Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:07:39.439Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The common sense model in Raynaud’s phenomenon: do illness perceptions account for variance in symptom severity and quality of life?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2024

Jessica Broughton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Dulcie Irving
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Jo Daniels*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
*
Corresponding author: Jo Daniels; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

People with Raynaud’s phenomenon (RP) experience poorer mental health and quality of life than the general population, and there is limited evidence for treatment options in RP. The Common Sense Model of illness representations (CSM) is a well-established theoretical model, which has not yet been robustly investigated in RP, but may provide potential avenues for psychological interventions with the ability to explore perceptions and beliefs, such as cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). The study aims were to investigate illness perceptions and examine the relationship between illness perceptions and symptom severity and quality of life in RP to explore a theoretical basis for potential treatment avenues. A cross-sectional online questionnaire design was employed and 169 adults with RP (primary or secondary) were analysed. Illness perceptions significantly differed between primary and secondary RP types on all but one domain (p < .05). Hierarchical multiple regressions indicated that illness perception subscales made a significant unique contribution to the models explaining 65% variance in symptom severity (R2 = .65, p < .001) and 30% variance in quality of life (R2 = .30, p < .001). This novel study provides preliminary evidence regarding the applicability of the CSM to RP in a clinically meaningful way. CBT, which can specifically target illness perceptions within a wider psychological formulation, may be helpful for individuals with RP who are experiencing psychological distress in relation to symptom severity. Further work is needed to develop outcome measures specific to RP and tailor interventions to manage distress and impaired quality of life.

Key learning aims

  1. (1) The Common Sense Model is applicable and relevant to Raynaud’s phenomenon (RP) and there are important differences between illness perceptions in those with primary and secondary RP subtypes.

  2. (2) Findings show that illness-specific cognitions make a significant contribution to the variance in symptom severity and quality of life in those with both subtypes of RP, which has notable implications for the assessment, formulation and treatment of psychological difficulties in RP.

  3. (3) This offers a basis for further replication and development and adaptation of an intervention for this group, drawing on the evidence base for long-term conditions.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Raynaud’s phenomenon (RP) is characterised by episodic ‘attacks’ of vasoconstriction (restricted blood flow) which ranges from intrusive but ‘benign’, to severe ischaemia which threatens tissue viability (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Hughes and Pope2019a). During RP ‘attacks’ blood flow to extremities is reduced, causing skin discolouration, pain, numbness and paraesthesia (ICD-11, World Health Organization, 2018; Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Saketkoo, Matucci-Cerinic, Ingegnoli and Khanna2019b), which interferes with performance of everyday activities (Shapiro and Wigley, Reference Shapiro and Wigley2017) due to the hands being the most common extremities to be affected. Cold temperature reliably triggers RP episodes; however, a third of attacks are triggered by ‘emotional stress’, hypothesised as a result of an overactive autonomic nervous system which exaggerates vasoconstriction in RP (Freedman and Ianni, Reference Freedman and Ianni1983; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Snapir, Wilkinson, Snapir, Wigley and Herrick2015).

There are two subtypes of RP, described as ‘primary RP’ (pRP) and ‘secondary RP’ (sRP). pRP is independent of any other health condition, being present in 5% of the general population (Garner et al., Reference Garner, Kumari, Lanyon, Doherty and Zhang2015) and representing approximately 80–90% of RP cases (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2019; National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2020b). In sRP, Raynaud’s occurs alongside another condition, commonly autoimmune or connective tissue disorders such as systemic sclerosis (SSc), where 95% of patients have sRP (Merkel et al., Reference Merkel, Herlyn, Martin, Anderson, Mayes, Bell, Korn, Simms, Csuka, Medsger, Rothfield, Ellman, Collier, Weinstein, Furst, Jiminez, White, Seibold, Wigley and Clinical Trials Consortium2002). When compared with pRP, sRP is recognised as having more severe symptoms (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Domsic, Saketkoo, Almeida, Withey, Jay, Frech, Ingegnoli, Dures, Robson, McHugh, Herrick, Matucci-Cerinic, Khanna and Hewlett2018) with significant medical consequences (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Hughes and Pope2019a).

Individuals with RP report higher levels of anxiety and depression and poorer quality of life compared with the general population; those with sRP are more severely affected (Fabian et al., Reference Fabian, Fabian, Bugan and Csiki2019), with around 50% meeting criteria for an anxiety disorder (Sierakowska et al., Reference Sierakowska, Doroszkiewicz, Sierakowska, Olesińska, Grabowska-Jodkowska, Brzosko, Leszczynski, Pawlak-Bus, Batko, Wiland, Bykowska-sochacka, Romanowski, Zon-giebel and Jeka2019). Anxiety exacerbates RP symptoms, causing greater frequency and severity of attacks and higher levels of pain (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Middaugh, Haythornthwaite and Bielory2001). In conditions similar to RP, where anxiety contributes to distress and symptom exacerbation, (and indeed anxiety as a standalone condition) evidence favours cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). For example, in irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), which also has unpredictable episodic attacks, CBT successfully reduces symptoms and distress, and enhances quality of life (Windgassen et al., Reference Windgassen, Moss-Morris, Goldsmith and Chalder2019). A systematic review of the mechanisms of change within CBT for IBS found targeting illness-specific cognitions was key to a reduction in anxiety and subsequent improvement in symptom severity and adjustment (Windgassen et al., Reference Windgassen, Moss-Morris, Chilcot, Sibelli, Goldsmith and Chalder2017). Cognition and behavioural response to health and illness are a core component of the CBT model for any health condition, offering promising results for other related conditions such as chronic pain (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2021) and psychological difficulties often seen in physical health problems (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2020a; National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2022). There is an emerging evidence base for a biopsychosocial understanding of these conditions, and with it a more holistic and multi-disciplinary approach to treatment is required.

Treatment options for RP are limited, given that pharmacological interventions are often ineffective, and/or with unpleasant side-effects (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Snapir, Wilkinson, Snapir, Wigley and Herrick2015; Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Pauling and Eccleston2018) and there is currently insufficient evidence to support or refute behaviour change interventions (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Pauling and Eccleston2018), although prior research indicates cognitive affective factors are closely related to symptom severity and quality of life in RP (Irving and Daniels, Reference Irving and Daniels2024), indicating that psychological interventions such as CBT may be efficacious. Self-management strategies such as ‘keeping warm’ and ‘minimising emotional stress’ are currently recommended for those with RP, to try to manage the impact of symptoms (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2020b). In practice, this guidance is challenging to implement, making it difficult to reduce or minimise RP attacks and leading to the development of coping strategies, such as avoiding activities (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Domsic, Saketkoo, Almeida, Withey, Jay, Frech, Ingegnoli, Dures, Robson, McHugh, Herrick, Matucci-Cerinic, Khanna and Hewlett2018), which are detrimental to quality of life. It is clear more research is needed to identify further treatment avenues, with a focus on both improving quality of life and symptom management (Shapiro and Wigley, Reference Shapiro and Wigley2017), for those who would benefit from support.

The Common Sense Model of illness representation (CSM; Leventhal et al., Reference Leventhal, Meyer, Nerenz and Rachman1980) proposes that coping responses to illness are guided by inter-related beliefs called ‘illness perceptions’, which in turn impact upon medical, behavioural and psychological outcomes (Petrie et al., Reference Petrie, Jago and Devcich2007). Illness perceptions are influenced by life experiences, socio-cultural beliefs and knowledge, as well as implicit cognitive and emotional perceptions of illness (Hagger and Orbell, Reference Hagger and Orbell2003; Petrie and Weinman, Reference Petrie and Weinman2006). The CSM identifies five cognitive illness perception domains (Leventhal et al., Reference Leventhal, Meyer, Nerenz and Rachman1980), as outlined in Table 1.

Table 1. Illness perception domains within the CSM

Adapted from Broadbent et al. (Reference Broadbent, Petrie, Main and Weinman2006).

‘Negative’ illness perceptions (for example that illness will have severe consequences) are associated with increased disability, slower recovery and reduced quality of life – independently of medical severity (Petrie and Weinman, Reference Petrie and Weinman2006). This is replicated in chronic pain (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Vale, Sobral and Graca Pereira2016); chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (Zoeckler et al., Reference Zoeckler, Kenn, Keuhl, Stenzel and Rief2014); ME/chronic fatigue syndrome (Haines et al., Reference Haines, Loades and Davis2019); asthma (Kaptein et al., Reference Kaptein, Klok, Moss-Morris and Brand2010) diabetes (Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Donkin and Stroh2011) and SSc (Arat et al., Reference Arat, Verscheuren, De Langhe, Smith, Vanthuyne, Diya, Van den Heede, Blockmans, Houssai and Westhovens2012).

A recent meta-analysis across medical conditions found that illness perceptions account for between 25 and 30% of the variance in anxiety, depression and quality of life (Dempster et al., Reference Dempster, Howell and McCorry2015). Encouragingly, illness perceptions are amenable to change, for example in response to new information, and are therefore useful targets for psychological intervention (Petrie and Weinman, Reference Petrie and Weinman2006). The relationships between these variables are undoubtedly complex; however, there is an emerging evidence base for a biopsychosocial understanding of these conditions, and with it a more holistic and multi-disciplinary approach to treatment is required.

To date, no studies have applied the CSM to RP or assessed differences between the two subtypes. This is surprising given its prevalence and strong association with symptom severity, mental health and quality of life. This is a significant gap which, if addressed, offers a foundation for future interventions to successfully target illness cognitions to improve quality of life, as we have seen in many long-term health conditions that are amenable to a cognitive behavioural approach (White, Reference White2001). In the absence of evidence-based treatment options in RP (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Pauling and Eccleston2018), it is important to ascertain whether the CSM may provide a theoretical basis and useful intervention target for those struggling with the psychological impact of living with RP.

Based on previous literature, it was anticipated that illness perceptions would differ between adults with pRP and sRP and that they would be significantly predict quality of life and symptom severity in RP. More specifically:

  1. (1) Adults with sRP will have higher scores on ‘consequences’, ‘timeline’, ‘identity’, ‘concern’ and ‘emotional response’, and lower ‘personal control’ than those with pRP.

  2. (2) Adults with sRP will report greater ‘understanding’ of their condition, than those with pRP.

  3. (3) There will be no statistically significant difference in perceptions of ‘treatment control’ between groups.

  4. (4) ‘Consequences’, ‘identity’, ‘timeline’, ‘personal control’ and ‘concern’ will significantly predict quality of life and symptom severity in RP, and this relationship will remain when controlling for anxiety and depression.

Method

This study analysed primary data collected as part of a larger project exploring cognitive affective factors in RP (Irving and Daniels, Reference Irving and Daniels2024).

Design and procedure

A cross-sectional questionnaire design was used to collect data via an online survey. A Qualtrics link leading to an information sheet, consent and questionnaire measures was shared via social media. Following completion of the study, participants were offered debriefing information. Inclusion criteria were that respondents needed to be adults (aged 18+) with Raynaud’s (pRP or sRP).

Participants

Two hundred and sixty-nine participants completed the main survey between June and July 2020 which was advertised via the social media pages of the research team and two Raynaud’s charities (Scleroderma & Raynaud’s UK and Raynaud’s Association). Data from 57 participants were removed due to attrition, two were excluded due to age inclusion criteria (18+) and 14 did not complete measures relevant to this project. The final respondent sample was 196 (87 pRP; 95 sRP; 14 RP type missing).

Measures

Demographic questions included participants’ age, gender, ethnicity, education level, marital status, smoking status, RP diagnosis (pRP vs sRP) and illness duration. See Table 2 for details of included measures.

Table 2. Measures included within Qualtrics survey

Analysis

Analysis was conducted using SPSS version 26. Alpha levels were set at .05.

Analytic strategy

Screening

During comprehensive data screening (as outlined by Tabachnick and Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013), outcome measure data (BIPQ, DASS, BAS-G and ONS4) were found to largely violate assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance. Bootstrapping methods were therefore employed (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheets2002) to enable use of powerful parametric tests (Field, Reference Field2013) whilst retaining valuable information relating to non-normality of data (Pek et al., Reference Pek, Wong and Wong2018; Wright et al., Reference Wright, London and Field2011) and avoiding deletion of ‘true’ values (Bakker and Wicherts, Reference Bakker and Wicherts2014; Wilcox, Reference Wilcox2012).

As an exception, non-parametric tests (Spearman’s rho) were used to ensure robustness to non-normality during correlation analyses (as recommended by Bishara and Hittner, Reference Bishara and Hittner2017).

Outliers

Potential outliers were visually identified through inspection of box plots and statistically assessed using pre-determined Z-score cut-offs of Z > 3.29, p < .001 (Tabachnick and Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013). One univariate statistically significant outlier was identified on the DASS Anxiety Scale (Z = 3.36) which was a plausible, albeit high, value. However, review of Cook’s distance values (Pallant, Reference Pallant2016) indicated it was not influential during analyses.

Missing data

2.45% of values across the dataset were found to be missing completely at random (MCAR), as identified by Little’s test (c2 = 1521.763, d.f. = 1604, p = .929; Parent, Reference Parent2012). The majority, dispersed throughout the dataset, were replaced by series means, as is appropriate for a large sample with minimal missing data (Tabachnick and Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013).

The BIPQ, which had been the final measure within the online questionnaire battery, contained notably higher levels of missing data than other variables, which at 6.9% (> 5%) was ‘non-ignorable’ (Graham, Reference Graham2009). The amount of missing data varied across individual BIPQ subscales, ranging from 0.5% (Q2: ‘timeline’) to 11.2% (Q4: ‘treatment control’) but a series of two-tailed t-tests demonstrated this was not statistically significant (p > .05 in all instances). SPSS frequencies indicated that there were more instances of missing BIPQ data from primary RP respondents; however, this was also not statistically significant (p = .142, Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed). Visual inspection of missing values graphs and Little’s MCAR test (c2 = 174.788, d.f. = 161, p = .216) indicated BIPQ data were MCAR.

Non-normally distributed, missing BIPQ subscale values were managed using series mean replacement, as is widely used within psychology research (Cook, Reference Cook2021), thus, avoiding risk of introducing bias or error from ad hoc data transformation (Leys et al., Reference Leys, Delacre, Mora, Lakens and Ley2019). As we are aware that caution is advised when using series mean substitution (Cook, Reference Cook2021; Scheffer, Reference Scheffer2002), for example due to potential loss of variance and subsequent inflated risk of Type 1 errors (McKnight et al., Reference McKnight, McKnight, Sidani and Figueredo2007) a sensitivity analysis was completed, comparing results with original data only, as suggested by Thabane et al. (Reference Thabane, Mbuagbaw, Zhang, Samaan, Marcucci, Ye, Thabane, Giangregorio, Dennis, Kosa, Borg Debono, Dillenburg, Fruci, Bawor, Lee, Wells and Holdsmith2013).

Power analysis

A priori power analysis using G*Power (version 3.1; Faul et al., Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and Lang2009) indicated that the sample was sufficiently powered for analysis, as a total sample of 118 participants was required to detect a ‘medium’ effect size (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988) observed in prior related work (Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Wilkes, Koschwanez, Weinman, Norton and Petrie2015) using α = 0.05, 1–β0.8.

Statistical analysis

Tables 4 and 5 outline descriptive statistics and between-group differences that were evaluated on demographic and predictor variables. Categorical variables (gender, ethnicity, education, marital status, smoking) were analysed using chi-squared tests for independence, whilst independent samples t-tests were used for continuous variables (anxiety, depression, quality of life, symptom severity, BIPQ subscales 1–8). Fisher’s exact probability test statistics and likelihood ratios were reported in cases of violation of chi-squared assumptions (Field, Reference Field2013).

Table 3. Details of hierarchical multiple regression

Table 4. Demographic information by group

Table 5. Qualitative responses to BIPQ Q9.1

To enable quantitative analysis, qualitative responses to item 9 of the BIPQ were thematically coded into categorical data. As within prior research (Broadbent et al., Reference Broadbent, Wilkes, Koschwanez, Weinman, Norton and Petrie2015) only causal item 1 (top cause) were used for further analyses. Differences in causal attributions between groups were explored using chi-squared tests for independence. Bivariate correlations (Spearman’s rho) were calculated to assess the relationships between anxiety, depression, quality of life, symptom severity and the eight continuous BIPQ subscales.

Finally, two bootstrapped hierarchical multiple regressions (1000 resamples) were conducted to assess how much of the variance in (1) symptom severity and (2) quality of life across the whole sample, can be explained by BIPQ subscales. Assumptions for hierarchical multiple regression were met (Pallant, Reference Pallant2016; Tabachnick and Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013) and no instances of multi-collinearity were observed (Field, Reference Field2013).

As outlined in Table 3, IVs were added in three blocks, using the entry method with RP Type added at step 1 to control for differences between groups. Predictor variables anticipated to be potentially confounding (anxiety, depression, quality of life, symptom severity) were added as controls (second step), whilst BIPQ IVs were added in the final (third) step.

Results

Descriptive statistics

As outlined within Table 4, groups significantly differed on education level [c2(5) = 23.02, p = .000), marital status (likelihood ratio of c2(5) = 11.52, p = .042] and smoking history [likelihood ratio of c2(2) = 12.43, p = .002]. Groups did not significantly differ on gender or ethnicity. Between-group differences were statistically significant for the factors that participants stated they believe caused their condition (BIPQ, Q9), [c2(8) = 20.85, p = .008], as outlined in Table 5.

Between-group differences on illness perceptions and predictor variables

There were statistically significant differences between those with pRP and sRP on all but one illness perception domains, and so it was not felt meaningful to summarise patterns of illness perceptions across RP. Instead, between group patterns of illness perceptions are reported below.

As outlined in Table 6, participants with pRP reported significantly less severe ‘consequences’, t (172.58) = –5.59, p = .000,, a significantly shorter ‘timeline’, t (107.01) = –2.70, p = 0.2, significantly less ‘treatment control’, t (180) = –6.01, p = .000, and significantly lower levels of ‘understanding’, t (180) = –4.70, p = .000, than those with sRP. Participants with sRP reported significantly higher scores on ‘illness identity’, t (169.17) = –4.20, p = .000, significantly greater ‘emotional response’ t (174,61) = –4.15, p = .000, and significantly higher levels of ‘concern’, t (166.91) = –7.06, p = .000,. Groups did not significantly differ on ‘personal control’, t (180) = –1.51, p = .13.

Table 6. Between-groups comparison scores on BIPQ and predictor variables

*p < .05, **p < .001.

As anticipated, participants with pRP (M = 7.25, SD = 5.17) reported significantly lower levels of anxiety (M = 9.86, SD = 7.61), t (166.41) = –2.73, p = .006, and depression (M = 8.30, SD = 8.15) than those with sRP (M = 10.74, SD = 8.53), t (180) = –1.97, p = 0.05,. Those with pRP (M = 6.6, SD = 1.74) also reported significantly higher quality of life (M = 5.55, SD = 2.26), t (174.88) = 3.53, p = .000, and lower symptom severity (M = 4.35, SD = 2.31) compared with those with sRP (M = 5.94, SD = 2.18), t (180) = –4.78, p = .000.

Relationships between variables

Bivariate correlations (Table 7) demonstrated that across the whole sample, BIPQ ‘consequences’, ‘timeline’, ‘personal control’, ‘identity’, ‘concern’, ‘emotional response’, anxiety, depression and quality of life were significantly correlated with symptom severity in expected directions. BIPQ ‘consequences’, ‘personal control’, ‘identity’, ‘concern’, ‘emotional response’, anxiety, depression and symptom severity were significantly correlated with quality of life in expected directions. Minor between-group differences were noted when correlations were conducted for pRP and sRP separately. In pRP only, BIPQ ‘consequences’ and anxiety were both negatively correlated with quality of life, whilst ‘timeline’ was significantly positively correlated with symptom severity. In sRP only, depression was significantly positively correlated with symptom severity and ‘personal control’ was significantly positively correlated with quality of life. No instances of multi-collinearity were identified according to cut-offs of 0.8 as outlined by Field (Reference Field2013).

Table 7. Bivariate correlations (Spearman’s rho) for BIPQ and control variables

Significant at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < .001 (2-tailed).

Variance explained by illness perceptions

Both bootstrapped hierarchical multiple regressions (1000 resamples) met assumptions of linearity and homoscedasticity of residuals.

Symptom severity

At Step 1, RP type significantly explained 11% of the variance in symptom severity, R 2 = .11, F 1,180 = 22.89, p < .001. After entry at Step 2 anxiety, depression and quality of life explained an additional 14% of the variance in symptom severity, R 2 change = .14, F change (3,177) = 10.80, p < .001. At Step 3 the BIPQ subscales explained an additional 40% of the variance in symptom severity, R 2 change = .40, F change (6,171) = 32.25, p < .001. At Step 3, anxiety, β = .041, p < .05, 95% bootstrapped CI [.003, .079], BIPQ ‘consequences’, β = .439, p = .001, 95% bootstrapped CI [.260, .614] and BIPQ ‘concern’, β = .160, p < .05, 95% bootstrapped CI [.016, .316], made statistically significant unique contributions to the model. Total variance explained by the model as a whole was 65%, R 2 = .65, F 10,171 = 31.51, p < .001. See Table 8 for summary of results.

Table 8. Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analysis – symptom severity

*p < .05, **p < .001, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval (CI) based on 1000 resamples.

Quality of life

At Step 1, RP type explained 6% of the variance in quality of life, R 2 = .063, F 1,180 = 12.18, p = .001. At Step 2 anxiety, depression and symptom severity explained an additional 20% of the variance in quality of life, R 2 change = .20, F change (3,177) = 16.1, p < .001. At Step 3, R2 change was not statistically significant, p = .09.

However, at Step 3, RP type (β = –.861, p = .017, 95% bootstrapped CI [–1.56, –.14), depression (β = –.089, p = .001, 95% bootstrapped CI [–.132, –.046] and BIPQ ‘emotional response’ (β = –.162, p = .01, 95% bootstrapped CI [–.273, –.043] made statistically significant unique contributions to the model. Total variance explained by the model as a whole was 30%, R 2 = .30, F 9,172 = 8.32, p < .001. See Table 9 for summary of results.

Table 9. Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analysis – quality of life

Sensitivity analyses

Sensitivity analyses were conducted using only complete data (pairwise exclusion of missing BIPQ values) and yielded similar results to those outlined above. Final models explained 72% of variance in symptom severity, R 2 = .72, F 10,123 = 31.65, p < .001, and 33% of the variance in quality of life, R 2 = .33, F 9,124 = 6.68, p < .001. This demonstrates that series mean replacement of missing BIPQ values did not unduly influence results.

Discussion

This study addressed an important gap within the literature, being the first to apply the CSM to RP. Aiming to identify and compare illness perceptions across pRP and sRP, this study investigated the relationship between illness perceptions and symptom severity and quality of life in RP, to explore a theoretical basis for potential treatment avenues.

Characteristics of included participants were as expected, given prior research and known between group differences. Both pRP and sRP participants had higher levels of anxiety and depression than the general population (Henry and Crawford, Reference Henry and Crawford2005), with sRP respondents scoring within the ‘moderate’ range for depression and anxiety on average (Lovibond and Lovibond, Reference Lovibond and Lovibond1995). Quality of life scores were consistent with prior research (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Snapir, Wilkinson, Snapir, Wigley and Herrick2015) and as anticipated were lower than the general population for both groups (Office for National Statistics, 2020). Statistically significant between-group differences on anxiety, depression, quality of life and symptom severity echo findings from previous research (Fabian et al., Reference Fabian, Fabian, Bugan and Csiki2019, Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Snapir, Wilkinson, Snapir, Wigley and Herrick2015; Sierakowska et al., Reference Sierakowska, Doroszkiewicz, Sierakowska, Olesińska, Grabowska-Jodkowska, Brzosko, Leszczynski, Pawlak-Bus, Batko, Wiland, Bykowska-sochacka, Romanowski, Zon-giebel and Jeka2019).

A key finding was that illness perceptions significantly differ between pRP and sRP, and so it was not felt meaningful to summarise illness perceptions ‘across RP’ as had been a study aim. Instead, the between-groups pattern of illness perceptions is discussed, which partially supported hypotheses. Participants with sRP reported more severe ‘consequences’, longer ‘timeline’, higher illness ‘identity’, greater ‘emotional impact’ and greater ‘concern’ than those with pRP. SRP participants also reported greater ‘understanding’ and more ‘treatment control’ than those with pRP, reflecting differences between clinical features and patient experience of pRP and sRP (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Domsic, Saketkoo, Almeida, Withey, Jay, Frech, Ingegnoli, Dures, Robson, McHugh, Herrick, Matucci-Cerinic, Khanna and Hewlett2018; Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Hughes and Pope2019a). Contrary to hypotheses, there was no statistically significant difference between groups on ‘personal control’ which may reflect both the lack of evidence-based treatment interventions for RP (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Pauling and Eccleston2018) as well as the intrusive nature of RP attacks (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Domsic, Saketkoo, Almeida, Withey, Jay, Frech, Ingegnoli, Dures, Robson, McHugh, Herrick, Matucci-Cerinic, Khanna and Hewlett2018) and indicates that the ‘little personal control’ previously reported by SSc participants (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, Boonstra, Huizinga, Kaptein and de Vries-Bouwstra2020) may apply similarly to those with pRP.

As was anticipated, BIPQ subscales made a statistically significant contribution to overall models predicting both symptom severity and quality of life, when controlling for anxiety and depression. However, in contrast to prior research across medical conditions (Dempster et al., Reference Dempster, Howell and McCorry2015) and within SSc (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, Boonstra, Huizinga, Kaptein and de Vries-Bouwstra2020), ‘personal control’, ‘timeline’ and ‘identity’ did not make statistically significant contributions to final models. This may reflect variability in the participant sample for this project, as both pRP and sRP groups were pooled to ensure adequate statistical power for analysis. Future research would benefit from an appropriately powered between-groups evaluation of the relationship between illness perceptions and quality of life and symptom severity. The amount of variance in quality of life explained by the final model as a whole was consistent with a prior meta-analysis (Dempster et al., Reference Dempster, Howell and McCorry2015); however, the variance in symptom severity accounted for in the final model in this study was significantly greater than in prior research in carpal tunnel syndrome where BIPQ subscales added 13% variance in symptom severity (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Walbeehm, Selles, Jansen, Slijper, Ulrich and Porsius2019). It would be helpful for future research to further explore the reasons for this difference, for example to identify possible mediating/moderating variables which were beyond the scope of this research.

It is possible that the relationship between illness perceptions and symptom severity may vary between medical conditions, according to moderating or mediating factors. Given those with RP are known to experience significant levels of anxiety, which importantly triggers and exacerbates symptoms (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Middaugh, Haythornthwaite and Bielory2001), it is notable that the illness perceptions most predictive of symptom severity were ‘consequences’ and ‘concern’, both of which relate to threat appraisal, an important aspect of the well-established cognitive behavioural model of anxiety (Beck and Clark, Reference Beck and Clark1997). Future research is needed to explore the role of variables such as anxiety which potentially mediate the relationship between illness perceptions and symptom severity in RP; however, taken together with the CSM findings, this provides a strong basis from which to develop and adapt existing models of CBT for RP as an adjuvant therapy for this group.

Prior research in IBS has identified differences in the relationships between illness perceptions and quality of life depending on whether measures captured global or individual domains (de Gucht et al., Reference de Gucht2015). Although there is a lack of an agreed definition of health-related quality of life, several prominent conceptualisations are multi-dimensional, for example including ‘physical, psychological, social, spiritual, role functioning, and general well-being dimensions of health’ (Finlayson et al., Reference Finlayson, Moyer and Sonnad2004; p. 337). Illness-specific cognitions are likely to have differing relationships with different dimensions of quality of life, but may be more strongly related to symptom severity, which is a less varied construct. As single item measures such as the ONS4 used within this project capture a ‘global’ measure of quality of life, future research would benefit from exploration of these relationships using a multi-dimensional measure specific to health-related quality of life in RP.

Clinical implications

Overall, the findings of this study support the relevance of the CSM in RP, demonstrating that illness specific cognitions make a significant contribution to the variance in symptom severity and quality of life in those with pRP and sRP. In the absence of a robust evidence base for intervention in RP (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Pauling and Eccleston2018) and as illness perceptions have been demonstrated to be amenable (Petrie and Weinman, Reference Petrie and Weinman2006), this has notable implications for the assessment, formulation and treatment of psychological difficulties in RP. It is important that any psychological intervention takes into account the neurobiological components which underpin exaggerated physiological responses to anxiety in RP alongside treating illness specific perceptions which may serve to maintain the problem (Irving and Daniels, Reference Irving and Daniels2024; Moseley and Butler, Reference Moseley and Butler2015).

Results highlight the importance of particular illness perceptions to both symptom severity (‘consequences’, ‘concern’) and quality of life (‘emotional response’) in RP, which importantly differ from those found to be significant across medical conditions (Dempster et al., Reference Dempster, Howell and McCorry2015) and within SSc (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, Boonstra, Huizinga, Kaptein and de Vries-Bouwstra2020). This indicates the importance of considering illness perceptions holistically as part of a neuro-biopsychosocial assessment and formulation to enable appropriately tailored psychological interventions for those with RP.

Psychological interventions such as CBT which specifically target illness perceptions, within a comprehensive wider psychological formulation, may be helpful for individuals with RP who are experiencing psychological distress in relation to symptom severity, particularly the 50% of those with sRP who are known to also experience moderate-severe anxiety disorders (Sierakowska et al., Reference Sierakowska, Doroszkiewicz, Sierakowska, Olesińska, Grabowska-Jodkowska, Brzosko, Leszczynski, Pawlak-Bus, Batko, Wiland, Bykowska-sochacka, Romanowski, Zon-giebel and Jeka2019). CBT interventions targeting illness perceptions have been successfully used in IBS (Windgassen et al., Reference Windgassen, Moss-Morris, Chilcot, Sibelli, Goldsmith and Chalder2017), which shares some clinical features with RP such as episodic ‘attacks’ triggered by anxiety. Interventions for RP may sit well within the Improving Access to Psychological Therapies Long Term Conditions pathway as part of a multi-disciplinary approach (IAPT LTC; Panchal et al., Reference Panchal, Rich, Rowland, Ryan and Watts2020) which was developed further as part of the NHS Five Year Forward View for Mental Health (Mental Health Taskforce, 2016) to encompass long-term conditions, with the recent NHS Long Term Plan (2019) acknowledging further the reciprocal nature of physical and mental health. However, further work is needed.

Limitations and future research

This study was limited by online recruitment via Qualtrics. While a pragmatic approach to data collection, it obscured the possibility of being able to clinically verify or reliably confirm participants’ diagnoses. Future research would benefit from the recruitment of a clinically verified sample; however, the distinct and unique features of RP (white and purple fingers) indicate a good degree of confidence in the self-selected sample.

It is also important to note that the cross-sectional design of this study means that it is not possible to infer how relationships between the variables studied may differ over time. This is notable given that illness perceptions have been found to be amenable to change, evolving for example with lived experience of a health condition or access to new information (Petrie and Weinman, Reference Petrie and Weinman2006). Similarly, we know that symptom severity may also be changeable for those living with RP, for example sRP symptoms are known to increase in severity and new symptoms may also develop with increasing illness duration (Pauling et al., Reference Pauling, Hughes and Pope2019a). Given that the average illness duration of respondents in this study was notable (19.5 years pRP, 18.5 years sRP), future researchers may wish to further explore these relationships within a heterogenous group of individuals living with RP. The absence of RP specific outcome measures is a significant barrier in the field, also highlighted in prior research (Daniels et al., Reference Daniels, Pauling and Eccleston2018; Irving and Daniels, Reference Irving and Daniels2024). In the absence of measures specific to RP, symptom severity and quality of life were measured using generic single item questionnaires. Whilst single item measures are a pragmatic option and useful for preliminary and novel research (Waltz et al., Reference Waltz, Strickland and Lenz1991; Wanous et al., Reference Wanous, Reichers and Hudy1997), they cannot facilitate exploration of multiple dimensions of these constructs. Future research would benefit from building upon these initial findings with development and use of a validated measure of health-related quality of life specific to RP, and perhaps more importantly, building on this research to develop interventions to manage the physical and psychological distress and impaired quality of life for this neglected group.

Conclusion

This study provides preliminary evidence of the applicability of the CSM to RP, identifying different patterns of illness perceptions in individuals with pRP and sRP, as well as demonstrating that illness perceptions significantly contribute to variance in symptom severity and quality of life in RP. Findings have notable implications for the provision of psychological interventions such as CBT which, as part of a wider psychological formulation, target cognitions to improve mental health, symptom severity and quality of life. Future research would benefit from the development of a validated measure of health-related quality of life in RP, and further development which would underpin future interventions.

Key practice points

  1. (1) It is important to consider illness perceptions as part of a biopsychosocial assessment and formulation to enable appropriately tailored psychological interventions for those with RP.

  2. (2) Illness perceptions most predictive of symptom severity were ‘consequences’ and ‘concern’, both of which relate to threat appraisal, an important aspect of the well-established cognitive behavioural model of anxiety, which is known to trigger episodes.

  3. (3) Taken together with the support for the relevance of the CSM in RP, this is a strong basis from which to develop and adapt existing models of CBT for this group, which have been successfully used in other long-term conditions.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, J.D., upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all the people who generously participated in this study.

Author contributions

Jessica Broughton: Conceptualization (supporting), Formal analysis (lead), Methodology (equal), Project administration (equal), Writing – original draft (lead), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Dulcie Irving: Conceptualization (supporting), Data curation (lead), Investigation (lead), Methodology (equal), Resources (equal), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Jo Daniels: Conceptualization (lead), Methodology (supporting), Project administration (equal), Resources (equal), Supervision (lead), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (lead).

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. It was completed as part of the lead author’s Doctoral Thesis with the University of Bath.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Ethical standards

Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Bath Psychology Research Ethics Committee (ref. 20-114). The authors abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS. Informed consent was obtained from all participants to participate and publish for the purposes of research.

References

Further reading

Daniels, J., Pauling, J. D., & Eccleston, C. (2018). Behaviour change interventions for the management of Raynaud’s phenomenon: a systematic literature review. BMJ Open, 8, e024528. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024528 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irving, D., & Daniels, J. (2024). Psychological factors in symptom severity and quality of life in Raynaud’s phenomenon. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465823000620 Google ScholarPubMed
Petrie, K. J., & Weinman, J. (2006). Why illness perceptions matter. Clinical Medicine, 6, 536539. https://doi.org/10.7861/clinmedicine.6-6-536 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sage, N., Sowden, M., Chorlton, E., & Edeleanu, A. (2008). CBT for Chronic Illness and Palliative Care: A Workbook and Toolkit. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
White, C. A. (2001). Cognitive Behaviour Therapy for Chronic Medical Problems: A Guide to Assessment and Treatment in Practice. John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar

References

Arat, S., Verscheuren, P., De Langhe, E., Smith, V., Vanthuyne, M., Diya, L., Van den Heede, K., Blockmans, D., Houssai, F. A., & Westhovens, R. (2012). The association of illness perceptions with physical and mental health in systemic sclerosis patients: an exploratory study. Musculoskeletal Care, 10, 1828. https://doi.org/10.1002/msc.223 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakker, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2014). Outlier removal, sum scores and the inflation of the Type 1 error rate in independent samples t tests: the power of alternatives and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 19, 409427. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 4958. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00069-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishara, A. J., & Hittner, J. B. (2017). Confidence intervals for correlations when data are not normal. Behaviour Research Methods, 49, 294309. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0702-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, E., Donkin, L., & Stroh, J. C. (2011). Illness and treatment perceptions are associated with adherence to medications, diet and exercise in diabetic patients. Diabetes Care, 34, 338340. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc10-1779 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadbent, E., Petrie, K. J., Main, J., & Weinman, J. (2006). The Brief Illness Perceptions Questionnaire. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 631637. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.10.020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, E., Wilkes, C., Koschwanez, H., Weinman, J., Norton, S., & Petrie, K J. (2015). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire. Psychology & Health, 30, 13611385. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1070851 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, K. M., Middaugh, S. J., Haythornthwaite, J. A., & Bielory, L. (2001). The effects of stress, anxiety and outdoor temperature on the frequency and severity of Raynaud’s attacks: the Raynaud’s Treatment Study. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 24, 137153. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010758530555 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camfield, L., & Skevington, S. M. (2008). On subjective well-being and quality of life. Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 764775. https://doi/10.1177/1359105308093860 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clara, I. P., Cox, B. J., & Enns, M. W. (2001). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Depression-Anxiety-Stress Scales in depressed and anxious patients. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioural Assessment, 23, 6167. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011095624717 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd edn). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587 Google Scholar
Cook, R. M. (2021). Addressing missing data in quantitative counselling research. Counselling Outcome Research & Evaluation, 12, 4353. https://doi.org/10.1080/21501378.2019.1711037 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, E. C., Vale, S., Sobral, M., & Graca Pereira, M. (2016). Illness perceptions are the main predictors of depression and anxiety symptoms in patients with chronic pain. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 21, 483495. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2015.1109673 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, J., Pauling, J. D., & Eccleston, C. (2018). Behaviour change interventions for the management of Raynaud’s phenomenon: a systematic literature review. BMJ Open, 8, e024528. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024528 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Boer, A. G. E. M., van Lanschot, J. J. B., Stalmeier, P. F. M., van Sandick, J. W., Hulscher, J. B. F. de Haes, J. C. J. M., & Spranger, M. A. G. (2004). Is a single item visual analogue scale as valid, reliable and responsive as multi-item scales in measuring quality of life? Quality of Life Research, 13, 311320. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:QURE.0000018499.64574.1f CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Gucht, V. (2015). Illness perceptions mediate the relationship between bowel symptom severity and health-related quality of life in IBS patients. Quality of Life Research, 24, 18451856. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-015-0932-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dempster, M., Howell, D., & McCorry, M. K. (2015). Illness perceptions and coping in physical health conditions: a meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 6, 506513. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.10.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabian, B., Fabian, A. K., Bugan, A., & Csiki, Z. (2019). Comparison of mental and physical health between patients with primary and secondary Raynaud’s phenomenon. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 116, 69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.11.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3:1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behaviour Research Methods, 41, 11491160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, A. (2013). Discovering Statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics. SAGE.Google Scholar
Finlayson, T. L., Moyer, C. A., & Sonnad, S. S. (2004). Assessing symptoms, disease severity and quality of life in the clinical context: a theoretical framework. American Journal of Managed Care, 10, 336344.Google ScholarPubMed
Freedman, R. R., & Ianni, P. (1983). Role of cold and emotional stress in Raynaud’s disease and scleroderma. British Medical Journal, 287, 14991502. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.287.6404.1499 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garner, R., Kumari, R., Lanyon, P. Doherty, M., & Zhang, W. (2015). Prevalence, risk factors and associations of primary Raynaud’s phenomenon: systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. BMJ Open, 5. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006389 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gloster, A. T., Rhoades, H. M., Novy, D., Klotsche, J., Senior, A., Kunik, M., Wilson, N., & Stanley, M. A. (2008). Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 in older primary care patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 110, 248259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.01.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 549576. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagger, M. S., & Orbell, S. (2003). A meta-analytic review of the common-sense model of illness representations. Psychology and Health, 18, 141184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haines, C., Loades, M., & Davis, C. (2019). Illness perceptions in adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome and other physical health conditions: application of the common sense model. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 546563. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104519829796 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short form of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large nonclinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 227239. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X29657 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M., Snapir, A., Wilkinson, J., Snapir, D., Wigley, F. M., & Herrick, A. L. (2015). Prediction and impact of attacks of Raynaud’s phenomenon, as judged by patient perception. Rheumatology, 54, 14431447. https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/kev002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irving, D., & Daniels, J. (2024). Psychological factors in symptom severity and quality of life in Raynaud’s phenomenon. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465823000620 Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. D., Steiner, A., Garrett, S. L., & Calin, A. (1996). The Bath Ankylosing Spondylitis Patient Global Score (BAS-G). British Journal of Rheumatology, 35, 6671. https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/35.1.66 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaptein, A. A., Klok, T., Moss-Morris, R., & Brand, P. L. P. (2010). Illness perceptions: impact on self-management and control in asthma. Current Opinion in Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 10, 194199. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACI.0b013e32833950c1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The common sense representation of illness danger. In Rachman, S. (ed), Medical Psychology, vol. II, pp. 730. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Leys, C., Delacre, M., Mora, Y. L., Lakens, D., & Ley, C. (2019). How to classify, detect and manage univariate and multivariate outliers, with emphasis on pre-registration. International Review of Social Psychology, 32, 110. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.289 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the Depression and Anxiety Stress Scales (2nd edn). University of New South Wales.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83104. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.83 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madsen, O. R., Rytter, A., Hansen, L. B., Suetta, C., & Egsmose, C. (2010). Reproducibility of the Bath Ankylosing Spondylitis Indices of disease activity (BASDAI), functional status (BASFI) and overall well-being (BAS-G) in anti-tumour necrosis factor-treated spondyloarthropathy patients. Clinical Rheumatology, 29, 849854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKnight, P. E., McKnight, K. M., Sidani, S. & Figueredo, A. J. (2007). Missing Data: A Gentle Introduction. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mental Health Task Force (2016). The Five Year Forward View for Mental Health. Redditch: NHS England. Available at: https://www.england.nhs.uk/wpcontent/uploads/2016/02/Mental-Health-Taskforce-FYFV-final.pdf Google Scholar
Merkel, P. A., Herlyn, K., Martin, R. W., Anderson, J. J., Mayes, M. D., Bell, P., Korn, J. H., Simms, R. W., Csuka, M. E., Medsger, T. A., Rothfield, N. F., Ellman, M. H., Collier, D. H., Weinstein, A., Furst, D. E., Jiminez, S. A., White, B., Seibold, J. R., Wigley, F. M., & Clinical Trials Consortium, Scleroderma (2002). Measuring disease activity and functional status in patient with scleroderma and Raynaud’s phenomenon. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 46, 24102420. doi: 10.1002/art.10486 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moseley, G. L., & Butler, D. S. (2015). Fifteen years of explaining pain: the past, present, and future. Journal of Pain, 16, 807813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Horne, R., Cameron, L. D., & Buick, D. (2002). The Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R). Psychology & Health, 17, 116. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440290001494 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (2019). Raynaud’s phenomenon. https://cks.nice.org.uk/topics/raynauds-phenomenon/ Google Scholar
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (2020a). Generalised anxiety disorder and panic disorder in adults: management. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg113 Google Scholar
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (2020b). Raynaud’s phenomenon: how common is it? https://cks.nice.org.uk/topics/raynauds-phenomenon/background-information/prevalence-incidence Google Scholar
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (2021). Chronic pain (primary and secondary) in over 16s: assessment of all chronic pain and management of chronic primary pain. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng193/chapter/Recommendations Google Scholar
National Institute of Health and Care Excellence (2022). Depression in adults: treatment and management. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng222 Google Scholar
Office for National Statistics (2018, September 26). Personal well-being in the UK. Office for National Statistics. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/methodologies/personalwellbeingintheukqmi Google Scholar
Office for National Statistics (2020). Personal Wellbeing in the UK quarterly: April 2011 to September 2020. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/bulletins/personalwellbeingintheukquarterly/april2011toseptember2020 Google Scholar
Pallant, J. (2016). SPSS Survival Manual: A Step by Step Guide to Data Analysis Using SPSS Program (6th edn). London, UK: McGraw-Hill Education.Google Scholar
Panchal, R., Rich, B., Rowland, C., Ryan, T. & Watts, S. (2020). The successful impact of adapting CBT in IAPT for people with complex long-term physical health conditions. the Cognitive Behaviour Therapist, 13, 18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1754470X20000306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parent, M. C. (2012). Handling item-level missing data: simpler is just as good. The Counselling Psychologist, 41, 568600. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000012445176 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pauling, J. D., Domsic, R. T., Saketkoo, L. A., Almeida, C., Withey, J., Jay, H., Frech, T. M., Ingegnoli, F., Dures, E., Robson, J. McHugh, N. J., Herrick, A. L., Matucci-Cerinic, M., Khanna, D., & Hewlett, S. (2018). Multinational qualitative research study exploring the patient experience of Raynaud’s phenomenon in systemic sclerosis. Arthritis Care & Research, 70, 13731384. https://doi.org/10.1002/acr.23475 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pauling, J. D., Hughes, M., & Pope, J. E. (2019a). Raynaud’s phenomenon – an update on diagnosis, classification and management. Clinical Rheumatology, 38, 33173330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10067-019-04745-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pauling, J. D., Saketkoo, L. A., Matucci-Cerinic, M., Ingegnoli, F., & Khanna, D. (2019b). The patient experience of Raynaud’s phenomenon in systemic sclerosis. Rheumatology, 58, 826. https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/key026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pek, J., Wong, O., & Wong, A. C. M. (2018). How to address non-normality: a taxonomy of approaches, reviewed and illustrated. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 21042121. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrie, K. J., Jago, L. A., & Devcich, D. A. (2007). The role of illness perceptions in patients with medical conditions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 163167. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e328014a871 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrie, K. J., & Weinman, J. (2006). Why illness perceptions matter. Clinical Medicine, 6, 536539. https://doi.org/10.7861/clinmedicine.6-6-536 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffer, J. (2002). Dealing with missing data. Research Letters in the Information and Mathematical Sciences, 3, 153160. http://hdl.handle.net/10179/4355 Google Scholar
Shapiro, S. C., & Wigley, F. M. (2017). Treating Raynaud phenomenon: beyond staying warm. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 84, 797804. https://doi.org/10.3949/ccjm.84a.17025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sierakowska, M., Doroszkiewicz, H., Sierakowska, J., Olesińska, M., Grabowska-Jodkowska, A., Brzosko, M., Leszczynski, P., Pawlak-Bus, K., Batko, B., Wiland, P., Bykowska-sochacka, M., Romanowski, W., Zon-giebel, A., Jeka, S., & Ndosi., M. (2019). Factors associated with quality of life in systemic sclerosis: a cross-sectional study. Quality of Life Research, 28, 33473354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-019-02284-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, S. J., Siefert, C. J., Slavin-Mulford, J. M., Stein, M. B., Renna, M., & Blais, M. A. (2012). Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21) in a nonclinical sample of U.S. adults. Evaluation & The Health Professions, 35, 259279. https://doi.org/10.1177/0163278711424282 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, P. O., Walbeehm, E. T., Selles, R. W., Jansen, M. C., Slijper, H. P., Ulrich, D. J. O., & Porsius, J. T. (2019). Influence of illness perceptions, psychological distress and pain catastrophizing on self-reported symptom severity and functional status in patients with carpal tunnel syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 126, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2019.109820 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics. Harlow, UK: Pearson Education UK.Google Scholar
Thabane, L., Mbuagbaw, L., Zhang, S., Samaan, Z., Marcucci, M., Ye, C., Thabane, M., Giangregorio, L., Dennis, B., Kosa, D., Borg Debono, V., Dillenburg, R., Fruci, V., Bawor, M., Lee, J., Wells, G. & Holdsmith, C. H. (2013). A tutorial on sensitivity analyses in clinical trials: the what, why, when and how. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 13, 92104. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-13-92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, N. M., Boonstra, M., Huizinga, T. W. J., Kaptein, A. A., & de Vries-Bouwstra, J. K. (2020). Illness perceptions, risk perceptions and worries in patients with early systemic sclerosis: a focus group study. Musculoskeletal Care, 18, 177186. https://doi.org/10.1002/msc.1453 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waltz, C. F., Strickland, O. L., & Lenz, E. R. (1991). Measurement in Nursing Research (2nd edn). Philadelphia: F.A. Davis.Google Scholar
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: how good are single item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 247252. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-9010.82.2.247 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, C. A. (2001). Cognitive behavioral principles in managing chronic disease. Western Journal of Medicine, 175, 338342. https://doi.org/10.1136/ewjm.175.5.338 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, R. (2012). Modern Statistics for the Social and Behavioural Sciences: A Practical Introduction. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Windgassen, S., Moss-Morris, R., Chilcot, J., Sibelli, A., Goldsmith, K., & Chalder, T. (2017). The journey between brain and gut: a systematic review of psychological mechanisms of treatment effect in irritable bowel syndrome. British Journal of Health Psychology, 22, 701736. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12250 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windgassen, S., Moss-Morris, R., Goldsmith, K., & Chalder, T. (2019). Key mechanisms of cognitive behavioural therapy in irritable bowel syndrome: the importance of gastrointestinal related cognitions, behaviour and general anxiety. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 118, 7382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.11.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2018). International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics (11th revision).Google Scholar
Wright, D. B., London, K., & Field, A. P. (2011). Using bootstrap estimation and the plug-in principle for clinical psychology data. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2, 252270. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.013611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zochling, J. (2011). Measures of symptoms and disease status in ankylosing spondylitis: Ankylosing Spondylitis Disease Activity Score (ASDAS), Ankylosing Spondylitis Quality of Life Scale (ASQoL), Bath Ankylosing Spondylitis Disease Activity Index (BASDAI), Bath Ankylosing Spondylitis Functional Index (BASFI), Bath Ankylosing Spondylitis Global Score (BAS-G), Bath Ankylosing Spondylitis Metrology Index (BASMI), Dougados Functional Index (DFI), and Health Assessment Questionnaire for the Spondyloarthropathies (HAQ-S). Arthritis Care & Research, 63, 4758. https://doi.org/10.1002/acr.20575 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoeckler, N., Kenn, K., Keuhl, K., Stenzel, N., & Rief, W. (2014). Illness perceptions predict exercise capacity and psychological wellbeing after pulmonary rehabilitation in COPD patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 76, 146151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.11.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Illness perception domains within the CSM

Figure 1

Table 2. Measures included within Qualtrics survey

Figure 2

Table 3. Details of hierarchical multiple regression

Figure 3

Table 4. Demographic information by group

Figure 4

Table 5. Qualitative responses to BIPQ Q9.1

Figure 5

Table 6. Between-groups comparison scores on BIPQ and predictor variables

Figure 6

Table 7. Bivariate correlations (Spearman’s rho) for BIPQ and control variables

Figure 7

Table 8. Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analysis – symptom severity

Figure 8

Table 9. Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analysis – quality of life

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.