Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T21:14:44.576Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intergenerational Transmission of BMI and Educational Outcomes in Children and Adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 July 2023

Hekmat Alrouh*
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, Netherlands
Elsje van Bergen
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, Netherlands Research Institute LEARN!, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
Conor Dolan
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, Netherlands
Dorret I. Boomsma
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, Netherlands Amsterdam Research and Development Research Institute, Amsterdam, Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Hekmat Alrouh; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Individual differences in educational attainment (EA) and physical health, as indexed by body mass index (BMI), are correlated within individuals and across generations. The aim of our study was to assess the transmission of these traits from parents to their offspring in childhood and adolescence. We analyzed BMI and EA in 13,916 families from the Netherlands. Data were available for 27,577 parents (mean age 33) and 26,855 of their offspring at 4 and 12 years of age. We employed structural equation modeling to simultaneously estimate the phenotypic transmission of BMI and EA from parents to offspring, the spousal correlations, and the residual child BMI-EA associations after accounting for intergenerational transmission and testing for gender differences therein. We found a significant intergenerational transmission of BMI to BMI in childhood (age 4; standardized regression coefficient β = .10) and adolescence (age 12; β = .20), and of EA to academic achievement in adolescence (β = .19). Cross-trait parent-to-offspring transmission was weak. All transmission effects were independent of parent or offspring gender. We observed within-person EA-BMI correlations that were negative in parents (∼−.09), positive in children (∼.05) and negative in adolescents (∼−.06). Residual EA-BMI were positive in children (∼.05) and insignificant in adolescents. Spousal correlations were .46 for EA, .21 for BMI, and ∼−.09 cross-trait. After accounting for spousal correlations, the intergenerational transmission for BMI and EA is mainly predictive within, but not across, traits. The within-person correlation between BMI and EA can change in direction between childhood and adulthood.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Society for Twin Studies

Childhood obesity is a growing problem worldwide, with adverse effects on virtually all organ systems, and serious short- and long-term sequelae (Daniels, Reference Daniels2009). The prevalence of overweight (including obesity) in children and adolescents — defined as body mass index (BMI) +1 standard deviations above the median — in the Netherlands is estimated to be slightly lower than the average for European countries. In the Netherlands, the World Health Organization estimates the prevalence at 26.9% in children aged 5−9 years and 23.7% in children aged 10−19 years, whereas the averages for European countries are 28.9% and 24.3% respectively. These rates vary across countries and regions, where, for example, in the United States the prevalence is 43% in 5- to 9-year-olds and 41.2% in 10- to 19-year-olds, with corresponding rates of 10.3% and 8.1% in South-East Asia. Globally, overweight prevalence is 20.9% in 5- to 9-year-olds and 17.3% in 10- to 19-year-olds (World Health Organization, 2017).

Within countries, the most important predictor of a child’s BMI is the BMI of their parents (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Derraik, Taylor and Cutfield2018; Vos & Welsh, Reference Vos and Welsh2010). In addition, low socioeconomic status (SES) (Newton et al., Reference Newton, Braithwaite and Akinyemiju2017) and lower educational attainment (EA) (Benson et al., Reference Benson, von Hippel and Lynch2018) are strong predictors, with overweight and obesity tending to be more prevalent in lower SES and EA. For example, in the Netherlands, the relative risk of overweight and obesity is twice as high for children with low maternal education and low household income (White et al., Reference White, Awad, Gauvin, Spencer, McGrath, Clifford, Nikiema, Yang-Huang, Goldhaber-Fiebert, Markham, Mensah, van Grieken, Raat, Jaddoe, Ludvigsson and Faresjö2022).

While the association of parental EA and offspring BMI is well established (Benson et al., Reference Benson, von Hippel and Lynch2018; Lamerz et al., Reference Lamerz, Kuepper-Nybelen, Wehle, Bruning, Trost-Brinkhues, Brenner, Hebebrand and Herpertz-Dahlmann2005; Matthiessen et al., Reference Matthiessen, Stockmarr, Biltoft-Jensen, Fagt, Zhang and Groth2014; Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Jelenkovic, Latvala, Yokoyama, Sund, Sugawara, Tanaka, Matsumoto, Aaltonen, Piirtola, Freitas, Maia, Öncel, Aliev, Ji, Ning, Pang, Rebato, Saudino and Kaprio2019), the etiology of the association is not. One the one hand, higher EA leads to better occupation and higher income and SES, and higher income enables individuals to afford food with higher nutritional value and lower caloric density (Darmon & Drewnowski, Reference Darmon and Drewnowski2015; Drewnowski, Reference Drewnowski2010). On the other hand, Hanson and Conner (2014) showed that parents provide their offspring with food of high nutritional value regardless of income level. In addition to the possible role of nutrition, higher income is associated with many other factors; for example, it is associated with breastfeeding (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2009) and may facilitate physical activity in children, insofar as such activity involves cost (e.g., club memberships, sportswear and material; Telford et al., Reference Telford, Telford, Cochrane, Cunningham, Olive and Davey2016).

Many studies have established significant associations between parental SES and EA and offspring BMI, without controlling for parental BMI (Kleiser et al., Reference Kleiser, Schaffrath Rosario, Mensink, Prinz-Langenohl and Kurth2009; Murasko, Reference Murasko2009; Nogueira et al., Reference Nogueira, Gama, Mourão, Marques, Ferrão and Padez2013; O’Dea & Dibley, Reference O’Dea and Dibley2010; Springer et al., Reference Springer, Li, Ranjit, Delk, Mehta and Kelder2015; Wang, Reference Wang2001; Wang & Zhang, Reference Wang and Zhang2006). In a study of adult twins and their parents, we found a low negative, but statistically significant, correlation of −.06 between parental EA and offspring BMI. However, this correlation was not statistically significant, once we controlled for parental BMI (Alrouh et al., Reference Alrouh, van Bergen, de Zeeuw, Dolan and Boomsma2022).

SES is not the only possible pathway linking EA and obesity, as research has shown a link between obesity and cognitive performance, controlling for parental social class (Sørensen et al., Reference Sørensen, Sonne-Holm and Christensen1983). Behavioral factors, such as self-control and delayed gratification, are associated with both EA (Golsteyn et al., Reference Golsteyn, Grönqvist and Lindahl2014; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Krabbendam, Dekker, Boschloo, de Groot and Jolles2012; Mischel et al., Reference Mischel, Shoda and Rodriguez1989) and with obesity (Caleza et al., Reference Caleza, Yañez-Vico, Mendoza and Iglesias-Linares2016; Stoklosa et al., Reference Stoklosa, Shuval, Drope, Tchernis, Pachucki, Yaroch and Harding2018). Decreased cognitive function is linked to impaired metabolic pathways, such as insulin signaling (Craft & Stennis Watson, Reference Craft and Stennis Watson2004) and leptin regulation (Harvey, Reference Harvey2007), which are associated with obesity. Finally, obesity, as reflected by BMI, has significant genetic overlap with brain and cognitive measures (Vainik et al., Reference Vainik, Baker, Dadar, Zeighami, Michaud, Zhang, García Alanis, Misic, Collins and Dagher2018) and with EA (Li et al., Reference Li, Cai, Wang and Guo2021).

Parental BMI is the strongest predictor of offspring BMI. In a recent family-based study of Dutch adult twins, we found a parent-offspring BMI correlation of .20 (Alrouh et al., Reference Alrouh, van Bergen, de Zeeuw, Dolan and Boomsma2022). This finding is in line with meta-analytic results, which showed a mean parent-offspring correlation of .19 for BMI (Maes et al., Reference Maes, Neale and Eaves1997). The association is also manifest in terms of odds ratio: children with a single obese parent have an odds ratio of 3.49 for childhood obesity (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Min, Khuri and Li2017).

Prior studies generally examined the effects of parental EA and SES on offspring BMI without accounting for parental BMI, or the correlation between parental EA or SES and parental BMI. Therefore, it is possible that the finding that children who grow up in low EA households are more susceptible to obesity may be confounded by parental BMI. Indeed, we found this to be the case, when examining BMI of adult twins in the Dutch population (Alrouh et al., Reference Alrouh, van Bergen, de Zeeuw, Dolan and Boomsma2022). In the current study, we return to this possibility by now examining these pathways to offspring BMI at ages 4 and 12 years in a sample of young twins and their parents from the Dutch population and, at age 12, also include academic achievement in children. In short, we studied the within-trait and between-trait transmission of BMI and EA across generations, while controlling for the spousal correlations among BMI and EA.

Methods

Participants

Participants were registered with the Netherlands Twin Register (NTR). Since 1986, the NTR has approached parents of newborn twins born in the Netherlands in collaboration with a commercial ‘birth felicitation’ service. Additionally, recruitment is done with the support of the Dutch Society of Parents of Multiples (NVOM; https://www.nvom.nl). After obtaining informed consent, the family is registered with the NTR, and the parents receive a first survey on pre- and perinatal variables relating to their twins, as well as variables concerning the parent, including their height and weight. Subsequently, the family receives periodical surveys on a broad set of variables, including health variables and when the twins’ developmental milestones were achieved. The NTR has registered about 52% of all Dutch twin-pairs born between 1987 and 2017 (Ligthart et al., Reference Ligthart, Beijsterveldt, Kevenaar, Zeeuw, Bergen, Bruins, Pool, Helmer, Dongen, Hottenga, Ent, Dolan, Davies, Ehli, Bartels, Willemsen, Geus and Boomsma2019). For the present study, we selected twins and their parents who had completed surveys when their twins were age 5 and 12 years (referred to as survey 5 and survey 12). We excluded families in which zygosity of twins could not be determined (67 families), resulting in a sample of 54,432 participants. The sample comprises a total of 13,916 families, which includes 26,855 twins (13,347 males and 13,508 females) and 27,577 parents (13,739 fathers and 13,838 mothers), aged between 18 and 65 years at the time of birth of twins. The data were checked for outliers and inconsistent values (i.e., values more than 4 standard deviations above or below the mean, unexplained loss of height or weight in children during growth period, and large disagreement between mother and father reports). We excluded 26 BMI scores due to improbable values for height and weight. All measures of height and weight were obtained in centimeters (cm) and kilograms (kg).

Measures

Parental BMI was based on self-reported height and weight from the first survey sent after registration with the NTR, when the twins were 9 months old on average, and was adjusted for parents’ age. The measure of the twins BMI was based on height and weight data as reported by the parent (mothers: 55%, fathers: 4%, both: 41%). When both reports were available, we randomly selected one of the parent reports. Survey 5 included questions about the twins’ height and weight after the third birthday. We selected the data closest to the 4th birthday of the twins (Estourgie-van Burk et al., Reference Estourgie-van Burk, Bartels, Boomsma and Delemarre-van de Waal2010). Survey 12, which was sent to parents when the twins were about 12 years, again included parental reports of the twins’ height and weight. Parental EA was self-reported and categorized into four levels (primary, lower secondary, intermediate/higher secondary, and higher vocational/university). Child academic achievement was based on a standardized test for educational achievement, the CITO test (Citogroep, 2002), which is administered in the final grade (when children are 11 or 12 years old) of elementary school in February-March. Information on CITO scores was obtained from parents, teachers and/or self-report by twins at later ages (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, Rietveld, Van Baal and Boomsma2002).

Statistical Analyses

We applied structural equation modeling (SEM) to study the phenotypic transmission of BMI and EA from parents to BMI and EA of their twins at age 4 and 12 years. All measures (parental and child BMI and educational measures were analyzed as continuous variables. Our model includes the transmission of BMI and EA from parents to the twins, while including the correlation of BMI and EA within and between parents. The transmission includes the prediction of offspring BMI and EA from parental BMI and EA within (EA to EA, BMI to BMI) and across traits (e.g., BMI to EA, EA to BMI). We tested whether estimates of the parameters depended on the sex of the parent and the offspring. Frequency tables, descriptive statistics, and bivariate correlations were obtained using IBM SPSS (version 26). We carried out the structural equation modeling in the Lavaan package (version 0.6-6) in R (version 3.6.1). For all statistical tests, we used the Holm-Bonferroni correction method to control the family-wise error rate in multiple hypothesis testing (Holm, Reference Holm1979). The Holm-Bonferroni correction is a post-hoc statistical procedure used to adjust p values in order to reduce the probability of type I errors when multiple statistical tests are performed simultaneously, and has a lower increase of type II error risk than the classical Bonferroni method (Aickin & Gensler, Reference Aickin and Gensler1996). It works as follows:

  1. 1. All p values are sorted from smallest to largest. Let K be the number of the p values in a family of tests.

  2. 2. If the first p value is greater than or equal to α/K, the procedure is stopped and no p values are significant. Otherwise, the first p value is declared significant, and the procedure continues.

  3. 3. The second p value is compared to α/(K-1). If the second p value is greater than or equal to α/(K-1), the procedure is stopped and no further p values are significant. Otherwise, the second value is declared significant, and we go on until the i-th ordered p value is such that: p (i) ≥ α /(K-i + 1).

Model Specifications

We initially specified separate transmission parameters from mothers and fathers to sons and daughters, to account for sex dependence of these parameters. That is, we estimated four sets (mother-daughter, mother-son, father-daughter, and father-son) of six coefficients (parent EA to offspring EA, parent BMI to offspring BMI at ages 4 and 12 (denoted BMI4 and BMI12), and all cross-transmission coefficients). The model included the following parameters:

  • 24 parent-offspring transmission parameters: 2 parental traits (EA & BMI) x 3 offspring traits (EA, BMI4 and BMI12) x 2 parents x 2 offspring genders

  • 4 spousal correlations: 2 measures (EA and BMI) x 2 types of correlation (within- and cross-trait)

  • 2 within person EA/BMI correlations in the parental generation: fathers and mothers

  • 6 residual within-person correlations in the offspring generation: 3 trait combinations (EA/BMI4, EA/BMI12, BMI4/BMI12) x 2 genders (sons and daughters)

  • 4 variances in the parental generation: 2 traits (EA, BMI) x 2 genders (fathers and mothers)

  • 6 residual variances in the offspring generation: 3 traits x 2 genders

  • 4 intercepts in the parental generation: 2 traits x 2 genders

  • 6 intercepts in the offspring generation: 3 traits x 2 genders

  • 6 residual twin correlations: 3 traits (EA, BMI4, BMI12) x 2 zygosity groups (MZ/DZ)

  • 6 cross trait residual twin correlations.

Thus, in the full model, as shown in Figure 1, we estimated transmission parameters separately for sons and daughters and for mothers and fathers (24 parameters in all). Subsequently, we imposed equality constraints on these parameters across genders of parents and offspring to test the differences in influence across various parent-offspring gender combinations. Parameters were obtained by Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML). An advantage of FIML is that it handles missing data optimally: it exploits all available data and is more efficient than list- or pairwise deletion. Model fit was evaluated using the following fit indices: comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). CFI values range from 0 to 1, with larger values indicating better fit and a recommended cutoff of .95 (Xia & Yang, Reference Xia and Yang2019). RMSEA is an absolute fit index, based on the fact that the fitted model is an approximation to the true model. Interpretation of RMSEA values is often considered according to the following: : 0 = perfect fit; <.05 = close fit; .05 to .08 = fair fit; .08 to .10 = mediocre fit; >.10 = poor fit (Byrne, Reference Byrne1998). SRMR is defined as the difference between the observed correlations and the model implied correlation matrix. As a rule of thumb, an SRMR ≤ .08 indicates an acceptable fit (Xia & Yang, Reference Xia and Yang2019). Thus, it allows assessing the average magnitude of the discrepancies between observed and expected correlations as an absolute measure of (model) fit criterion.

Figure 1. Intergenerational transmission of BMI and educational outcomes, model 1 (full model). The figure shows the full SEM. The separate transmission coefficients for sons and daughters, cross-trait correlations for twin 2 and cross-trait, cross-twin correlations are not shown in the figure to avoid clutter.

Note: EA, educational attainment; CITO, standardized educational achievement test; MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Parents

Table 1 contains a summary of the average age, BMI and EA for the parents. Mean age at first survey of fathers at was slightly higher than mothers (33.8 vs. 31.4 years). Mean BMI was 24.9 for fathers and 23.9 for mothers. BMI variance was higher for mothers (14.4, 95% CI [14.1, 14.8]) than fathers 9.0, 95% CI [8.8, 9.3]); age was unrelated to BMI in both parents. The EA level of fathers was comparable to the EA of mothers, with fathers having slightly higher percentage of higher vocational training and university education, and mothers having slightly higher percentage of intermediate and higher secondary schooling (see Table 1). The variance of EA was higher in fathers than in mothers (.82, 95% CI [.80, .84] vs. .73, 95% CI [.72, .74]).

Table 1. Age, BMI, and EA in parents

Note: EA, educational attainment.

Offspring

Table 2 summarizes sample characteristics in offspring. Mean BMI at age 4 was slightly higher in boys (15.35, 95% CI [15.32, 15.37]) than girls (15.16, 95% CI [15.14, 15.19]), while mean BMI at age 12 was slightly higher in girls (17.58, 95% CI [17.52, 17.64]) than boys (17.31, 95% CI [17.26, 17.37]). BMI4 variance was 1.50 for boys and 1.71 for girls, while BMI12 variance was 5.66 for boys and 6.58 for girls. Mean CITO scores were marginally higher in boys (538.5, 95% CI [538.3, 538.7], variance: 71.2) than girls (537.3, 95% CI [537.1, 537.5], variance: 76.5).

Table 2. BMI4, BMI12, CITO in offspring

Note: CITO, standardized educational achievement test.

Full model

Table 3 shows results for the full model, which is depicted in Figure 1. Within traits, the transmission parameters were significant for all traits and all parent offspring combinations. In the regression of BMI4 on parental BMI, the standardized regression (i.e., transmission) coefficients (betas) ranged from .11 to .15. In the regression of BMI12 on parental BMI, the range was from .16 to .28. In the regression of the twins’ CITO scores on parental EA, the coefficients ranged from .18 to .20. In the regression analysis concerning cross-trait transmission — that is, parental EA to offspring BMI and parental BMI to offspring EA — we obtained mixed results. In the regression of BMI4 on the parental EA, we found that maternal EA significantly predicted daughters’ BMI, with a standardized beta of .04. In the regression of BMI12 on parental EA, we found that paternal EA significantly predicted BMI in both sons and daughters, with a standardized beta of −.04 and −.07 respectively; that is, a higher father’s EA was associated with lower BMI at age 12. In the regression of CITO on parental BMI, we found that maternal BMI significantly predicted CITO scores in both sons and daughters, with a standardized beta of −.05; that is, a higher maternal BMI was associated with lower CITO scores in children.

Table 3. Direct and indirect regression coefficients, full model

Note: EA, educational attainment; CITO, standardized educational achievement test. *significant p value based on a Holm-Bonferroni corrected target α of .05 with 24 correlated tests

Gender differences in transmission parameters

To assess gender differences in transmission from fathers and mothers to sons and daughters, we first estimated the transmission coefficients freely, and then imposed equality constraints on the transmission coefficients. We tested these constraints by means of likelihood ratio tests. The omnibus test of all 12 equality constraints for male and female offspring was not significant (df = 12, p = .139; see Supplementary Table S1). Subsequent individual (post hoc) tests of equality similarly were not significant. So, we concluded that the transmission coefficients are equal in male and female offspring. We then tested whether the transmission coefficients of the fathers equaled those of the mothers. The omnibus test was not significant (df = 6, p = .074; see Supplementary Table S2). Subsequent individual (post hoc) tests of equality similarly were not significant. Based on these results, we arrived at model 2. Parameter estimates in this model are shown in Table 4 and summarized in Figure 2. These results indicate small, but significant, effects for cross-trait transmission from parents to offspring BMI and CITO. The within-trait transmission coefficient of BMI4 (.10) was lower than other coefficients (BMI12: .20, CITO: .19). Estimates in this reduced model 2 were similar to those in the full model. Both models had good model fit measures, with negligible differences in fit indices: comparative fit index (CFI), .83; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) .068; and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), .061. The SRMR and RMSEA indicate acceptable model fit, while the CFI is below the recommended cutoff of .95. The chi square likelihood ratio test of model 1 (χ2: 5319.7, df: 374) vs model 2 (χ2: 5348.5, df: 392) had a p-value of .0509, indicating no significant difference between the two models. Model fit measures are presented in Supplementary Table S3.

Table 4. Regression coefficients of parental on offspring measures and residual correlations, reduced model

Note: EA, educational attainment; CITO, standardized educational achievement test. *significant p value based on a Holm-Bonferroni corrected target α of .05 with 12 correlated tests.

Figure 2. Intergenerational transmission of BMI and educational outcome, reduced model. The figure displays the transmission of parental BMI and educational attainment to children’s BMI and academic achievement, based on data from 13,916 families. The figure shows the estimated effects (in standardized units) of parental BMI and educational attainment on their children’s BMI (at ages 4 and 12) and academic achievement (nationally standardized test at age 12), as well as the residual correlations between BMI and academic achievement. The paths with an asterisk (*) had a significant p value based on a Holm-Bonferroni corrected target α of .05 with 12 correlated tests. Paths from parents to children were similar in boys and girls as well as fathers and mothers. The figure shows this reduced model, without gender differences in intergenerational transmission. The residual correlations are given separately for boys/girls.

Note: EA, educational attainment.

Within-person, spousal, and twin correlations

We observed negative within-person EA-BMI correlations for both parents (fathers: -.10; mothers: −.08) (see Table 5) (we obtained the same estimates in the SEM). The observed spousal correlation for EA was .46 (same estimate in the SEM). The observed spousal correlation for BMI was .22, and its estimate in the SEM was .21. Observed cross-trait spousal correlations were -.10 for father’s EA with mother’s BMI and −.08 for mother’s EA with father’s BMI, and we obtained the same estimates in SEM. In offspring, the observed correlations between BMI4 and CITO were positive (boys: .048; girls: .057), and the residual correlations (i.e., after taking parental EA/BMI into account) were estimated in the SEM at .057 for boys and .046 for girls. The observed correlations between BMI12 and CITO were negative (boys: −.052; girls: −.070). However, the residual correlations were not significant in the SEM after applying Holm-Bonferroni correction. The observed correlations between BMI4 and BMI12 were .43 (boys) and.46 (girls), and their residuals did not differ appreciably in the SEM. The observed MZ twin correlations were less than twice DZ correlations for all traits. The MZ and DZ correlations for BMI at age 4 equaled .84 and .47 respectively. For BMI at age 12, these are .90 and .50, respectively. For CITO, the correlations are .81 and .45, respectively (see Table 6).

Table 5. Observed covariance and correlation table for parental and offspring variables

Note: EA, educational attainment; CITO, standardized educational achievement test at age 12. Upper triangle, covariance. Lower triangle: correlation. Diagonal: variance.

Table 6. Observed/residual twin correlations

Note: MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic; CITO, standardized educational achievement test at age 12. Lower triangle, cross-trait correlations. Diagonal, within-trait correlations.

Discussion

In a large number of two-generation Dutch nuclear families we looked at the transmission from parents to offspring for BMI and educational attainment. Within traits, transmission for BMI induced a positive association between parental and offspring BMI at age 4 and BMI at 12 in the SEM. The association was twice as strong in 12-year-olds than in 4-year-olds, that is, r = .20 versus .10 (based on a reduced model, in which transmission coefficients were equal for all parent-offspring gender combinations). The parent-adolescent BMI transmission coefficient (.20) is in the same range of transmission coefficients observed in our prior study in an independent sample from the Netherlands of parents and their adult offspring sample (i.e., .15 to .26; Alrouh et al., Reference Alrouh, van Bergen, de Zeeuw, Dolan and Boomsma2022). For educational attainment, the observed parent-offspring correlation (r = ∼.28) was higher than the standardized transmission coefficient implied by SEM (.19), suggesting that parental BMI does contribute to the correlation between parental EA and academic achievement in adolescents.

Across traits, offspring BMI at 4 years was positively correlated with parental EA, while offspring BMI at 12 years was negatively correlated with parental EA. In 4-year-olds, the observed correlation was ∼.02 in mothers and negligible in fathers, and the standardized transmission coefficient (beta) was .02 in the reduced model. This weak correlation parallels findings in previous twin and population-based studies, which show no association (Morgen et al., Reference Morgen, Andersen, Mortensen, Howe, Rasmussen, Due, Sørensen and Andersen2017) or slightly lower BMI in the offspring of mothers with lower education levels (Howe et al., Reference Howe, Tilling, Galobardes, Smith, Ness and Lawlor2011; Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Jelenkovic, Latvala, Yokoyama, Sund, Sugawara, Tanaka, Matsumoto, Aaltonen, Piirtola, Freitas, Maia, Öncel, Aliev, Ji, Ning, Pang, Rebato, Saudino and Kaprio2019). In 12-year-olds, the observed correlation was about -.10, and the standardized beta was about −.04. After accounting for parental BMI, we saw a slight decrease in the strength of correlation between parental EA and BMI in adolescents. This decrease is in line with our earlier study of adult offspring, where we found that accounting for parental BMI all but eliminated the effect of parental EA on offspring BMI. In our current study, however, the association between parental EA and offspring BMI at age 12 remained statistically significant. This suggests that while parental BMI may drive some of the association between parental EA and offspring BMI, it does not account for the entire association. Parental EA does in fact have a distinctive effect (i.e., after taking parental BMI into account) on increasing the risk of obesity in adolescence, even if this effect decreases as children reach adulthood. This association is possibly due, but not limited, to the effects of parental EA on household income and associated consequences for breastfeeding (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2009), nutrition (Darmon & Drewnowski, Reference Darmon and Drewnowski2015) and physical activity (Telford et al., Reference Telford, Telford, Cochrane, Cunningham, Olive and Davey2016).

There were generally no significant differences in transmission parameters between fathers and mothers to sons and daughters. At the offspring level, the lack of sex differences in the transmission parameters might be explained by the relatively young age of offspring (Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Bartels, Posthuma, Estourgie-van Burk, Willemsen, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2007), where gender specific differences have yet to develop to be seen in adulthood (Schousboe et al., Reference Schousboe, Willemsen, Kyvik, Mortensen, Boomsma, Cornes, Davis, Fagnani, Hjelmborg, Kaprio, de Lange, Luciano, Martin, Pedersen, Pietiläinen, Rissanen, Saarni, Sørensen, van Baal and Harris2003). On the parental level, the lack of differences in transmission between fathers and mothers might be due to the relatively high level of gender equality in the Netherlands, which would result in a more comparable effect of the EAs of father and mother on household income.

Observed correlations and residual correlations (after accounting for transmission in the SEM) of academic achievement in adolescence and BMI in childhood were positive at r = ∼.05, indicating that a higher BMI in childhood was associated with better academic achievement later in life. In 12-year-olds, however, the observed correlation between academic achievement and BMI was negative (r = ∼−.06), but the residual correlation was not significant. This result contrasts with our prior results in adults, where the residual correlation between BMI and EA remained significant at r = -.10. Whereas our measure of EA at age 12 is mainly a reflection of cognitive ability (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, Rietveld, Van Baal and Boomsma2002), it is likely that additional factors influence the highest educational degree attained in adulthood.

Our study design allowed us to estimate the phenotypic intergenerational transmission of BMI and EA. The present phenotypic model does not address environmental or cultural transmission in the presence of genetic transmission. This question can be addressed by applying the parents and twins design, which specifies both association between parents and their twins stemming from both types of transmission, in the presence of phenotypic assortative mating (Fulker, Reference Fulker, Weir, Eisen, Goodman and Namkoong1988; Keller et al., Reference Keller, Medland, Duncan, Hatemi, Neale, Maes and Eaves2009). The present data are suitable to conduct such a follow-up study. Alternatively, polygenic scores can be used to detect phenotypic transmission in the presence of genetic transmission. This approach requires genetic data in the in the parents and their twins, and phenotypic data in the twins (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maher, Medland, McAloney, Wright, Hansell, Kendler, Martin and Gillespie2018; Hart et al., Reference Hart, Little and van Bergen2021; Kong et al., Reference Kong, Thorleifsson, Frigge, Vilhjalmsson, Young, Thorgeirsson, Benonisdottir, Oddsson, Halldorsson, Masson, Gudbjartsson, Helgason, Bjornsdottir, Thorsteinsdottir and Stefansson2018; Okbay et al., Reference Okbay, Wu, Wang, Jayashankar, Bennett, Nehzati, Sidorenko, Kweon, Goldman, Gjorgjieva, Jiang, Hicks, Tian, Hinds, Ahlskog, Magnusson, Oskarsson, Hayward, Campbell and Young2022).

It is well known that positive cultural transmission is a source of additive genetic (A) and common environment (C) covariance, and that A-C covariance biases upwards the estimated C variance component in the classical twin design (Purcell, Reference Purcell2002). The C component of the CITO scores is known to be low in the Netherlands. For CITO, we have previously reported standardized variance components of 0.74 (A), 0.08 (C), and 0.18 for unique environment (E; de Zeeuw et al., Reference de Zeeuw, van Beijsterveldt, Glasner, de Geus and Boomsma2016). In contrast, the twin correlations for BMI suggest a substantial C component. We have previously reported on BMI standardized variance components of ∼.65(A), ∼.16(C), and ∼.19(E) at age 4 and ∼.78 (A), ∼.11(C) and ∼.10(E) at age 12 (Silventoinen et al., Reference Silventoinen, Bartels, Posthuma, Estourgie-van Burk, Willemsen, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2007). The small C component of CITO does not rule out cultural transmission, but does suggest that the effect of cultural transmission is likely to be small. Indeed, in previous work we found for children’s CITO scores no genetic-nurture effect (de Zeeuw et al., Reference de Zeeuw, Hottenga, Ouwens, Dolan, Ehli, Davies, Boomsma and van Bergen2020). The larger C component of BMI could well be attributable partly to cultural transmission.

The main strengths of this study are a large sample size and a wide age range for parents. Our study sample covers different geographic areas and socioeconomic classes in the Netherlands. Recruitment of twins — who are generally considered representative of the general population to which they belong (Hur et al., Reference Hur, Bogl, Ordoñana, Taylor, Hart, Tuvblad, Ystrom, Dalgård, Skytthe and Willemsen2019) — and their families into the NTR was done through multiple channels, leading to a sample that is reasonably representative of the Dutch population. Although the study relied on self-reported measures for height, weight and EA for parents and offspring, the reliability in self-reporting of height and weight is good: in an analysis of a subsample of 6026 individuals, we observed a correlation of .93 between self-reported BMI and BMI measured by a research nurse or assistant (Willemsen et al., Reference Willemsen, Geus, Bartels, Beijsterveldt, Brooks, Burk, Fugman, Hoekstra, Hottenga, Kluft, Meijer, Montgomery, Rizzu, Sondervan, Smit, Spijker, Suchiman, Tischfield, Lehner and Boomsma2010). We analyzed BMI as a continuous variable rather than a (clinical) binary variable (obese/overweight vs. normal weight), because we wanted to address the process of transmission as it pertains to the full range of BMI in the general population in the Netherlands. In this approach, we assume that the process of transmission as we identify it is relevant to the full range of BMI and EA. We note that it is possible to fit our present model (see Figure 1) to a binary BMI variable, but this would merely result in a loss of information. Specifically, the analysis in the case of a binary BMI variable is based on the liability threshold model, in which the model is fitted to latent continuous BMI variables. There is no advantage to this compared to fitting the directly to observed continuous BMI variables, but there is the disadvantage of a loss of power associated with loss of information. For this reason, we opted to use BMI as a continuous variable in our analysis.

There are also some limitations to the current study. While BMI is a common, convenient measure of obesity, other methods, such as skinfold thickness and percent body fat from dual energy X-ray absorptiometry, may provide more accurate estimates. In our analyses, we have modeled the process of transmission of EA and BMI from adult parents to adult offspring. Our resulting model is supposed to represents this process as it takes place in the general Dutch population with respect to the full range of BMI. As such, this model assumes that the process is the same regardless of the actual level of BMI in the parents or their offspring. We recognize that it is possible that the transmission process may differ in extremes of the BMI distribution. Detecting such heterogeneity is a statistically challenging task, which is beyond our present aim.

Our results pertain to young offspring who are still in the parental household, whereas our earlier study examined intergenerational transmission in adult offspring. The differences between findings in young and adult offspring may highlight different parental effects on child and adult obesity, especially given that younger offspring share the same household environment with their parents while adult offspring do not. One of the striking findings is the persistent association between low BMI in childhood and low academic achievement in adolescence, even after accounting for parental transmission. This highlights the need to address childhood underweight as well as overweight, as either condition can cause a different set of adverse outcomes later on in life.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2023.18.

Financial support

This work was supported by The Dutch Research Council (NWO): The impact of parental genes on offspring health: nurture via nature (Hestia; VidW.1154.19.013); Zwaartekracht CID: Individual development: Why some children thrive, and others don’t (through the Gravitation program of the Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science and the NWO (Grant 024.001.003)); Twin-family database for behavior genetics and genomics studies (NWO-MagW NWO 480-04-004); and 480-15-001/674: Netherlands Twin Registry Repository: researching the interplay between genome and environment. Elsje van Bergen is a Jacobs Foundation Research Fellow.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standard

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Aickin, M., & Gensler, H. (1996). Adjusting for multiple testing when reporting research results: The Bonferroni vs Holm methods. American Journal of Public Health, 86, 726728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alrouh, H., van Bergen, E., de Zeeuw, E., Dolan, C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2022). Intergenerational transmission of body mass index and associations with educational attainment. BMC Public Health, 22, 890. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13270-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, M., Rietveld, M. J. H., Van Baal, G. C. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2002). Heritability of educational achievement in 12-year-olds and the overlap with cognitive ability. Twin Research, 5, 544553. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905202762342017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Breastfeeding, maternal education and cognitive function: A prospective study in twins. Behavior Genetics, 39, 616622. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-009-9293-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, T. C., Maher, B. S., Medland, S. E., McAloney, K., Wright, M. J., Hansell, N. K., Kendler, K. S., Martin, N. G., & Gillespie, N. A. (2018). The nature of nurture: Using a virtual-parent design to test parenting effects on children’s educational attainment in genotyped families. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 21, 7383. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2018.11 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benson, R., von Hippel, P. T., & Lynch, J. L. (2018). Does more education cause lower BMI, or do lower-BMI individuals become more educated? Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979. Social Science & Medicine, 211, 370377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.03.042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, É. M., Derraik, J. G. B., Taylor, R. W., & Cutfield, W. S. (2018). Prediction models for early childhood obesity: Applicability and existing issues. Hormone Research in Paediatrics, 90, 358367. https://doi.org/10.1159/000496563 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Caleza, C., Yañez-Vico, R. M., Mendoza, A., & Iglesias-Linares, A. (2016). Childhood obesity and delayed gratification behavior: A systematic review of experimental studies. The Journal of Pediatrics, 169, 201207.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.10.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Citogroep. (2002). Cito: Eindtoets basisonderwijs. https://www.cito.nl/ Google Scholar
Craft, S., & Stennis Watson, G. (2004). Insulin and neurodegenerative disease: Shared and specific mechanisms. The Lancet Neurology, 3, 169178. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00681-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, S. R. (2009). Complications of obesity in children and adolescents. International Journal of Obesity, 33, S6065. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2009.20 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darmon, N., & Drewnowski, A. (2015). Contribution of food prices and diet cost to socioeconomic disparities in diet quality and health: A systematic review and analysis. Nutrition Reviews, 73, 643660. https://doi.org/10.1093/nutrit/nuv027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Zeeuw, E. L., Hottenga, J.-J., Ouwens, K. G., Dolan, C. V., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Boomsma, D. I., & van Bergen, E. (2020). Intergenerational transmission of education and ADHD: Effects of parental genotypes. Behavior Genetics, 50, 221232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-020-09992-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Zeeuw, E. L., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Glasner, T. J., de Geus, E. J. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2016). Arithmetic, reading and writing performance has a strong genetic component: A study in primary school children. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 156166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.01.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drewnowski, A. (2010). The cost of US foods as related to their nutritive value. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 92, 11811188. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2010.29300 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estourgie-van Burk, G. F., Bartels, M., Boomsma, D. I., & Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A. (2010). Body size of twins compared with siblings and the general population: From birth to late adolescence. The Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 586591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.10.045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fulker, D. (1988). Genetic and cultural transmission in human behavior. In Weir, S. B., Eisen, E. J., Goodman, M. M. & Namkoong, G. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Quantitative Genetics (pp. 318340). Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Golsteyn, B. H. H., Grönqvist, H., & Lindahl, L. (2014). Adolescent Time Preferences Predict Lifetime Outcomes. The Economic Journal, 124, F739F761. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecoj.12095 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, S. A., Little, C., & van Bergen, E. (2021). Nurture might be nature: Cautionary tales and proposed solutions. NPJ Science of Learning, 6, 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-020-00079-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, J. (2007). Leptin regulation of neuronal excitability and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 7, 643647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2007.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holm, S. (1979). A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 6570.Google Scholar
Howe, L. D., Tilling, K., Galobardes, B., Smith, G. D., Ness, A. R., & Lawlor, D. A. (2011). Socioeconomic disparities in trajectories of adiposity across childhood. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 6, e144e153. https://doi.org/10.3109/17477166.2010.500387 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hur, Y.-M., Bogl, L. H., Ordoñana, J. R., Taylor, J., Hart, S. A., Tuvblad, C., Ystrom, E., Dalgård, C., Skytthe, A., & Willemsen, G. (2019). Twin Family Registries Worldwide: An Important Resource for Scientific Research. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22, 427437. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.121 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. C., Medland, S. E., Duncan, L. E., Hatemi, P. K., Neale, M. C., Maes, H. H. M., & Eaves, L. J. (2009). Modeling extended twin family data I: Description of the cascade model. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 818. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.12.1.8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleiser, C., Schaffrath Rosario, A., Mensink, G. B., Prinz-Langenohl, R., & Kurth, B.-M. (2009). Potential determinants of obesity among children and adolescents in Germany: Results from the cross-sectional KiGGS study. BMC Public Health, 9, 46. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-9-46 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, A., Thorleifsson, G., Frigge, M. L., Vilhjalmsson, B. J., Young, A. I., Thorgeirsson, T. E., Benonisdottir, S., Oddsson, A., Halldorsson, B. V., Masson, G., Gudbjartsson, D. F., Helgason, A., Bjornsdottir, G., Thorsteinsdottir, U., & Stefansson, K. (2018). The nature of nurture: Effects of parental genotypes. Science, 359 (6374), 424428. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan6877 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamerz, A., Kuepper-Nybelen, J., Wehle, C., Bruning, N., Trost-Brinkhues, G., Brenner, H., Hebebrand, J., & Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2005). Social class, parental education, and obesity prevalence in a study of six-year-old children in Germany. International Journal of Obesity, 29, 373380. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802914 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, N. C., Krabbendam, L., Dekker, S., Boschloo, A., de Groot, R. H. M., & Jolles, J. (2012). Academic motivation mediates the influence of temporal discounting on academic achievement during adolescence. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1, 4348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2012.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y., Cai, T., Wang, H., & Guo, G. (2021). Achieved educational attainment, inherited genetic endowment for education, and obesity. Biodemography and Social Biology, 66, 132144. https://doi.org/10.1080/19485565.2020.1869919 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ligthart, L., Beijsterveldt, C. E. M. van, Kevenaar, S. T., Zeeuw, E. de, Bergen, E. van, Bruins, S., Pool, R., Helmer, Q., Dongen, J. van, Hottenga, J.-J., Ent, D. van’t, Dolan, C. V., Davies, G. E., Ehli, E. A., Bartels, M., Willemsen, G., Geus, E. J. C. de, & Boomsma, D. I. (2019). The Netherlands Twin Register: Longitudinal research based on twin and twin-family designs. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22, 623636. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.93 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maes, H. H. M., Neale, M. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1997). Genetic and environmental factors in relative body weight and human adiposity. Behavior Genetics, 27, 325351. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025635913927 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthiessen, J., Stockmarr, A., Biltoft-Jensen, A., Fagt, S., Zhang, H., & Groth, M. V. (2014). Trends in overweight and obesity in Danish children and adolescents: 2000-2008 ¾ exploring changes according to parental education. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 42, 385392. https://doi.org/10.1177/1403494813520356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of Gratification in Children. Science, 244, 933938. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgen, C. S., Andersen, P. K., Mortensen, L. H., Howe, L. D., Rasmussen, M., Due, P., Sørensen, T. I. A., & Andersen, A.-M. N. (2017). Socioeconomic disparities in birth weight and body mass index during infancy through age 7 years: A study within the Danish National Birth Cohort. BMJ Open, 7, e011781. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011781 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murasko, J. E. (2009). Socioeconomic status, height, and obesity in children. Economics & Human Biology, 7, 376386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2009.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newton, S., Braithwaite, D., & Akinyemiju, T. F. (2017). Socio-economic status over the life course and obesity: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS One, 12, e0177151. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177151 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nogueira, H., Gama, A., Mourão, I., Marques, V., Ferrão, M., & Padez, C. (2013). The associations of SES, obesity, sport activity, and perceived neighborhood environments: Is there a model of environmental injustice penalizing portuguese children? American Journal of Human Biology, 25, 434436. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.22384 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Dea, J. A., & Dibley, M. J. (2010). Obesity increase among low SES Australian schoolchildren between 2000 and 2006: Time for preventive interventions to target children from low income schools? International Journal of Public Health, 55, 185192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-009-0079-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okbay, A., Wu, Y., Wang, N., Jayashankar, H., Bennett, M., Nehzati, S. M., Sidorenko, J., Kweon, H., Goldman, G., Gjorgjieva, T., Jiang, Y., Hicks, B., Tian, C., Hinds, D. A., Ahlskog, R., Magnusson, P. K. E., Oskarsson, S., Hayward, C., Campbell, A., … Young, A. I. (2022). Polygenic prediction of educational attainment within and between families from genome-wide association analyses in 3 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 54, 437449. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01016-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance Components models for gene–environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5, 554571. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.5.6.554 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schousboe, K., Willemsen, G., Kyvik, K. O., Mortensen, J., Boomsma, D. I., Cornes, B. K., Davis, C. J., Fagnani, C., Hjelmborg, J., Kaprio, J., de Lange, M., Luciano, M., Martin, N. G., Pedersen, N., Pietiläinen, K. H., Rissanen, A., Saarni, S., Sørensen, T. I. A., van Baal, G. C. M., & Harris, J. R. (2003). Sex differences in heritability of BMI: A comparative study of results from twin studies in eight countries. Twin Research, 6, 409421. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905203770326411 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silventoinen, K., Bartels, M., Posthuma, D., Estourgie-van Burk, G. F., Willemsen, G., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Genetic regulation of growth in height and weight from 3 to 12 years of age: A longitudinal study of Dutch twin children. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 354363. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.10.2.354 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silventoinen, K., Jelenkovic, A., Latvala, A., Yokoyama, Y., Sund, R., Sugawara, M., Tanaka, M., Matsumoto, S., Aaltonen, S., Piirtola, M., Freitas, D. L., Maia, J. A., Öncel, S. Y., Aliev, F., Ji, F., Ning, F., Pang, Z., Rebato, E., Saudino, K. J., … Kaprio, J. (2019). Parental education and genetics of BMI from infancy to old age: A pooled analysis of 29 twin cohorts. Obesity, 27, 855865. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22451 Google ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, ThorkildI. A., Sonne-Holm, S., & Christensen, U. (1983). Cognitive deficiency in obesity independent of social origin. The Lancet, 321, 11051106. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(83)91940-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Springer, A. E., Li, L., Ranjit, N., Delk, J., Mehta, K., & Kelder, S. H. (2015). School-level economic disadvantage and obesity in middle school children in central Texas, USA: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 12, S8. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-12-S1-S8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoklosa, M., Shuval, K., Drope, J., Tchernis, R., Pachucki, M., Yaroch, A., & Harding, M. (2018). The intergenerational transmission of obesity: The role of time preferences and self-control. Economics & Human Biology, 28, 92106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2017.12.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Telford, R. M., Telford, R. D., Cochrane, T., Cunningham, R. B., Olive, L. S., & Davey, R. (2016). The influence of sport club participation on physical activity, fitness and body fat during childhood and adolescence: The LOOK Longitudinal Study. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 19, 400406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2015.04.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vainik, U., Baker, T. E., Dadar, M., Zeighami, Y., Michaud, A., Zhang, Y., García Alanis, J. C., Misic, B., Collins, D. L., & Dagher, A. (2018). Neurobehavioral correlates of obesity are largely heritable. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 93129317. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1718206115 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vos, M. B., & Welsh, J. (2010). Childhood obesity: Update on predisposing factors and prevention strategies. Current Gastroenterology Reports, 12, 280287. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11894-010-0116-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y. (2001). Cross-national comparison of childhood obesity: The epidemic and the relationship between obesity and socioeconomic status. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30(5), 11291136. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/30.5.1129 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Min, J., Khuri, J., & Li, M. (2017). A systematic examination of the association between parental and child obesity across countries. Advances in Nutrition, 8, 436448. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.116.013235 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., & Zhang, Q. (2006). Are American children and adolescents of low socioeconomic status at increased risk of obesity? Changes in the association between overweight and family income between 1971 and 2002. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 84, 707716. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/84.4.707 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, P. A., Awad, Y. A., Gauvin, L., Spencer, N. J., McGrath, J. J., Clifford, S. A., Nikiema, B., Yang-Huang, J., Goldhaber-Fiebert, J. D., Markham, W., Mensah, F. K., van Grieken, A., Raat, H., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Ludvigsson, J., & Faresjö, T. (2022). Household income and maternal education in early childhood and risk of overweight and obesity in late childhood: Findings from seven birth cohort studies in six high-income countries. International Journal of Obesity, 46(9), 17031711. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41366-022-01171-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willemsen, G., Geus, E. J. C. de, Bartels, M., Beijsterveldt, C. E. M. T. van, Brooks, A. I., Burk, G. F. E., Fugman, D. A., Hoekstra, C., Hottenga, J.-J., Kluft, K., Meijer, P., Montgomery, G. W., Rizzu, P., Sondervan, D., Smit, A. B., Spijker, S., Suchiman, H. E. D., Tischfield, J. A., Lehner, T., … Boomsma, D. I. (2010). The Netherlands Twin Register Biobank: A resource for genetic epidemiological studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 13, 231245. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.13.3.231 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xia, Y., & Yang, Y. (2019). RMSEA, CFI, and TLI in structural equation modeling with ordered categorical data: The story they tell depends on the estimation methods. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 409428. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1055-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Intergenerational transmission of BMI and educational outcomes, model 1 (full model). The figure shows the full SEM. The separate transmission coefficients for sons and daughters, cross-trait correlations for twin 2 and cross-trait, cross-twin correlations are not shown in the figure to avoid clutter.Note: EA, educational attainment; CITO, standardized educational achievement test; MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic.

Figure 1

Table 1. Age, BMI, and EA in parents

Figure 2

Table 2. BMI4, BMI12, CITO in offspring

Figure 3

Table 3. Direct and indirect regression coefficients, full model

Figure 4

Table 4. Regression coefficients of parental on offspring measures and residual correlations, reduced model

Figure 5

Figure 2. Intergenerational transmission of BMI and educational outcome, reduced model. The figure displays the transmission of parental BMI and educational attainment to children’s BMI and academic achievement, based on data from 13,916 families. The figure shows the estimated effects (in standardized units) of parental BMI and educational attainment on their children’s BMI (at ages 4 and 12) and academic achievement (nationally standardized test at age 12), as well as the residual correlations between BMI and academic achievement. The paths with an asterisk (*) had a significant p value based on a Holm-Bonferroni corrected target α of .05 with 12 correlated tests. Paths from parents to children were similar in boys and girls as well as fathers and mothers. The figure shows this reduced model, without gender differences in intergenerational transmission. The residual correlations are given separately for boys/girls.Note: EA, educational attainment.

Figure 6

Table 5. Observed covariance and correlation table for parental and offspring variables

Figure 7

Table 6. Observed/residual twin correlations

Supplementary material: PDF

Alrouh et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Alrouh et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 625.1 KB