenez-Arriero, M. A., . . . Parg, . (2013). Negative symptoms and executive function in schizophrenia: Does their relationship change with illness duration? Psychopathology, 46, 241248.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. C., Fry, B., Maller, J., & Daly, M. J. (2005). Haploview: Analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics, 21, 263265.Google Scholar
Bauer, L. O. (1997). Frontal P300 decrements, childhood conduct disorder, family history, and the prediction of relapse among abstinent cocaine abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 44, 110.Google Scholar
Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., Light, G. A., Sprock, J., Perry, W., Cadenhead, K. S., . . . Swerdlow, N. R. (2001a). Impact of prepulse characteristics on the detection of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 49, 171178.Google Scholar
Braff, D. L., Geyer, M. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2001b). Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: Normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology, 156, 234258.Google Scholar
Bucholz, K. K., Cadoret, R., Cloninger, C. R., Dinwiddie, S. H., Hesselbrock, V. M., Nurnberger, J. I. Jr., . . . Schuckit, M. A. (1994). A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 149158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, L. E., Hughes, M., Budd, T. W., Cooper, G., Fulham, W. R., Karayanidis, F., . . . Schall, U. (2007). Primary and secondary neural networks of auditory prepulse inhibition: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of sensorimotor gating of the human acoustic startle response. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 23272333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castaneda, A. E., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Marttunen, M., Suvisaari, J., & Lonnqvist, J. (2008). A review on cognitive impairments in depressive and anxiety disorders with a focus on young adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 106, 127.Google Scholar
Chan, S. K. W., Chan, K. K. S., Hui, C. L., Wong, G. H. Y., Chang, W. C., Lee, E. H. M., . . . Chen, E. Y. H. (2014). Correlates of insight with symptomatology and executive function in patients with first-episode schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: A longitudinal perspective. Psychiatry Research, 216, 177184.Google Scholar
Christoforou, A., Le Hellard, S., Thomson, P. A., Morris, S. W., Tenesa, A., Pickard, B. S., . . . Evans, K. L. (2007). Association analysis of the chromosome 4p15-p16 candidate region for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 10111025.Google Scholar
Crawley, J. N., & Corwin, R. L. (1994). Biological actions of cholecystokinin. Peptides, 15, 731755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Criado, J. R., & Ehlers, C. L. (2007). Electrophysiological responses to affective stimuli in Mexican Americans: Relationship to alcohol dependence and personality traits. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 88, 148157.Google Scholar
Criado, J. R., Gizer, I. R., Edenberg, H. J., & Ehlers, C. L. (2014). CHRNA5 and CHRNA3 variants and level of neuroticism in young adult Mexican American men and women. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 17, 8088.Google Scholar
De Koning, M. B., Bloemen, O. J., Van Duin, E. D., Booij, J., Abel, K. M., De Haan, L., . . . Van Amelsvoort, T. A. (2014). Pre-pulse inhibition and striatal dopamine in subjects at an ultra-high risk for psychosis. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 28, 553560.Google Scholar
De Pascalis, V., Cozzuto, G., & Russo, E. (2013). Effects of personality trait emotionality on acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition including N100 and P200 event-related potential. Clinical Neurophysiology, 124, 292305.Google Scholar
Ehlers, C. L., Gilder, D. A., Criado, J. R., & Caetano, R. (2009). Acculturation stress, anxiety disorders, and alcohol dependence in a select population of young adult Mexican Americans. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 3, 227233.Google Scholar
Ehlers, C. L., Gilder, D. A., Criado, J. R., & Caetano, R. (2010). Sleep quality and alcohol-use disorders in a select population of young-adult Mexican Americans. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71, 879884.Google Scholar
Ehlers, C. L., Liang, T., & Gizer, I. R. (2012). ADH and ALDH polymorphisms and alcohol dependence in Mexican and Native Americans. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 38, 389394.Google Scholar
Ehlers, C. L., & Phillips, E. (2007). Association of EEG alpha variants and alpha power with alcohol dependence in Mexican American young adults. Alcohol, 41, 1320.Google Scholar
Ehlers, C. L., Phillips, E., Criado, J. R., & Gilder, D. A. (2011). N4 component responses to pre-pulse startle stimuli in young adults: Relationship to alcohol dependence. Psychiatry Research, 188, 237244.Google Scholar
Ehlers, C. L., Stouffer, G. M., & Gilder, D. A. (2014). Associations between a history of binge drinking during adolescence and self-reported responses to alcohol in young adult native and Mexican Americans. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 38, 20392047.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, D. P., & Berman, K. F. (2010). Executive function, neural circuitry, and genetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 258277.Google Scholar
Encode Project Consortium. (2012). An integrated encyclopedia of DNA elements in the human genome. Nature, 489, 5774.Google Scholar
Erol, A., Kosger, F., Putgul, G., & Ersoy, B. (2014). Ventral prefrontal executive function impairment as a potential endophenotypic marker for bipolar disorder. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 68, 1823.Google Scholar
Feifel, D., Priebe, K., & Shilling, P. D. (2001). Startle and sensorimotor gating in rats lacking CCK-A receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology, 24, 663670.Google Scholar
Feifel, D., Reza, T. L., Wustrow, D. J., & Davis, M. D. (1999). Novel antipsychotic-like effects on prepulse inhibition of startle produced by a neurotensin agonist. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 288, 710713.Google ScholarPubMed
Feifel, D., & Swerdlow, N. R. (1997). The modulation of sensorimotor gating deficits by mesolimbic cholecystokinin. Neuroscience Letters, 229, 58.Google Scholar
Fendt, M., Koch, M., Kungel, M., & Schnitzler, H. U. (1995). Cholecystokinin enhances the acoustic startle response in rats. Neuroreport, 6, 20812084.Google Scholar
Fendt, M., Li, L., & Yeomans, J. S. (2001). Brain stem circuits mediating prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Psychopharmacology, 156, 216224.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Serrano, M. J., Perez-Garcia, M., Schmidt Rio-Valle, J., & Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2010). Neuropsychological consequences of alcohol and drug abuse on different components of executive functions. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 24, 13171332.Google Scholar
Ferreira, M. A., & Purcell, S. M. (2009). A multivariate test of association. Bioinformatics, 25, 132133.Google Scholar
Filion, D. L., Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1998). The psychological significance of human startle eyeblink modification: A review. Biological Psychology, 47, 143.Google Scholar
Filion, D. L., Kelly, K. A., & Hazlett, E. A. (1999). Behavioral analogies of short lead interval startle inhibition. In Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Bohmelt, A. H. (Eds.), Startle modification: Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science, and clinical science (pp. 269283). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ford, J. M., Roth, W. T., Menon, V., & Pfefferbaum, A. (1999). Failures of automatic and strategic processing in schizophrenia: Comparisons of event-related brain potential and startle blink modification. Schizophrenia Research, 37, 149163.Google Scholar
Ford, J. M., White, P., Lim, K. O., & Pfefferbaum, A. (1994). Schizophrenics have fewer and smaller P300s: A single-trial analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 35, 96103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geyer, M. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2001). Measurement of startle response, prepulse inhibition, and habituation. Current Protocols in Neuroscience, 3, 8.7.1–8.7.15.Google Scholar
Gierski, F., Hubsch, B., Stefaniak, N., Benzerouk, F., Cuervo-Lombard, C., Bera-Potelle, C., . . . Limosin, F. (2013). Executive functions in adult offspring of alcohol-dependent probands: Toward a cognitive endophenotype? Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 37 (Suppl. 1), E356E363.Google Scholar
Gosso, F. M., de Geus, E. J., Polderman, T. J., Boomsma, D. I., Posthuma, D., & Heutink, P. (2007). Exploring the functional role of the CHRM2 gene in human cognition: Results from a dense genotyping and brain expression study. BMC Medical Genetics, 8, 66.Google Scholar
Grillon, C., Morgan, C. A., Southwick, S. M., Davis, M., & Charney, D. S. (1996). Baseline startle amplitude and prepulse inhibition in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research, 64, 169178.Google Scholar
Hesselbrock, M., Easton, C., Bucholz, K. K., Schuckit, M., & Hesselbrock, V. (1999). A validity study of the SSAGA — A comparison with the SCAN. Addiction, 94, 13611370.Google Scholar
Holmen, A., Juuhl-Langseth, M., Thormodsen, R., Ueland, T., Agartz, I., Sundet, K., . . . Melle, I. (2012). Executive function in early- and adult onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 142, 177182.Google Scholar
Hsieh, M. H., Swerdlow, N. R., & Braff, D. L. (2006). Effects of background and prepulse characteristics on prepulse inhibition and facilitation: Implications for neuropsychiatric research. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 555559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josselyn, S. A., Frankland, P. W., Petrisano, S., Bush, D. E., Yeomans, J. S., & Vaccarino, F. J. (1995). The CCKB antagonist, L-365260, attenuates fear-potentiated startle. Peptides, 16, 13131315.Google Scholar
Kent, W. J., Sugnet, C. W., Furey, T. S., Roskin, K. M., Pringle, T. H., Zahler, A. M., . . . Haussler, D. (2002). The human genome browser at UCSC. Genome Research, 12, 9961006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knott, V. J., Mahoney, C., Gunnarsson, T., Bradwejn, J., & Shlik, J. (2002). Acute cholecystokinin effects on event-related potentials in healthy volunteers. Human Psychopharmacology, 17, 285291.Google Scholar
Knott, V., Mahoney, C., Bradwejn, J., Shlik, J., & Gunnarsson, T. (2003). Effects of acute cholecystokinin infusion on hemispheric EEG asymmetry and coherence in healthy volunteers. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 27, 179184.Google Scholar
Kohl, S., Heekeren, K., Klosterkotter, J., & Kuhn, J. (2013). Prepulse inhibition in psychiatric disorders — Apart from schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 445452.Google Scholar
Konturek, S. J., Brzozowski, T., Pytko-Polonczyk, J., & Drozdowicz, D. (1995). Exogenous and endogenous cholecystokinin protects gastric mucosa against the damage caused by ethanol in rats. European Journal of Pharmacology, 273, 5762.Google Scholar
Koszycki, D., Prichard, Z., Fiocco, A. J., Shlik, J., Kennedy, J. L., & Bradwejn, J. (2012). CCK-B receptor gene and response to cholecystokinin-tetrapeptide in healthy volunteers. Peptides, 35, 913.Google Scholar
Kulkarni, S., Jain, S., Reddy, Y. C. J., Kumar, K. J., & Kandavel, T. (2010). Impairment of verbal learning and memory and executive function in unaffected siblings of probands with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 12, 647656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumari, V., Antonova, E., Zachariah, E., Galea, A., Aasen, I., Ettinger, U., . . . Sharma, T. (2005). Structural brain correlates of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in healthy humans. Neuroimage, 26, 10521058.Google Scholar
Li, M. X., Yeung, J. M., Cherny, S. S., & Sham, P. C. (2012). Evaluating the effective numbers of independent tests and significant p-value thresholds in commercial genotyping arrays and public imputation reference datasets. Human Genetics, 131, 747756.Google Scholar
Loeber, S., Vollstadt-Klein, S., von der Goltz, C., Flor, H., Mann, K., & Kiefer, F. (2009). Attentional bias in alcohol-dependent patients: The role of chronicity and executive functioning. Addiction Biology, 14, 194203.Google Scholar
Ludewig, K., Ludewig, S., Seitz, A., Obrist, M., Geyer, M. A., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2003). The acoustic startle reflex and its modulation: Effects of age and gender in humans. Biological Psychology, 63, 311323.Google Scholar
Marin, M., Ponce, G., Martinez-Gras, I., Koeneke, A., Curivil, P., Jimenez-Arriero, M. A., . . . Rubio, G. (2012). Impairments of prepulse inhibition of the startle response in abstinent alcoholic male patients. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 47, 545551.Google Scholar
Maron, E., Toru, I., Tasa, G., Must, A., Toover, E., Lang, A., . . . Shlik, J. (2008). Association testing of panic disorder candidate genes using CCK-4 challenge in healthy volunteers. Neuroscience Letters, 446, 8892.Google Scholar
Maurage, P., de Timary, P., Billieux, J., Collignon, M., & Heeren, A. (2014). Attentional alterations in alcohol dependence are underpinned by specific executive control deficits. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 38, 21052112.Google Scholar
Maurano, M. T., Humbert, R., Rynes, E., Thurman, R. E., Haugen, E., Wang, H., . . . Stamatoyannopoulos, J. A. (2012). Systematic localization of common disease-associated variation in regulatory DNA. Science, 337, 11901195.Google Scholar
Miyasaka, K., Yoshida, Y., Matsushita, S., Higuchi, S., Maruyama, K., Niino, N., . . . Funakoshi, A. (2004). Association of cholecystokinin-A receptor gene polymorphism with alcohol dependence in a Japanese population. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 39, 2528.Google Scholar
Neuner, I., Stocker, T., Kellermann, T., Ermer, V., Wegener, H. P., Eickhoff, S. B., . . . Shah, N. J. (2010). Electrophysiology meets fMRI: Neural correlates of the startle reflex assessed by simultaneous EMG-fMRI data acquisition. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 16751685.Google Scholar
Okubo, T., & Harada, S. (2001). Polymorphisms of the CCK, CCKAR and CCKBR genes: An association with alcoholism study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62, 413421.Google Scholar
Okubo, T., Harada, S., Higuchi, S., & Matsushita, S. (2002). Investigation of quantitative trait loci in the CCKAR gene with susceptibility to alcoholism. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 26, 2S5S.Google Scholar
Owens, D. C., Johnstone, E. C., Miller, P., Macmillan, J. F., & Crow, T. J. (2010). Duration of untreated illness and outcome in schizophrenia: Test of predictions in relation to relapse risk. British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 296301.Google Scholar
Parker, S. C., Hansen, L., Abaan, H. O., Tullius, T. D., & Margulies, E. H. (2009). Local DNA topography correlates with functional noncoding regions of the human genome. Science, 324, 389392.Google Scholar
Purcell, S., Neale, B., Todd-Brown, K., Thomas, L., Ferreira, M. A., Bender, D., . . . Sham, P. C. (2007). PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, 559575.Google Scholar
Putnam, L. E., & Roth, W. T. (1990). Effects of stimulus repetition, duration, and rise time on startle blink and automatically elicited P300. Psychophysiology, 27, 275297.Google Scholar
Regier, D. A., Farmer, M. E., Rae, D. S., Locke, B. Z., Keith, S. J., Judd, L. L., . . . Goodwin, F. K. (1990). Comorbidity of mental disorders with alcohol and other drug abuse. Results from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) Study. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 25112518.Google Scholar
Sanjuan, J., Toirac, I., Gonzalez, J. C., Leal, C., Molto, M. D., Najera, C., . . . De Frutos, R. (2004). A possible association between the CCK-AR gene and persistent auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. European Psychiatry, 19, 349353.Google Scholar
Schall, U., Catts, S. V., Karayanidis, F., & Ward, P. B. (1999). Auditory event-related potential indices of fronto-temporal information processing in schizophrenia syndromes: Valid outcome prediction of clozapine therapy in a three-year follow-up. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 2, 8393.Google Scholar
Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature, 511, 421427.Google Scholar
Schuckit, M. A., Kelsoe, J. R., Braff, D. L., & Wilhelmsen, K. C. (2003). Some possible genetic parallels across alcoholism, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64, 157159.Google Scholar
Shlik, J., Zhou, Y., Koszycki, D., Vaccarino, F. J., & Bradwejn, J. (1999). Effects of CCK-4 infusion on the acoustic eye-blink startle and psychophysiological measures in healthy volunteers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 13, 385390.Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Braff, D. L., Taaid, N., & Geyer, M. A. (1994). Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 139154.Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Caine, S. B., Braff, D. L., & Geyer, M. A. (1992). The neural substrates of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: A review of recent findings and their implications. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 6, 176190.Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., & Geyer, M. A. (1999). Neurophysiology and neuropharmacology of short lead interval startle modification. In Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M. & Bohmelt, A. H. (Eds.), Startle modification: Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science, and clinical science (pp. 114133). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Swerdlow, N. R., Light, G. A., Sprock, J., Calkins, M. E., Green, M. F., Greenwood, T. A., . . . Braff, D. L. (2014). Deficient prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia detected by the multi-site COGS. Schizophrenia Research, 152, 503512.Google Scholar
Takimoto, T., Terayama, H., Waga, C., Okayama, T., Ikeda, K., Fukunishi, I., . . . Iwahashi, K. (2005). Cholecystokinin (CCK) and the CCKA receptor gene polymorphism, and smoking behavior. Psychiatry Research, 133, 123128.Google Scholar
van der Plas, E. A., Crone, E. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Tranel, D., & Bechara, A. (2009). Executive control deficits in substance-dependent individuals: A comparison of alcohol, cocaine, and methamphetamine and of men and women. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 706719.Google Scholar
Vanakoski, J., Virkkunen, M., Naukkarinen, H., & Goldman, D. (2001). No association of CCK and CCK(B) receptor polymorphisms with alcohol dependence. Psychiatry Research, 102, 17.Google Scholar
Wilson, J., Markie, D., & Fitches, A. (2012). Cholecystokinin system genes: Associations with panic and other psychiatric disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 136, 902908.Google Scholar
Yen, C. F., Cheng, C. P., Huang, C. F., Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chang, Y. P., . . . Chen, C. S. (2009). Relationship between psychosocial adjustment and executive function in patients with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in remission: The mediating and moderating effects of insight. Bipolar Disorders, 11, 190197.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, M. E., DelBello, M. P., Getz, G. E., Shear, P. K., & Strakowski, S. M. (2006). Anterior cingulate subregion volumes and executive function in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 8, 522522.Google Scholar
Zorko, M., Marusic, A., Cebasek-Travnik, Z., & Bucik, V. (2004). The frontal lobe hypothesis: Impairment of executive cognitive functions in chronic alcohol in-patients. Psychiatria Danubina, 16, 2128.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Demographics for Mexican American Study Participants

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Significant Variants for Pre-pulse Phenotype

Figure 2

TABLE 3 Allele Frequencies, Genotype Frequencies, and Association Analysis Results

Figure 3

FIGURE 1 Manhattan plot for pre-pulse phenotype.

Note: Manhattan plot across all chromosomes, using covariates age and gender. Minor allele frequency cut-off of 0.01 applied to the plot. Suggestive significance line calculated from GEC software. Green rectangles in plot highlight SNPs rs2171755 and rs58905541.
Figure 4

FIGURE 2 UCSC Genome browser view of the SNP locations.

Note: This figure shows the genomic location of the SNPs rs2171755 and rs58905541.