Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T15:28:22.325Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sleep Duration During Adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2013

Saskia J. te Velde
Affiliation:
EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Niels van der Aa
Affiliation:
Stichting Centrum ‘45, Oegstgeest, The Netherlands
Dorret I. Boomsma
Affiliation:
EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Eus J. W. van Someren
Affiliation:
Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience (NIN), Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Integrative Neurophysiology, Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Eco J. C. de Geus
Affiliation:
EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Johannes Brug
Affiliation:
EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Meike Bartels*
Affiliation:
EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
address for correspondence: Dr Meike Bartels, Department of Biological Psychology and EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University, Van der Boechorststraat 5, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study assessed to what extent genetic and environmental factors contributed to individual differences in adolescent sleep duration, and whether genetic and environmental contributions to sleep duration changed throughout adolescence. A twin-family design was used to gain insight into the genetic and environmental contributions to variation in sleep duration. The study sample consisted of 6,319 adolescent twins (44% males) and 1,359 non-twin siblings (44% males) in the age range of 12 to 20 years (mean age = 16.85, SD = 1.40). The participants self-reported usual sleep duration, which was categorized as less than 8 hours per night, 8–9 hours per night, and more than 9 hours per night. Results showed that the prevalence of shorter than optimum sleep duration, that is, less than 8 hours per night, was high, with the highest prevalence rates in later adolescence. The contribution of genetic and environmental factors to individual differences in sleep duration was dependent on age. Variation in sleep duration at the age of 12 years was accounted for by genetic (boys: 34%, girls: 36%), shared environmental (boys: 28%, girls: 45%), and non-shared environmental factors (boys: 38%, girls: 19%). At the age of 20 years, the role of genetic (boys: 47%, girls: 33%) and non-shared environmental factors (boys: 53%, girls: 67%) was more pronounced. It can be concluded from the results that individual differences in sleep duration were accounted for by genetic and non-shared environmental factors throughout adolescence, whereas shared environmental factors account for a substantial part of variation during early adolescence only.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013 

Sleep is essential in human life, and we spend about one-third of our lives sleeping. During childhood and adolescence, sleep plays an important role in growth, maturation, and physical and mental health (Mindell et al., Reference Mindell, Owens and Carskadon1999). Research regarding adolescents’ sleep duration indicated that those reporting short sleep duration reported higher levels of depressed moods (Fallone et al., Reference Fallone, Owens and Deane2002; Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008) and suboptimal performance at school (Fallone et al., Reference Fallone, Owens and Deane2002; Wolfson & Carskadon, Reference Wolfson and Carskadon2003) than those reporting optimal sleep duration. Sleep duration is also associated with physiological factors, such as glucose tolerance, insulin sensitivity, leptin and ghrelin levels, hunger, endocrine function, and appetite (Spiegel et al., Reference Spiegel, Leproult and Cauter1999; Spruyt et al., Reference Spruyt, Molfese and Gozal2011; Van Cauter & Knutson, Reference Van Cauter and Knutson2008; Van Cauter et al., Reference Van Cauter, Splegel, Tasali and Leproult2008). In addition, reviews and original studies have reported that short sleep duration is associated with overweight (Altenburg et al., Reference Altenburg, Chinapaw, van der Knaap, Brug, Manios and Singh2013; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang and Li2012; Taheri et al., Reference Taheri, Lin, Austin, Young and Mignot2004); however, the evidence remains inconclusive due to methodological limitations (Guidolin & Gradisar, Reference Guidolin and Gradisar2012).

Despite the beneficial effects of adequate sleep, over past years a decline in sleep duration among children and adolescents has been observed (Matricciani et al., Reference Matricciani, Olds and Petkov2012), suggesting that many children and adolescents do not get adequate sleep. Moreover, studying sleep duration among adolescents is of particular interest because sleep duration on school nights steadily decreases from an average of 10 to 11 hours during childhood to approximately 8 hours during adolescence (Fallone et al., Reference Fallone, Owens and Deane2002; Iglowstein et al., Reference Iglowstein, Jenni, Molinari and Largo2003), that is, to sleep duration that is shorter than the amount of sleep they actually need during school nights (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo2002; Carskadon et al., Reference Carskadon, Harvey, Duke, Anders, Litt and Dement1980). Adolescents start to feel sleepy later in the evening as a result of changes in the relative phase of circadian rhythms occurring during puberty (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo2002; Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008). In addition, for many adolescents’ bedtimes are delayed due to, for example, increased amount of time spent on extracurricular activities (e.g., sports and music), jobs, homework, personal computer and Internet activities, playing video games, and social interaction with peers (Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008). In combination with early school hours, late bedtimes result in shorter sleep duration (Hysing et al., Reference Hysing, Pallesen, Stormark, Lundervold and Sivertsen2013). The proportion of slow wave sleep (i.e., the restorative state of sleep) relative to rapid eye movement (REM) sleep decreases by approximately 40% during adolescence (Mindell et al., Reference Mindell, Owens and Carskadon1999; Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008). To get the same amount of restorative sleep, adolescents’ sleep duration should therefore be longer rather than shorter as compared with younger children's sleep (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo2002; Carskadon et al., Reference Carskadon, Harvey, Duke, Anders, Litt and Dement1980). Adolescent sleep duration may therefore be a possible risk factor for suboptimal performance at school and dysregulation of physical and mental health.

To inform potential prevention and intervention strategies regarding adolescent sleep behavior, insight into the etiology of adolescent sleep duration is needed. Twin-family studies offer the possibility to gain insight into the sources of individual differences in sleep duration by disentangling the total variation into genetic variation, environmental variation due to influences shared by members of the same family (shared environmental influences), and environmental variation due to influences unique to an individual (non-shared environmental influences). A number of twin studies among adults investigated the relative influence of genetic and environmental factors on self-reported sleep duration, and typically reported individual differences in sleep duration were accounted for 30–40% by genetic factors, whereas the remaining part was accounted for by non-shared environmental factors (Barclay & Gregory, Reference Barclay and Gregory2013; de Castro, Reference de Castro2002; Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, Rijsdijk and Eley2006; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Kendler, Eaves and Martin1990; Hublin et al., Reference Hublin, Partinen, Koskenvuo and Kaprio2013; Partinen et al., Reference Partinen, Kaprio, Koskenvuo, Putkonen and Langinvainio1983; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Buchwald, Vitiello, Noonan and Goldberg2010).

It is well known that adolescence is a period of important social, neurobiological, and behavioral changes (Paus et al., Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008). As a consequence, the etiology of sleep duration might change throughout adolescence. For example, influence of parents on adolescents’ sleep duration is likely to decrease during adolescence, whereas the relative influence of extracurricular and social activities might increase. The relative contribution of genetic factors influencing biological processes regulating sleep, such as the circadian rhythm, may also increase during puberty. However, to our knowledge, it has not been investigated whether the relative contribution of genetic and environmental influences to individual differences in sleep duration is subject to change during adolescence.

In the present study, we assessed regular sleep duration in a large sample of Dutch adolescent twins and their non-twin siblings in the age range of 12 to 20 years. The main objective was to assess the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to individual differences in self-reported sleep duration during adolescence and whether the contribution of these factors was dependent on age.

Methods

Participants

Participants were registered as newborns with the Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR), which was established by the Department of Biological Psychology at the VU University in Amsterdam. In 2005, the NTR started to collect data on behavior, well-being, lifestyle, and health in adolescent twins and their non-twin siblings (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, van Beijsterveldt, Derks, Stroet, Polderman, Hudziak and Boomsma2007; Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, de Geus, Vink, Stubbe, Distel, Hottenga and Willemsen2006, Van Beijsterveldt et al., Reference Van Beijsterveldt, Groen-Blokhuis, Hottenga, Franić, Hudziak, Lamb and Boomsma2013). During adolescence, 14-, 16-, and 18-year-old twins and their non-twin siblings (aged 12 to 25 years) received an online or a paper and pencil self-report survey upon written parental consent. A detailed description of the data collection procedures among adolescent twins registered with the NTR was reported elsewhere (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, Van der Aa, van Beijsterveldt, Middeldorp and Boomsma2011). Data collection procedures and the content of self-report survey were approved by an institutional review board on human research.

For the present study, data from surveys collected in adolescent twins (born in cohorts 1986–1994) and their non-twin siblings were analyzed. Data from one additional non-twin sibling per family were included for the present study. From families with more than one additional participating sibling, we selected the sibling closest in age to the twin for the analyses. The total sample consisted of 6,319 twins (44% male) and 1,359 non-twin siblings (44% male) from 3,514 families. In Table 1, the exact constellation of the participating families is presented. Age of the twins and siblings ranged between 12 and 20 years. Mean age of the twins was 15.7 years (SD = 1.4). Mean age of the siblings was 16.9 years (SD = 2.3). Zygosity for 799 (34.4%) of the same-sex twin pairs was determined on the basis of blood group or DNA typing. Zygosity for the remaining same-sex twin pairs was determined by questionnaire items about physical similarities and confusion obtained from family members and strangers, which were provided by parents on multiple time points in previous questionnaires. These items allow accurate determination of zygosity in 93% of the same-sex twin pairs (Rietveld et al., Reference Rietveld, van Der Valk, Bongers, Stroet, Slagboom and Boomsma2000).

TABLE 1 Sample Constellation

1 twin = families with data from one twin (incomplete twin pair); 2 twins = families with data from complete twin pairs; 0 twins + sibling = families with data from one sibling; 1 twin + sibling = families with data from one twin (incomplete twin pair) and one sibling; 2 twins + siblings = families with data from a complete twin pair and one additional sibling; MZ = monozygotic twin pair; DZ = dizygotic twin pair; M = male; F = female; Siblings only = families with data from non-twin siblings only.

Measures

The majority of participants (i.e., 83%) were asked to indicate their usual sleep duration during a regular school or working week on a 3-point scale: 1 = ‘less than 8 hours per night’ (24.3%), 2 = ‘8–9 hours per night’ (66.8%), and 3 = ‘over 9 hours per night’ (8.9%). In the latest self-report surveys, the content of the item regarding sleep duration was slightly changed. Participants in the latest surveys (i.e., 17% of the total sample) were asked to report on their usual sleep duration during the past four weeks on a 6-point scale: 1 = ‘5 hours or less’ (0.7%), 2 = ‘6 hours’ (4.4%), 3 = ‘7 hours’ (17.4%), 4 = ‘8 hours’ (35.7%), 5 = ‘9 hours’ (31.6%), and 6 = ‘10 hours or more’ (10.2%). For each response category the percentage of participants endorsed is shown in parentheses. It can be seen that the distributions of both items are similar. Therefore, data from both data collection procedures were combined by classifying the participants into three categories. The first category consisted of participants who indicated average sleep duration of less than 8 hours per night. The second category consisted of participants who reported average sleep duration between 8 and 9 hours. The third category consisted of those who indicated average sleep duration of over 9 hours per night. Although the self-report measure of sleep duration used in the present study was not formally validated, this measure has already been used in other twin-family studies assessing sleep duration (Heath et al., Reference Heath, Kendler, Eaves and Martin1990; Partinen et al., Reference Partinen, Kaprio, Koskenvuo, Putkonen and Langinvainio1983). It has been reported that among healthy individuals, measurement of sleep duration based on self-report is in agreement with objective laboratory-based measures of sleep duration based on polysomnography (Hoch et al., Reference Hoch, Reynolds, Kupfer, Berman, Houck and Stack1987; Manconi et al., Reference Manconi, Ferri, Sagrada, Punjabi, Tettamanzi, Zucconi and Ferini-Strambi2010), indicating that self-report is a valid measure of sleep duration.

Statistical Analyses

In the following section, the statistical methods are briefly described. A more elaborate description is presented in the online supplement. (To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.74) Resemblance between twins and non-twin siblings is used to decompose individual differences in sleep duration as due to additive genetic (A), shared or common environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) factors (ACE model). Monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs are genetically identical, whereas dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs and twin-sibling pairs share on average 50% of their segregating genes. When the MZ resemblance (expressed as a correlation) is higher than the resemblance in DZ twin or twin-sibling pairs, genetic variation is likely to influence individual differences in sleep duration. When MZ twin pairs resemble each other more than DZ twin and twin-sibling pairs, but not to the extent that would be expected based on their twice larger genetic resemblance, this implies that shared environmental factors influence variation in sleep duration. Other variation is attributed to non-shared environment. This component also includes measurement error variance.

As the data on sleep duration were ordinal, a liability threshold model was used. The basic assumption underlying the liability threshold model is that many genetic and environmental factors contribute to the liability of a trait (Falconer & Mackay, Reference Falconer and Mackay1996). The latent liability continuum is assumed to be a standard normal distribution (SD = 1). To model the three categories of sleep duration, two thresholds were specified that divide the sample into three categories. Twin and twin-sibling correlations were estimated on the liability scale for each of the five sex by zygosity groups (i.e., monozygotic male: MZM; dizygotic male: DZM; monozygotic female: MZF; dizygotic female: DZF; and dizygotic opposite-sex: DOS). In order to assess sex differences in prevalence of sleep duration and whether the prevalence of sleep duration changed with age, thresholds were estimated conditionally on sex and age before genetic modeling was started.

Next, genetic structural equation modeling in the software package Mx (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2006) was carried out to estimate the contribution of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors to individual differences in liability (Figure 1). A moderator model as described by Purcell (Reference Purcell2002) was fitted to the data in which age (z-score) was regressed on the genetic (a), shared environmental (c), and non-shared environmental (e) path coefficients (for details see supplementary material online). In Figure 1, regression weights of age are represented by the α, γ, and η coefficients. As described by Medland et al. (Reference Medland, Neale, Eaves and Neale2009), in the moderator model thresholds were constrained at fixed values (i.e., 0 and 1) to enable the estimation of the variance of the latent liability distribution of sleep duration, and hence the absolute contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the variance. To assess sex differences in the genetic architecture of sleep duration, the regression coefficients of age as well as the path coefficients, a, c, and e, were allowed to differ for boys and girls, and it was tested whether constraining these parameter estimates to be equal for boys and girls reduced model fit significantly. The significance of the age effects was also assessed by testing whether constraining the regression weights of age at zero resulted in a significant deterioration of model fit. Finally, to assess whether genetic and shared environmental factors contributed significantly to individual differences in liability, we tested whether constraining the parameter estimates for A and C (i.e., a and c) at zero resulted in a significant deterioration of model fit.

FIGURE 1 Univariate liability threshold model for sleep duration. Note: Sleep duration was measured with three categories. Thresholds were constrained at fixed values (i.e., 0 and 1) to enable the estimation of absolute variance of liability distribution. Total variance in sleep duration is modeled as caused by latent factors A (additive genetic influences), C (shared environmental influences), and E (non-shared environmental influences). Under this model, a, c, and e represent the unmoderated path coefficients, and α-, γ-, and η-coefficients represent the moderating effects of age. If, for example, α is significantly different from zero, the magnitude of A changes as a linear function of age. Path coefficients a, c, and e, as well as α-, γ-, and η-coefficients, were allowed to differ for boys and girls. Genetic correlation (rg): MZ twin pairs = 1, DZ twin pairs and twin-sibling pairs = 0.5; shared environmental correlation (rc) = 1.

Submodels were nested in the sense that one model could be derived from the other by the imposition of one or more constraints on the parameters. The fit of submodels was compared by means of the log-likelihood ratio test (LRT). If a p value higher than .05 was obtained from the resulting χ2-test statistic, the fit of the constrained model was not significantly worse than the fit of the more complex model. The fit of the genetic models was also compared with the full ACE model by means of Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC), keeping the model with the lowest AIC as the best fitting model (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2006).

Results

Figure 2 presents the prevalence of sleep duration as a function of age and sex. For boys and girls, short sleep duration (<8 hours) was more prevalent among older adolescents, whereas long (>9 hours) and average sleep durations (8–9 hours) were more prevalent among younger adolescents. These age differences in the prevalence of sleep duration were larger for boys than girls. Formal tests on the thresholds showed these age effects on the prevalence of sleep duration and sex differences in the magnitude of these age effects to be significant. The magnitude of age effects on the first, χ2(1) = 25.02, p < .01, and the second threshold, χ2(1) = 23.29, p < .01, was significantly different for boys and girls. Age effects on the thresholds were significant for boys: first threshold: χ2(1) = 259.70, p < .01, standardized regression coefficient β = 0.45; second threshold: χ2(1) = 154.18, p < .01, standardized regression coefficient β = 0.48; and for girls: first threshold: χ2(1) = 117.81, p < .01, standardized regression coefficient β = 0.25; second threshold: χ2(1) = 58.15, p < .01, standardized regression coefficient β = 0.22.

FIGURE 2 Changes in the prevalence of sleep duration as a function of sex and age.

Twin and twin-sibling correlations for sleep duration, uncorrected for age, are presented in Table 2. DZ twin correlations were not significantly different from twin-sibling correlations, χ2(3) = 5.70, p = .13. The MZ twin correlations were significantly higher than the DZ twin or twin-sibling correlations, boys: χ2(1) = 24.94, p < .01, girls: χ2(1) = 39.82, p < .01. This suggests that individual differences in sleep duration are influenced by genetic factors.

TABLE 2 Polychoric Twin and Twin–Sibling Correlations, Uncorrected for Age*, and Their 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) for Sleep Duration

Brother = male twin-brother pairs; Sister = female twin-sister pairs; Brother–sister = male twin-sister pairs, female twin-brother pairs.

*Results in twin correlations for the mean age of the sample (=15.9).

The genetic model fitting revealed that there were no sex differences in the magnitude of age effect on the genetic, (α) p = .80, and shared environmental, (γ) p = .67, path coefficients, whereas the magnitude of the age effect on the non-shared environmental (η) path coefficient was significantly different between boys and girls, p ≤ .05. The age effects on the shared environmental (γ) and non-shared environmental path coefficient for girls (ηgirls) were statistically significant, p < .05. This indicates that the contribution of shared (for boys and girls) and non-shared environmental factors (for girls only) to variation in sleep duration changed as a function of age. The age effects on the genetic (α) and non-shared environmental path coefficient for boys (ηboys) were not statistically significant. The absolute and relative changes in contribution of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors to variation in sleep duration as a function of age and sex can be seen in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3 Changes in the absolute and relative contributions of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental effects to variation in sleep duration as a function of age for boys and girls.

Next we tested whether constraining the genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental parameter estimates to be equal for boys and girls led to a significant deterioration of model fit. There appeared to be significant sex differences in the magnitude of the shared environmental path coefficient, while the magnitude of the genetic and non-shared environmental path coefficients was similar for boys and girls. Shared environmental effects on individual differences in sleep duration were statistically significant for girls but not for boys. Note, however, that the age effect on shared environment is significant for both boys and girls, resulting in the presence of shared environmental influences at the beginning of adolescence for both boys and girls (see Figure 3). Additive genetic effects on variation in sleep duration were statistically significant. Statistical analyses (LRT tests and the AIC, see supplementary material online) pointed to a model in which variance in sleep duration was explained by genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors with significant age effects on the shared environmental path coefficients for boys and girls and the non-shared environmental path coefficient for girls as the most parsimonious model. By constraining the thresholds to fixed values, the variance of the latent liability distribution of sleep duration could be estimated and, hence the absolute magnitude of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental effect on the variance of sleep duration (Medland et al., Reference Medland, Neale, Eaves and Neale2009).

Figure 3 presents the unstandardized (presented on the left side) and standardized (presented on the right side) contribution of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors to variation in sleep duration as a function of age and sex, derived from the most parsimonious model. For boys and girls, shared environmental factors contributed to variation in sleep duration among younger, but not among older adolescents. For boys, the absolute contribution of genetic and non-shared environmental factors to variation in sleep duration was similar for all ages, whereas the relative contribution of genetic and non-shared environmental effects was smaller in younger compared with older participants. This is due to shared environmental influences on variation in sleep duration for younger boys and the absence of these influences among older boys, leading to an overall lower variation in sleep duration among older boys. For girls, the absolute and relative contribution of non-shared environmental effects to variation in sleep duration was larger in older than younger participants. The absolute contribution of genetic effects to variation in sleep duration was similar at all ages, whereas the relative contribution of genetic effects was larger in younger compared with older girls. This is due to the fact that the contribution of shared and non-shared environmental factors to variation in sleep duration was different for younger and older girls, leading to differences in absolute variation in sleep duration throughout adolescence.

Discussion

In a large sample of Dutch adolescent twins and their siblings, shorter than optimum sleep duration, that is, less than 8 hours per night, was more prevalent among older compared to younger adolescents. This is in line with previous studies that reported sleep duration became shorter during adolescence (Fallone et al., Reference Fallone, Owens and Deane2002; Iglowstein et al., Reference Iglowstein, Jenni, Molinari and Largo2003). Variation in sleep duration was accounted for by genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors. In addition, the contribution of shared and non-shared environmental influences to individual differences in sleep duration differed by age. Specifically, shared environmental effects on sleep duration were important during early adolescence, but were of no importance during middle and late adolescence. For girls, non-shared environmental effects on sleep duration were larger among older than younger adolescents. For boys and girls, the absolute contribution of genetic factors to individual differences in sleep duration was similar at all ages, as was the case for non-shared environmental factors for boys.

The present study showed individual differences in adolescent sleep duration to be accounted for 35–45% by genetic factors. The absolute influence of genetic factors on individual differences in sleep duration was similar for all ages. Genetic factors on sleep duration may influence biological processes such as the biological circadian timing system, which, in turn, affects sleep duration. Changes in the relative phase of circadian rhythm, a process occurring during adolescence, cause adolescents to feel sleepy later in the evening (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo2002; Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008). A number of genetic variants, such as the Per3, CLOCK, TRIB1, Cry1, and Cry2 genes, have been associated with circadian rhythmicity, slow wave sleep, and abnormalities in the circadian timing system (Cirelli, Reference Cirelli2009; Dauviltiers et al., Reference Dauviltiers, Maret and Tafti2005; Ollila et al., Reference Ollila, Utge, Kronholm, Aho, Van, Silander and Paunio2012; Tafti, Reference Tafti2009; Wulff et al., Reference Wulff, Porcheret, Cussans and Foster2009) and some have also been linked to depression and lipid metabolism (Ollila et al., Reference Ollila, Utge, Kronholm, Aho, Van, Silander and Paunio2012; Utge et al., Reference Utge, Soronen, Loukola, Kronholm, Ollila, Pirkola and Paunio2010).

Due to a genetically defined ‘preference’ for late bedtimes (Carskadon & Acebo, Reference Carskadon and Acebo2002; Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008), increasing adolescents’ sleep duration by advancing bed times may not be feasible for many middle and late adolescents. Instead, delaying the onset of school days may have better perspective for these adolescents (Kirby et al., Reference Kirby, Maggi and D'Angiulli2011). Previous studies have reported that when high school start times are delayed, adolescents’ bedtimes do not change and, consequently, sleep duration increases (Owens et al., Reference Owens, Belon and Moss2010; Wahlstrom, Reference Wahlstrom2002; Wolfson et al., Reference Wolfson, Spaulding, Dandrow and Baroni2007). It must be noted that lack of sleep is not suffered by the entire adolescent population because of a preference for late bedtimes. It has been reported that the prevalence of suboptimal sleep is significantly smaller among adolescents with a preference for early bed and rising times than those with a preference for late bedtimes (Giannotti et al., Reference Giannotti, Cortesi, Sebastiani and Ottaviano2002).

For early adolescents, shared environmental factors accounted for a major part of individual differences in sleep duration, whereas the contribution of shared environmental was absent among middle and late adolescents. Shared environmental factors may include the influence of parents on early adolescents’ sleep duration by setting rules about bed and wakening times. It has been reported that parents set fewer rules about bedtimes throughout adolescence (Carskadon, Reference Carskadon1990, Reference Carskadon and Acebo2002), which is in line with the finding of the present study that the influence of shared environmental factors on sleep duration is larger for early adolescents compared with middle and late adolescents. The diminishing contribution of shared environmental factors during early adolescence is in line with other phenotypes that have been assessed among adolescents, such as exercise behavior (Van der Aa et al., Reference Van der Aa, de Geus, van Beijsterveldt, Boomsma and Bartels2010), sedentary behavior (Van der Aa et al., Reference Van der Aa, Bartels, te Velde, Boomsma, de Geus and Brug2012), and psychopathology (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, Van der Aa, van Beijsterveldt, Middeldorp and Boomsma2011; Lamb et al., Reference Lamb, Middeldorp, van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Van der Aa, Polderman and Boomsma2010).

For early adolescent girls, non-shared environmental factors accounted for a modest part of variation in sleep duration, whereas non-shared environmental factors accounted for a major part of individual differences in sleep duration among middle and late adolescent girls. For boys, non-shared environmental factors constituted the main source of variation in sleep duration throughout adolescence and the contribution of these factors was similar for all ages. Factors from adolescents’ personal environment that have previously been associated with individual differences in bedtime and hence sleep duration include participation in sports late in the evening, after-school employment, homework, engagement in personal computer and internet activities, playing video games, and increasing amount of time in the evening spent in social interaction with peers (Moore & Meltzer, Reference Moore and Meltzer2008).

Additional analyses in our own data indicated that sleep duration is associated with time spent in playing video games, engagement in personal computer and internet activities, social interaction with peers, and after-school employment. Specifically, the prevalence of suboptimal sleep duration (i.e., less than 8 hours per night) was approximately 10% higher among adolescents who reported to spend 3 hours or more on playing video games and engagement in personal computer and internet activities compared with those spending less time on these activities. Among adolescents engaged in after-school employment, prevalence of suboptimal sleep duration was about 10% higher than among those reporting no after-school employment. Prevalence of short sleep duration was also approximately 10% higher for adolescents spending time in social interaction with peers on a daily basis versus those reporting to be less frequently engaged in social interaction with peers. This indicates that changes in the social and economic environments that emerge during adolescence result in suboptimal sleep duration for some adolescents, whereas for others these changes do not affect their sleep duration. No indication was found, however, that sleep duration is associated with educational level, watching television, and participation in exercise. Prevalence of short, normal, and long sleep duration was similar for participants enrolled in lower, moderate, and higher secondary education, as well as for vigorous, moderate, and non-exercisers and those reporting to spend 3 hours or more and less than 3 hours on watching television. Although schooldays are longer for adolescents enrolled in higher levels of secondary education and they have more homework than those enrolled in lower levels of secondary education, no indication was found that this affects sleep duration. Exercise activities and TV shows aimed at adolescents are taking place later in the evening compared with those aimed at younger children, but no evidence was found that this negatively affects adolescent sleep duration.

An explanation for why the influence of non-shared environmental factors on individual differences in sleep duration is larger for boys than girls during early adolescence is that young adolescent boys already spend more time in activities that shorten their sleep duration by delayed bedtimes than girls. For example, adolescent boys are more frequently engaged than girls in sedentary activities, including engagement in personal computer and Internet activities and playing video games (Van der Aa et al., Reference Van der Aa, Bartels, te Velde, Boomsma, de Geus and Brug2012). An alternative explanation is that girls are more obedient toward parental control over sleep duration than boys. Spilsbury et al. (Reference Spilsbury, Storfer-Isser, Drotar, Rosen, Kirchner and Redline2005) reported that a parenting style encouraging social maturity was associated with healthy sleep patterns among girls but not boys. Parental control over sleep duration may also be larger for early adolescent girls compared with boys, such that parental control over sleep duration may prevent early adolescent girls engaging in the activities associated with shortened sleep duration late in the evening. This might explain the modest influence of non-shared environmental factors on individual differences in sleep duration among young adolescent girls. Parents are more likely to set clear expectations for behavior to daughters than to sons (Radziszewska et al., Reference Radziszewska, Richardson, Dent and Flay1996).

To our knowledge, the genetic architecture of adolescent sleep duration has not been addressed before, although some studies have investigated this among younger children (Brescianini et al., Reference Brescianini, Volzone, Fagnani, Patriarca, Grimaldi, Lanni and Stazi2011; Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, Rijsdijk and Eley2006) or addressed sleep problems among youth (Barclay & Gregory, Reference Barclay and Gregory2013; Gehrman et al., Reference Gehrman, Meltzer, Moore, Pack, Perlis, Eaves and Silberg2011; Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, Rijsdijk and Eley2006). However, a number of studies assessed the relative influence of genetic and environmental factors on sleep duration among adults (de Castro, Reference de Castro2002; Heath et al., Reference Heath, Kendler, Eaves and Martin1990; Hublin et al., Reference Hublin, Partinen, Koskenvuo and Kaprio2013; Partinen et al., Reference Partinen, Kaprio, Koskenvuo, Putkonen and Langinvainio1983; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Buchwald, Vitiello, Noonan and Goldberg2010). These studies showed that genetic factors accounted for 30–40% of individual differences in adult sleep duration, which is consistent with the findings of our study. The remaining part of variation in adult sleep duration is accounted for by non-shared environmental factors, which is also in line with our findings among middle and late adolescents.

A limitation to the present study was the use of a cross-sectional twin-family design to assess whether the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to individual differences in sleep duration is subject to change during the different stages of adolescence. This would be most properly addressed in a longitudinal design. So far, data at two time points are only available for a small sub-sample, and longitudinal data throughout adolescence (12–20 years of age) are absent. As our data collection is a continuous process at the NTR, large enough longitudinal sample sizes are anticipated in the next 5 years.

Conclusions

Our data showed that individual differences in adolescent sleep duration were accounted for by genetic and non-shared environmental factors, whereas shared environmental factors accounted for a substantial part of variation among early adolescents. For girls, the contribution of non-shared environmental factors to individual differences in sleep duration was larger among older adolescents compared with younger adolescents, while for boys, non-shared environmental factors constituted the main source of individual differences in sleep duration throughout adolescence and the contribution of these factors was similar at all ages.

Acknowledgments

Funding was obtained from the following grants: ‘Genes and environment in obesogenic behaviour: Exploring the relative importance of environmental and genetic factors on sedentary behaviour and sleep duration during adolescence — a study in adolescent twins’ (WCRF 2009/08); ‘Twin-family database for behavior genetics and genomics studies’ (NWO 480-04-004); ‘Spinozapremie’ (NWO/SPI 56-464-14192); ‘Genetic and family influences on adolescent psychopathology and wellness’ (NWO 463-06-001); ‘A twin-sibling study of adolescent wellness’ (NWO-VENI 451-04-034); ‘Determinants of adolescent exercise behavior; towards evidence-based intervention’ (National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases [NIDDK], RO1DK092127).

References

Altenburg, T. M., Chinapaw, M. J., van der Knaap, E. T., Brug, J., Manios, Y., & Singh, A. S. (2013). Longer sleep — Slimmer kids: The ENERGY-project. PLoS One, 8, e59522.Google Scholar
Barclay, N. L., & Gregory, A. M. (2013). Quantitative genetic research on sleep: A review of normal sleep, sleep disturbances and associated emotional, behavioural, and health-related difficulties. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 17, 2940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Derks, E. M., Stroet, T. M., Polderman, T. J. C., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A longitudinal multiple informant study of problem behavior. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 311.Google Scholar
Bartels, M., Van der Aa, N., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Middeldorp, C. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2011). Adolescent self-report of emotional and behavioral problems: Interactions of genetic factors with sex and age. Journal of the Canadian Acadademy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 20, 3552.Google Scholar
Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J. C., Vink, J. M., Stubbe, J. H., Distel, M. A., Hottenga, J. J., . . . Willemsen, G. (2006). Netherlands twin register: From twins to twin families. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 849857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brescianini, S., Volzone, A., Fagnani, C., Patriarca, V., Grimaldi, V., Lanni, R., . . . Stazi, M. A. (2011). Genetic and environmental factors shape infant sleep patterns: A study of 18-month-old twins. Pediatrics, 127, 12961302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carskadon, M. A. (1990). Patterns of sleep and sleepiness in adolescents. Pediatrician, 17, 512.Google ScholarPubMed
Carskadon, M. A. (2002). Factors influencing sleep patterns of adolescents. In Carskadon, M. A. (Ed.), Adolescent sleep patterns: Biological, social, and psychological influences (pp. 426). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carskadon, M. A., & Acebo, C. (2002). Regulation of sleepiness in adolescents: Update, insights, and speculation. Sleep, 25, 606614.Google Scholar
Carskadon, M. A., Harvey, K., Duke, P., Anders, T. F., Litt, I. F., & Dement, W. C. (1980). Pubertal changes in daytime sleepiness. Sleep, 2, 453460.Google Scholar
Cirelli, C. (2009). The genetic and molecular regulation of sleep: From fruit flies to humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 549560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dauviltiers, Y., Maret, S., & Tafti, M. (2005). Genetics of normal and pathological sleep in humans. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 9, 91100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Castro, J. M. (2002). The influence of heredity on self-reported sleep patterns in free-living humans. Physiology and Behavior, 76, 479486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falconer, D. S., & Mackay, T. F. C. (1996). Introduction to quantitative gentics. Essex, UK: Pearson.Google Scholar
Fallone, G., Owens, J. A., & Deane, J. (2002). Sleepiness in children and adolescents: Clinical implications. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 6, 287306.Google Scholar
Gehrman, P. R., Meltzer, L. J., Moore, M., Pack, A. I., Perlis, M. L., Eaves, L. J., & Silberg, J. L. (2011). Heritability of insomnia symptoms in youth and their relationship to depression and anxiety. Sleep, 34, 16411646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giannotti, F., Cortesi, F., Sebastiani, T., & Ottaviano, S. (2002). Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence. Journal of Sleep Research, 11, 191199.Google Scholar
Gregory, A. M., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Eley, T. C. (2006). A twin-study of sleep difficulties in school-aged children. Child Development, 77, 16681679.Google Scholar
Guidolin, M., & Gradisar, M. (2012). Is shortened sleep duration a risk factor for overweight and obesity during adolescence? A review of the empirical literature. Sleep Medicine, 13, 779786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, A. C., Kendler, K. S., Eaves, L. J., & Martin, N. G. (1990). Evidence for genetic influences on sleep disturbance and sleep pattern in twins. Sleep, 13, 318335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoch, C. C., Reynolds, C. F., Kupfer, D. J., Berman, S. R., Houck, P. R., & Stack, J. A. (1987). Empirical note — Self-report versus recorded sleep in healthy seniors. Psychophysiology, 24, 293299.Google Scholar
Hublin, C., Partinen, M., Koskenvuo, M., & Kaprio, J. (2013). Genetic factors in evolution of sleep length — A longitudinal twin study in Finnish adults. Journal of Sleep Research, 22, 513518.Google Scholar
Hysing, M., Pallesen, S., Stormark, K. M., Lundervold, A. J., & Sivertsen, B. (2013). Sleep patterns and insomnia among adolescents: A population-based study. Journal of Sleep Research, 22, 549556.Google Scholar
Iglowstein, I., Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., & Largo, R. H. (2003). Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics, 111, 302307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, M., Maggi, S., & D'Angiulli, A. (2011). School start times and the sleep-wake cycle of adolescents: A review and critical evaluation of available evidence. Educational Researcher, 40, 5661.Google Scholar
Lamb, D. J., Middeldorp, C. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Bartels, M., Van der Aa, N., Polderman, T. J. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2010). Heritability of anxious-depressive and withdrawn behavior: Age-related changes during adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 248255.Google ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., Zhang, A., & Li, L. (2012). Sleep duration and overweight/obesity in children: Review and implications for pediatric nursing. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing, 17, 193204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manconi, M., Ferri, R., Sagrada, C., Punjabi, N. M., Tettamanzi, E., Zucconi, M., . . . Ferini-Strambi, L. (2010). Measuring the error in sleep estimation in normal subjects and in patients with insomnia. Journal of Sleep Research, 19, 478486.Google Scholar
Matricciani, L., Olds, T., & Petkov, J. (2012). In search of lost sleep: Secular trends in the sleep time of school-aged children and adolescents. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16, 203211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medland, S. E., Neale, M. C., Eaves, L. J., & Neale, B. M. (2009). A note on the parameterization of Purcell's G × E model for ordinal and binary data. Behavior Genetics, 39, 220229.Google Scholar
Mindell, J. A., Owens, J. A., & Carskadon, M. A. (1999). Developmental features of sleep. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 8, 695725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, M., & Meltzer, L. J. (2008). The sleepy adolescent: Causes and consequences of sleepiness in teens. Paediatric Respiratory Reviews, 9, 114121.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. M. (2006). Mx: Statistical modeling (7th ed.). Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Ollila, H. M., Utge, S., Kronholm, E., Aho, V., Van, L. W., Silander, K., . . . Paunio, T. (2012). TRIB1 constitutes a molecular link between regulation of sleep and lipid metabolism in humans. Translational Psychiatry, 2, e97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, J. A., Belon, K., & Moss, P. (2010). Impact of delaying school start time on adolescent sleep, mood, and behavior. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 164, 608614.Google Scholar
Partinen, M., Kaprio, J., Koskenvuo, M., Putkonen, P., & Langinvainio, H. (1983). Genetic and environmental determination of human sleep. Sleep, 6, 179185.Google Scholar
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 947957.Google Scholar
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radziszewska, B., Richardson, J. L., Dent, C. W., & Flay, B. R. (1996). Parenting style and adolescent depressive symptoms, smoking, and academic achievement: Ethnic, gender, and SES differences. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 289305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietveld, M. J., van Der Valk, J. C., Bongers, I. L., Stroet, T. M., Slagboom, P. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2000). Zygosity diagnosis in young twins by parental report. Twin Research, 3, 134141.Google Scholar
Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., & Van Cauter, E. (1999). Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet, 354, 14351439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spilsbury, J. C., Storfer-Isser, A., Drotar, D., Rosen, C. L., Kirchner, H. L., & Redline, S. (2005). Effects of the home environment on school-aged children's sleep. Sleep, 28, 14191427.Google Scholar
Spruyt, K., Molfese, D. L., & Gozal, D. (2011). Sleep duration, sleep regularity, body weight, and metabolic homeostasis in school-aged children. Pediatrics, 127, e345–e352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tafti, M. (2009). Genetic aspects of normal and disturbed sleep. Sleep Medicine, 10, S17–S21.Google Scholar
Taheri, S., Lin, L., Austin, D., Young, T., & Mignot, E. (2004). Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Medicine, 1, 210217.Google Scholar
Utge, S. J., Soronen, P., Loukola, A., Kronholm, E., Ollila, H. M., Pirkola, S., . . . Paunio, T. (2010). Systematic analysis of circadian genes in a population-based sample reveals association of TIMELESS with depression and sleep disturbance. PloS One, 5, e9259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Groen-Blokhuis, M. M., Hottenga, J. J., Franić, S., Hudziak, J. J., Lamb, D., . . . Boomsma, D. I. (2013). The Young Netherlands Twin Register (YNTR): Longitudinal twin and family studies in over 70,000 children. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 252267.Google Scholar
Van Cauter, E., & Knutson, K. L. (2008). Sleep and the epidemic of obesity in children and adults. European Journal of Endocrinology, 159, S59–S66.Google Scholar
Van Cauter, E., Splegel, K., Tasali, E., & Leproult, R. (2008). Metabolic consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Medicine, 9, S23–S28.Google Scholar
Van der Aa, N., Bartels, M., te Velde, S. J., Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J., & Brug, J. (2012). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in sedentary behavior during adolescence: A twin-family study. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 166, 509514.Google Scholar
Van der Aa, N., de Geus, E. J. C., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M., Boomsma, D. I., & Bartels, M. (2010). Genetic influences on individual differences in exercise behavior during adolescence. International Journal of Pediatrics, 138345.Google Scholar
Wahlstrom, K. (2002). Changing times: Findings from the first longitudinal study of later high school start times. NASSP Bulletin, 86, 321.Google Scholar
Watson, N. F., Buchwald, D., Vitiello, M. V., Noonan, C., & Goldberg, J. (2010). A twin study of sleep duration and body mass index. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 6, 1117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfson, A. R., & Carskadon, M. A. (2003). Understanding adolescents’ sleep patterns and school performance: A critical appraisal. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 7, 491506.Google Scholar
Wolfson, A. R., Spaulding, N. L., Dandrow, C., & Baroni, E. M. (2007). Middle school start times: The importance of a good night's sleep for young adolescents. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 5, 194209.Google Scholar
Wulff, K., Porcheret, K., Cussans, E., & Foster, R. G. (2009). Sleep and circadian rhythm disturbances: Multiple genes and multiple phenotypes. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 19, 237246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Sample Constellation

Figure 1

FIGURE 1 Univariate liability threshold model for sleep duration. Note: Sleep duration was measured with three categories. Thresholds were constrained at fixed values (i.e., 0 and 1) to enable the estimation of absolute variance of liability distribution. Total variance in sleep duration is modeled as caused by latent factors A (additive genetic influences), C (shared environmental influences), and E (non-shared environmental influences). Under this model, a, c, and e represent the unmoderated path coefficients, and α-, γ-, and η-coefficients represent the moderating effects of age. If, for example, α is significantly different from zero, the magnitude of A changes as a linear function of age. Path coefficients a, c, and e, as well as α-, γ-, and η-coefficients, were allowed to differ for boys and girls. Genetic correlation (rg): MZ twin pairs = 1, DZ twin pairs and twin-sibling pairs = 0.5; shared environmental correlation (rc) = 1.

Figure 2

FIGURE 2 Changes in the prevalence of sleep duration as a function of sex and age.

Figure 3

TABLE 2 Polychoric Twin and Twin–Sibling Correlations, Uncorrected for Age*, and Their 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) for Sleep Duration

Figure 4

FIGURE 3 Changes in the absolute and relative contributions of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental effects to variation in sleep duration as a function of age for boys and girls.

Supplementary material: File

te Velde et al. Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Download te Velde et al. Supplementary Material(File)
File 39.9 KB