Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T05:26:12.575Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Primary agoraphobia as a specific phobia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

H. Stefan Bracha
Affiliation:
National Center for PTSD, Department of Veterans Affairs, Pacific Islands Health Care System, Spark M. Matsunaga Medical Center, and Asia-Pacific Center for Biosecurity, Disaster and Conflict Research, University of Hawaii School of Medicine, Honolulu, USA. Email: [email protected]
S. M. Lenze
Affiliation:
National Center for PTSD, Department of Veterans Affairs, Pacific Islands Health Care System, Spark M. Matsunaga Medical Center, Honolulu, USA
J. Shelton
Affiliation:
National Center for PTSD, Department of Veterans Affairs, Pacific Islands Health Care System, Spark M. Matsunaga Medical Center, Honolulu, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2006 

The elegant study of 1920 participants from the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area programme concluded that ‘the implied one-way causal relationship between spontaneous panic attacks and agoraphobia in DSM–IV appears incorrect’ (Reference Bienvenu, Onyike and SteinBienvenu et al, 2006). Bienvenu et al echo the arguments of many researchers, beginning with Marks (Reference Marks1987), that agoraphobia without panic attacks (primary agoraphobia) should be reinstated in DSM–V as a stand-alone diagnosis as in ICD–10.

It has been argued that evolutionary biological reasoning predicts the existence of a ‘hard-wired’ primary stand-alone agoraphobia, which should be classified with other specific phobias (Reference BrachaBracha, 2006). Specific phobias have been considered as conserved traits that enhanced survival during the human era of evolutionary adaptedness (Reference Nesse and StearnsNesse, 1999; Reference BrachaBracha, 2006). Primary agoraphobia may similarly be traced back to the fact that humans relied on arboreality as a major escape response long after they diverged from chimpanzees. Homo sapiens expanded beyond its densely forested East-African indigenous niche into sparsely wooded habitats (savannahs and water-front dunes) only about 70 000 years ago. In sparsely wooded habitats, anxiety in wide-open spaces was arguably a survival-enhancing trait since opportunities for arboreal escape from large predators were limited (Reference BrachaBracha, 2006). These arguments may be relevant to psychiatric classification and contribute to the ‘neuroscience research agenda to guide development of a pathophysiologically based classification system’ emphasised in the research agenda for DSM–V (Reference Kupfer, First and RegierKupfer et al, 2002).

If, as one of us (Reference BrachaBracha, 2006) has argued, the two types of agoraphobia have different modes of acquisition, there might be some clinical implications. Primary agoraphobia might, like other specific phobias, be especially amenable to virtual reality exposure treatment. In contrast, agoraphobia secondary to panic attacks can be classified in DSM–V and treated along with post-traumatic stress disorder (and other fear–memory–overconsolidation disorders, which are misclassified as specific phobias in DSM–IV–TR, e.g. hospital phobia, dentist phobia, dog phobia, bird phobia, and bat phobia).

Finally, contrary to myth, predictions based on brain evolution are eminently testable/falsifiable (Reference Nesse and StearnsNesse, 1999). Some 30 such predictions are elaborated elsewhere (Reference BrachaBracha, 2006).

References

Bienvenu, O. J., Onyike, C. U., Stein, M. B., et al (2006) Agoraphobia in adults: incidence and longitudinal relationship with panic. British Journal of Psychiatry, 188, 432438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bracha, H. S. (2006) Human brain evolution and the “Neuroevolutionary Time-depth Principle”: implications for the reclassification of fear-circuitry-related traits in DSM–V and for studying resilience to warzone-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30, 827853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupfer, D. J., First, M. B. & Regier, D. A. (2002) A Research Agenda for DSM–V. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M. (1987) Fears, Phobias, and Rituals. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (1999) Testing evolutionary hypotheses about mental disorders. In Evolution in Health and Disease (ed. Stearns, S. C.), pp. 260266. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.