Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T19:40:53.707Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Overlap between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising disorders: separating unique from general psychopathology effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2020

Ebba Du Rietz*
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Erik Pettersson
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Isabell Brikell
Affiliation:
The National Centre for Register-based Research, Department of Economics and Business Economics, Business and Social Science, Aarhus University, Denmark
Laura Ghirardi
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Qi Chen
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Catharina Hartman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, the Netherlands
Paul Lichtenstein
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Henrik Larsson
Affiliation:
School of Medical Sciences, Örebro University; and Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Ralf Kuja-Halkola
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
*
Correspondence: Ebba Du Rietz. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is classified as a neurodevelopmental disorder in the latest diagnostic manuals, it shows phenotypic and genetic associations of similar magnitudes across neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising disorders.

Aims

To investigate if ADHD is aetiologically more closely related to neurodevelopmental than externalising or internalising disorder clusters, after accounting for a general psychopathology factor.

Method

Full and maternal half-sibling pairs (N = 774 416), born between 1980 and 1995, were identified from the Swedish Medical Birth and Multi-Generation Registers, and ICD diagnoses were obtained from the Swedish National Patient Register. A higher-order confirmatory factor analytic model was fitted to examine associations between ADHD and a general psychopathology factor, as well as a neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactor. Quantitative genetic modelling was performed to estimate the extent to which genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects influenced the associations with ADHD.

Results

ADHD was significantly and strongly associated with all three factors (r = 0.67–0.75). However, after controlling for a general psychopathology factor, only the association between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental-specific factor remained moderately strong (r = 0.43, 95% CI = 0.42–0.45) and was almost entirely influenced by genetic effects. In contrast, the association between ADHD and the externalising-specific factor was smaller (r = 0.25, 95% CI = 0.24–0.27), and largely influenced by non-shared environmental effects. There remained no internalising-specific factor after accounting for a general factor.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that ADHD comorbidity is largely explained by genetically influenced general psychopathology, but the strong link between ADHD and other neurodevelopmental disorders is also substantially driven by unique genetic influences.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors, 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

In the former versions of the diagnostic manuals DSM-IV-TR1 and ICD-10,2 attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was classified as part of disruptive behaviour disorders, often referred as ‘externalising’ disorders.Reference Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman and Zald3,Reference McMahon4 In the recently updated versions of the DSM-5 and ICD-11, ADHD is classified as a neurodevelopmental disorder. This change is supported by the high comorbidity and similar characteristics between ADHD and neurodevelopmental disorders.Reference Antshel, Zhang-James and Faraone5Reference Doernberg and Hollander7 A recent meta-analysis of twin studies, however, suggested substantial genetic overlap between ADHD and not only neurodevelopmental disorders, but also externalising and internalising disorders (genetic correlations, 0.49–0.56).Reference Andersson, Tuvblad, Chen, Du Rietz, Cortese and Kuja-Halkola8 These quantitative genetic findings have been paralleled by recent molecular genetic research, further suggesting similar magnitudes of genetic associations between ADHD and neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising disorders.Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als and Agerbo9Reference Du Rietz, Coleman, Glanville, Choi, O'Reilly and Kuntsi11 The substantial degree of genetic overlap between psychiatric disorders demonstrates that genetic variants that contribute to risk for developing psychiatric disorders have highly pleiotropic effects, i.e. influencing multiple disorders. There has been an increased research interest into a latent general psychopathology factor that explains phenotypic and genetic variation (10–57%) across psychiatric conditions, including ADHD.Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington and Israel12Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault and Eley18 However, it remains unclear whether genetic effects for ADHD are shared with other neurodevelopmental disorders, as well as externalising and internalising disorders, after accounting for general psychopathology. Some evidence for genetic specificity comes from a population-based study that investigated associations between polygenic risk score (PRS) for ADHD and childhood psychiatric symptoms after controlling for a general psychopathology factor;Reference Brikell, Larsson, Lu, Pettersson, Chen and Kuja-Halkola15 the specific association between ADHD PRS and hyperactivity–impulsivity symptoms was significant, but not with other neurodevelopmental, externalising or internalising symptoms.Reference Brikell, Larsson, Lu, Pettersson, Chen and Kuja-Halkola15 This finding is largely in line with another study that used ADHD PRS in children.Reference Riglin, Thapar, Leppert, Martin, Richards and Anney19 These prior studies, however, used PRSs of limited power to capture genetic variation associated with disease specificity,Reference Brikell, Larsson, Lu, Pettersson, Chen and Kuja-Halkola15,Reference Riglin, Thapar, Leppert, Martin, Richards and Anney19 and have mainly focused on psychiatric symptoms obtained in childhood. Further research, using statistically powerful methods and clinical assessments beyond childhood, are therefore needed to understand if ADHD is aetiologically closer to certain psychiatric domains after accounting for general psychopathology.

Aims

The aim of this study is to use a large-scale, multivariate sibling design of children and young adults to estimate the general versus specific (after controlling for general effects) psychopathologic influences that explain the associations between ADHD and comorbid disorder clusters (neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising), and to what extent these are genetic or environmental in origin. Despite uncertainties in the current research literature, we hypothesise that ADHD will be most aetiologically closely linked to the neurodevelopmental cluster after accounting for a general psychopathology factor, in light of the similar disorder characteristics,Reference Antshel, Zhang-James and Faraone5Reference Doernberg and Hollander7 which would support the structure of the recently revised diagnostic manuals.

Method

Sample

Our source population comprised all individuals born in Sweden between 1980 and 1995, identified from the Medical Birth Register (1 688 807 individuals). We excluded individuals who were born with congenital malformations (n = 80 912), died (n = 7412) or emigrated (n = 57 389) before the age of 15 years. Using Swedish personal identification numbers, we linked several nation-wide registers. The National Patient Register includes psychiatric in-patient admissions in Sweden since 1973 and out-patient diagnoses since 2001, classified according to the ICD-8 (1969–1986), ICD-9 (1987–1996) or ICD-10 (1997–present). Lifetime diagnoses were treated as binary variables (presence/absence). Each participant could receive more than one diagnosis during the study period.

Using the Multi-Generation Register, we identified all full and maternal half-siblings, excluding adopted children (n = 11 266). Among individuals without half-siblings, we selected one random full sibling pair per family who were not twins. In families with half-siblings, we randomly selected one pair of maternal half-siblings. In total, we included 774 416 individuals from 341 066 full and 46 142 maternal half-sibling pairs. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm, Sweden (Dnr 2013/862–31/5). Since our study was based on population registers, the requirement for informed consent was waived.

Measures

We extracted main and secondary ICD-9 and ICD-10 diagnoses from in-patient or out-patient services from the National Patient Register. We focused on neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising disorders that are polygenic in nature (e.g. Rett's syndrome was excluded; see Supplementary Table 1, available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.152, for included disorders and ICD codes).

Statistical analysis

Phenotypic analyses

We grouped the disorders into three clusters based on the structure in the DSM-5 and ICD-11, and on clustering structures from previous literature.Reference Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman and Zald3,Reference Kendler, Prescott, Myers and Neale20Reference Krueger22 The neurodevelopmental cluster included autism spectrum disorder, developmental and learning disorders, intellectual disability and motor disorders. The externalising cluster included oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, antisocial personality disorder, alcohol misuse and drug misuse. Conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder and antisocial personality disorders were grouped as one disorder category because of low prevalence rates and the moderately high conversion rate of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder into adult antisocial personality disorder.Reference Moffitt, Arseneault, Jaffee, Kim-Cohen, Koenen and Odgers23,Reference Simonoff, Elander, Holmshaw, Pickles, Murray and Rutter24 The internalising cluster included depression, general anxiety, phobic disorders, reactions to severe stress and adjustment disorders (e.g. post-traumatic stress disorder), and obsessive–compulsive disorder.

We fitted a higher-order confirmatory factor analytic model on individual-level phenotypic data across full and maternal half-sibling pairs, hereafter referred to as the general factor model. This model was suitable for our research question, as it enables partitioning the relative contributions of general and cluster-specific factors from the overall correlations between ADHD and disorder clusters. The three latent subfactors (neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors) were set to load onto the psychiatric disorders. We modelled a general psychopathology factor, which loaded onto each of the three subfactors. Correlations between the subfactors were fixed to zero so that they only correlated through the psychopathology factor. Each subfactor had an additional loading from a specific factor (i.e. residual variance not explained by the general factor), referred to as the neurodevelopmental-, externalising- and internalising-specific factors. Correlations between each specific factor and the general factor were fixed to zero. We then correlated ADHD with the general and the three specific factors.

Quantitative genetic analyses

We used a multivariate sibling design to establish the genetic and environmental aetiology of the general factor and the three subfactors (neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising) by specifying additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C) and non-shared environmental (E) latent variables as sources of variance and covariance between factors.Reference Pettersson, Larsson and Lichtenstein13 A-variables were estimated by fixing them to correlate between siblings at their expected average sharing of cosegregating genes (0.5 for full siblings, 0.25 for half-siblings). C-variables (i.e. non-genetic components that make sibling pairs similar) were estimated by fixing them to correlate at unity across full and half-siblings. Thus, we assumed the shared environment variables to be equally shared across full and maternal half-siblings, as previous literature has shown strong support for this assumption in Swedish registers.Reference Pettersson, Larsson and Lichtenstein13 E-variables were estimated by fixing them to correlate at zero across all siblings, thus measuring non-genetic components making siblings within a pair dissimilar.

All analyses included gender and birth year (as categories, see Table 1) as covariates to adjust for different follow-up lengths and cohort effects. Analyses were performed in R (version 3.4.1 for Windows), using the ‘corrplot’ (version 0.84) and ‘OpenMx’ packages (version 2.15.5).Reference Neale, Hunter, Pritikin, Zahery, Brick and Kirkpatrick25,Reference Wei and Simko26 Because half-siblings tend to display a higher rate of disorders than full siblings,Reference Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson and Lichtenstein27,Reference Yao, Kuja-Halkola, Martin, Lu, Lichtenstein and Norring28 prevalence rates were allowed to vary across full and half-siblings. We used weighted least squares for model fitting, and χ 2-tests to compare nested models. We evaluated if the models provided a good fit using the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA; comparison to the maximum possible fit to the data) and the comparative fit index (CFI; comparison to a model where correlations between observed variables are assumed to be zero).Reference Hu and Bentler29 Data analysis was performed between 3 May 2019 and 14 January 2020.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics in study sample: age, gender and prevalence rates of psychiatric disorders

Oppositional defiant and related disorders includes oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder.

Secondary analyses

To allow comparability with prior findings in the literature, we ran additional analyses with methodological approaches that have been more commonly used in research. First, we conducted univariate ACE modelling of the psychiatric disorders to allow comparability with estimates from twin studies. Second, we fitted a correlated factors model, where disorders loaded onto one of the three neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors (allowing subfactors to correlate and without a general factor). We additionally ran a bifactor model, which derives the general psychopathology factor from the correlation matrix between the psychiatric disorders rather than higher-order subfactors. With this bifactor model, we aimed to investigate if the patterns of within-individual and between-sibling correlations of ADHD with the cluster-specific factors remained when the correlation between disorders in different clusters were not forced to correlate through the general psychopathology factor via the cluster subfactors. To examine the robustness of our findings across different sibling selection criteria, we re-ran the general factor quantitative genetic analyses on sibling pairs born closest together rather than random pairs in a family. Finally, to examine whether differences in disease onset (e.g. early versus late adult onset of the non-neurodevelopmental disorders) had a substantial confounding effect on the structure of our general factor model, and on phenotypic and aetiologic associations of disorder clusters with ADHD, we repeated the general factor quantitative genetic analyses, decreasing the maximum follow-up age by 1 year at a time. This was achieved by increasing the earliest included birth year from 1980 to 1990 by 1 year at a time, with follow-up ages ranging from 18–33 to 18–23 years.

Results

In the sample, 51.3% were males and the average follow-up length was until 25.3 years of age (Table 1 for information on gender, birth year and prevalence of psychiatric disorders). Figure 1 displays the within-individual phenotypic (tetrachoric) correlations between psychiatric disorders (Supplementary Table 2 for 95% CIs). The overall pattern of correlations showed that psychiatric disorders most strongly correlated with other disorders within their respective, prespecified neurodevelopmental, externalising or internalising clusters, whereas the correlations between ADHD and other disorders were difficult to characterise.

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; OCD, obsessive–compulsive disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder.

Fig. 1 Observed within-individual phenotypic correlations between disorders.

General factor model

Phenotypic analyses

The general factor model fit the data well (RMSEA = 0.006, CFI = 0.974). The general psychopathology factor loaded moderately to strongly on the neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors (factor loadings 0.60, 0.77 and 0.99, respectively) (Supplementary Table 3a). ADHD was significantly and strongly correlated with the general psychopathology factor (r = 0.67). The total correlations between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors (sum of contributions through the general psychopathology and cluster-specific factors) were 0.75, 0.67 and 0.67, respectively. Because the general psychopathology factor loaded almost perfectly on the internalising subfactor, we could not estimate valid standard errors. We therefore fixed the internalising subfactor to have a loading of 1 from the psychopathology factor, which did not lead to a worse model fit (Supplementary Table 3b) and did not affect the subsequent estimates (Supplementary Tables 3–8). In this model, the magnitude of loadings on the neurodevelopmental and externalising subfactors from the general psychopathology factor, and their correlations with ADHD, remained unchanged (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Tables 3c and 6 for 95% CIs).

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; GAD, generalised anxiety disorders; OCD, obsessive–compulsive disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant, conduct and antisocial personality disorders; Stress, reactions to severe stress and adjustment disorders.

Fig. 2 General factor solution: factor loadings and phenotypic correlations between ADHD and the general psychopathology factor, neurodevelopmental-specific and externalising-specific factors.

After accounting for the general psychopathology factor, ADHD showed a significant and moderately strong phenotypic correlation with the neurodevelopmental-specific factor (r = 0.43, 95% CI = 0.42–0.45), and a significantly smaller correlation with the externalising-specific factor (r = 0.25, 95% CI = 0.24–0.27). Given that variance in the internalising factor was entirely subsumed by its covariance with the general psychopathology factor (loading fixed at 1), a correlation was not estimable with the internalising-specific factor (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 6).

Quantitative genetic analyses

Observed correlations among psychiatric disorders and latent factors between full and maternal half-sibling pairs are shown in Supplementary Tables 7 and 9.

The model fitting results estimated the heritability of the general psychopathology factor at 0.49, with the contribution of shared environment at 0.07 and non-shared environment at 0.44. For the neurodevelopmental-specific and externalising-specific factors, the estimated heritability was 0.89 and 0.80, the shared environment contribution was 0.09 and 0.06 and the non-shared environment contribution was 0.03 and 0.14, respectively. Estimates are plotted in Fig. 3 and corresponding 95% CIs are reported in Supplementary Table 10 (see Supplementary Table 11 for ACE correlations).

Fig. 3 General factor solution: proportion of variance in the subfactors, and in their phenotypic correlations with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), explained by specific and general additive genetic (A), specific and general shared (C) and specific and general non-shared (E) environmental effects. See Supplementary Table 14 for estimates and 95% CIs.

For the phenotypic correlation between ADHD and the general psychopathology factor (r = 0.67), genetics contributed with 0.35 (52% of total correlation), shared environment contributed with 0.06 (9%) and non-shared environment contributed with 0.26 (39%) (see Supplementary Table 10 for estimates and 95% CIs). For the phenotypic correlation between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental-specific factor (r = 0.43), genetics contributed with 0.46 (107%; >100% because of negative contribution to correlation from shared environment), shared environment contributed with −0.07 (−15%; negative contribution to correlation) and non-shared environment contributed with 0.04 (8%). For the phenotypic correlation between ADHD and the externalising-specific factor (r = 0.25), genetics contributed with 0.06 (23%), shared environment contributed with 0.06 (22%) and non-shared environment contributed with 0.14 (55%) (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Table 10).

Secondary analyses

See Supplementary Table 12 for univariate ACE model estimates for single disorders, which are overall in line with prior estimates from twin studies.Reference Larsson, Chang, D'Onofrio and Lichtenstein30,Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven and Visscher31 The correlated factors model (RMSEA = 0.006, CFI = 0.974) revealed that the three neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors were moderately to strongly intercorrelated (r = 0.46–0.77), and were all significantly and strongly correlated with ADHD (r = 0.67–0.75). The magnitude of correlations, along with the heritability and coheritability of the subfactors and ADHD, were overall in line with previous research (Supplementary Fig. 1).10,Reference Du Rietz, Coleman, Glanville, Choi, O'Reilly and Kuntsi11,Reference Sezlam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt and Plomin14,Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey17,Reference Krueger22 The bifactor model (RMSEA = 0.004, CFI = 0.982) revealed very similar within-individual and between-sibling correlations of ADHD with the general and specific factors compared with the higher-order model (Supplementary Fig. 2 and Table 13).

When we re-ran analyses on siblings born closest together, the estimates and model fit remained very similar to when we used random sibling pairs in a family (Supplementary Fig. 3). The negative contribution of the shared environment on the correlation between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental-specific factor decreased slightly in the analyses of siblings born closest together. Lastly, when analyses were repeated in subcohorts with different lengths of maximum follow-up age, the overall pattern of results remained. Although the sample size decreased with every 1-year reduction in follow-up, resulting in increased instability in estimates, the results remained similar in terms of decomposition of the correlations between ADHD and neurodevelopmental, internalising and externalising factors into specific and general A, C and E components (Supplementary Fig. 4).

Discussion

In this large-scale, register-based sibling study, we showed for the first time, using clinical diagnoses, that ADHD is more phenotypically and genetically linked to neurodevelopmental disorders than to externalising and internalising disorders, after accounting for a general psychopathology factor.

The phenotypic correlations between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors were all strong, and were moderately to strongly influenced by genetic variation, in line with previous research.Reference Andersson, Tuvblad, Chen, Du Rietz, Cortese and Kuja-Halkola8,Reference Lahey, van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz16,Reference Kessler, Adler, Barkley, Biederman, Conners and Demler32,Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters33 After accounting for the general psychopathology factor, the correlation between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental-specific factor remained moderately strong, and was largely genetic in origin, suggesting substantial unique sharing of biological mechanisms between disorders. In contrast, the correlation between ADHD and the externalising-specific factor was much smaller and was largely explained by non-shared environmental effects. The observation that there remained an association between ADHD and the externalising-specific factor after accounting for familial (genetic and shared environmental) effects is in line with a causal framework of ADHD on externalising disorders. ADHD, which often has an early onset, might subsequently lead to later-onset substance misuse or antisocial personality disorder. Additional research, using longitudinal causal modelling approaches, may further unravel the effect of ADHD on subsequent externalising disorders. Lastly, the correlation between ADHD and the internalising subfactor was almost entirely explained by the general psychopathology factor. This finding suggests that the comorbidity of ADHD and internalising disorders is largely because of pleiotropic genetic effects and non-shared environmental influences that have general effects on psychopathology.

Research and clinical implications

Although past research and results from our correlated factors model suggest similar magnitudes of genetic overlap between ADHD and different psychiatric disorders, we found that after accounting for general psychopathology, the strongest genetic sharing was with neurodevelopmental disorders. These findings demonstrate that isolating the psychopathology factor may allow for identifying specific mechanisms (e.g. biological pathways) and risk factors, uniquely related to only a subset of disorders or syndromes, and may have large potential for future aetiological research. For example, the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology framework hypothesises that a higher-order approach to phenotypes may increase the precision of molecular genetic findings by differentiating between genetic liability for broad psychopathology and dimension-specific genetic risk factors.Reference Waszczuk, Eaton, Krueger, Shackman, Waldman and Zald34

Our findings on the close unique genetic link between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental subfactor are in line with the current diagnostic classification of ADHD as a neurodevelopmental disorder, despite our use of ICD codes from the former classification system. We acknowledge that there is debate regarding to what extent genetic structure of diseases should be used to inform diagnostic nosology.Reference Hyman35Reference Smoller, Andreassen, Edenberg, Farone, Glatt and Kendler37 However, a common view in research is that genetic data, and mechanisms learned from them, can, at least in part, aid in the formulations and/or revisions of the clinical syndromes and diagnostic classification.Reference Smoller, Andreassen, Edenberg, Farone, Glatt and Kendler37

Limitations

This large-scale study has several strengths, including the use of comprehensive and clinical assessments of psychiatric disorders across childhood and young adulthood, and the use of a representative population cohort. However, findings should be interpreted in light of some limitations.

Individuals with multiple psychiatric diagnoses or with siblings who have diagnoses may be more likely to get in contact with the mental health system, which can lead to an overestimation of associations among disorders and sibling pairs. On the other hand, these registers do not include individuals with relatively mild psychiatric problems, or troubled individuals who do not seek help or receive specialist care, leading to an underestimation of prevalence rates. Nevertheless, we note that our results of a general factor of psychopathology are largely consistent with previous survey studies, suggesting that results are not entirely because of these biases.Reference Brikell, Larsson, Lu, Pettersson, Chen and Kuja-Halkola15,Reference Lahey, van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz16

During the follow-up period of individuals in our cohort, there have been changes in diagnostic practises for several psychiatric disorders (e.g. the rate of ADHD diagnoses increased fivefold from 2004 to 2015),Reference Rydell, Lundström, Gillberg, Lichtenstein and Larsson38 and the register coverage has improved. Further, there were variations in follow-up length between individuals in our study. To account for these issues, we adjusted for the association between birth year and the expected prevalence for each disorder. However, if the change in diagnostic practises and register coverage has changed the phenotypes and aetiologies of the diagnoses, or has had differential effects on the disorders, we may have remaining bias in our results. Replication using other data sources, or similar sources with longer follow-up, would strengthen the inferences from the current study. To further account for variations in follow-up length, we also repeated analyses in younger adult subcohorts (i.e. with narrower age ranges and follow-up length), by excluding one birth year cohort at a time, starting with the oldest; findings revealed consistent patterns across these subcohorts. For the ACE models, we assumed that full and maternal half-siblings share their common (family) environment to the same degree. We acknowledge that this is a simplification, but sensitivity analyses in a similar study showed strong support for this assumption.Reference Pettersson, Larsson and Lichtenstein13 Further, when we re-ran analyses with siblings born closest together (i.e. siblings who are closer in age and may therefore share more common environment), our overall results remained unchanged.

A limitation of our analysis of nation-wide administrative patient data is that findings cannot be interpreted in relation to more refined ADHD subtypes. Thus, it is important to note that there may be heterogeneity in the aetiology of ADHD, and individuals with ADHD may display different patterns of comorbidities. However, investigations into this is beyond the scope of our study and level of analysis.

Lastly, our finding that the general psychopathology factor loaded nearly perfectly on the internalising subfactor has been reported in previous research,Reference Lahey, van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz16 and studies have often reported that internalising disorders have the highest, or among the highest, loadings from the general factor.Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington and Israel12,Reference Sezlam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt and Plomin14,Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey17,Reference Gard, Ware, Hyde, Schmitz, Faul and Mitchell39 However, it is possible that this high loading may have partly resulted from the predefined confirmatory approach to factor definitions. Furthermore, the chosen disorders in a study dictates what the psychopathology factor captures; thus, the resulting structure will be partly specific to the current study design. The bifactor model produced very similar within-individual and between-sibling correlations between ADHD and the neurodevelopmental, externalising and internalising subfactors compared with the higher-order model. This supports the robustness of our results, and suggests that the almost perfect loading of the general psychopathology factor on the internalising subfactor is not a major source of bias.

In conclusion, ADHD comorbidity is largely explained by partly genetically influenced general psychopathology, but the strong link between ADHD and other neurodevelopmental disorders is also driven by specific genetic influences. These findings support the classification of ADHD as a neurodevelopmental disorder in the recently revised diagnostic manuals, and provide insights into the structure of the aetiologic underpinnings of psychopathology.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.152

Data availability

Data cannot be shared publicly because of the Swedish Secrecy Act. Data from the Medical Birth Register, the Multi-Generation Register and the National Patient Register were used for this study and made available by ethical approval. Researchers may apply for access through the Swedish Research Ethics Boards (www.etikprovningsmyndigheten.se) and from the primary data owners Statistics Sweden (www.scb.se) and the National Board of Health and Welfare (www.socialstyrelsen.se), in accordance with Swedish law.

Author contributions

E.D.R., H.L. and R.K.-H. were responsible for the study concept and design. E.D.R. and R.K.-H. were responsible for statistical analysis. E.D.R., E.P., I.B., L.G., Q.C., C.H., P.L., H.L. and R.K.-H. analysed and/or interpreted the data. E.D.R. and R.K.-H. drafted the manuscript. E.D.R., E.P., I.B., L.G., Q.C., C.H., P.L., H.L. and R.K.-H. provided critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content.

Funding

H.L. acknowledges financial support from the Swedish Research Council (2018-02599) and the Swedish Brain Foundation (FO2018-0273). E.D.R. was supported by grant 2019-01172 from the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life, and Welfare. E.P. was supported by grant 2017-01358 from the Swedish Research Council. This project has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant agreement no. 667302.

Declaration of interest

H.L. reported receiving grants from Shire Pharmaceuticals during the conduct of the study; personal fees from and serving as a speaker for Shire Pharmaceuticals and Evolan Pharma AB outside the submitted work; and sponsorship for a conference on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder from Shire Pharmaceuticals outside the submitted work. No other authors report any conflicts of interest.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.152

References

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn, Text Revision). American Psychiatric Publishing, 2000.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. World Health Organization, 1992.Google Scholar
Lahey, BB, Krueger, RF, Rathouz, PJ, Waldman, ID, Zald, DH. A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychol Bull 2017; 143(2): 142–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMahon, RJ. Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment of externalizing problems in children: the role of longitudinal data. J Consult Clin Psychol 1994; 62(5): 901–17.Google ScholarPubMed
Antshel, KM, Zhang-James, Y, Faraone, SV. The comorbidity of ADHD and autism spectrum disorder. Expert Rev Neurother 2013; 13: 1117–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rommelse, NN, Geurts, HM, Franke, B, Buitelaar, JK, Hartman, CA. A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35(6): 1363–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doernberg, E, Hollander, E. Neurodevelopmental disorders (ASD and ADHD): DSM-5, ICD-10, and ICD-11. CNS Spectrums 2016; 21: 295–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Andersson, A, Tuvblad, A, Chen, Q, Du Rietz, E, Cortese, S, Kuja-Halkola, R, et al. The strength of the genetic overlap between ADHD and other psychiatric symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analysis. To be published in J Child Psychol Psychiatry [Preprint] 2020. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13233 [cited 20 Mar 2020].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D, Walters, RK, Martin, J, Mattheisen, M, Als, TD, Agerbo, E, et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Genet 2019; 51(1): 6375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Genomic relationships, novel loci, and pleiotropic mechanisms across eight psychiatric disorders. Cell 2019; 179(7): 1469–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Du Rietz, E, Coleman, J, Glanville, K, Choi, SW, O'Reilly, PF, Kuntsi, J. Association of polygenic risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with co-occurring traits and disorders. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2018; 3(7): 635–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A, Houts, RM, Belsky, DW, Goldman-Mellor, SJ, Harrington, H, Israel, S, et al. The P factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci 2014; 2: 119–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettersson, E, Larsson, H, Lichtenstein, P. Common psychiatric disorders share the same genetic origin: a multivariate sibling study of the Swedish population. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21(5): 717–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sezlam, S, Coleman, JRI, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Plomin, R. A polygenic P factor for major psychiatric disorders. Transl Psychiatr 2018; 8(1): 205.Google Scholar
Brikell, I, Larsson, H, Lu, Y, Pettersson, E, Chen, Q, Kuja-Halkola, R, et al. The contribution of common genetic risk variants for ADHD to a general factor of childhood psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25: 1809–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, BB, van Hulle, CA, Singh, AL, Waldman, I, Rathouz, PJ. Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68(2): 181–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldman, ID, Poore, HE, van Hulle, C, Rathouz, PJ, Lahey, BB. External validity of a hierarchical dimensional model of child and adolescent psychopathology: tests using confirmatory factor analyses and multivariate behavior genetic analyses. J Abnorm Psychol 2016; 125: 1053–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allegrini, A, Cheesman, R, Rimfeld, K, Selzam, S, Pingault, JB, Eley, TC, et al. The P factor: genetic analyses support a general dimension of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2019; 61(1): 30–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riglin, L, Thapar, AK, Leppert, B, Martin, J, Richards, A, Anney, R, et al. Using genetics to examine a general liability to childhood psychopathology. Behav Genet 2020; 50: 213–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Prescott, CA, Myers, J, Neale, MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60(9): 929–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luningham, JM, Poore, HE, Yang, J, Waldman, ID. Testing structural models of psychopathology at the genomic level. BioRxiv [Preprint] 2019. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1101/502039 [cited 10 Mar 2020].Google Scholar
Krueger, RF. The structure of common mental disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56(10): 921–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L, Jaffee, SR, Kim-Cohen, J, Koenen, KC, Odgers, CL, et al. Research review: DSM-V conduct disorder: research needs for an evidence base. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49(1): 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonoff, E, Elander, J, Holmshaw, J, Pickles, A, Murray, R, Rutter, M. Predictors of antisocial personality disorder. Continuities from childhood to adult life. Br J Psychiatry 2004; 184: 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, MC, Hunter, MD, Pritikin, JN, Zahery, M, Brick, TR, Kirkpatrick, RM, et al. OpenMx 2.0: extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika 2016; 81(2): 535–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wei, T, Simko, V. R Package “corrplot”: Visualization of a Correlation Matrix (Version 0.84). GitHub, 2017 (https://github.com/taiyun/corrplot).Google Scholar
Kuja-Halkola, R, D'Onofrio, BM, Larsson, H, Lichtenstein, P. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and adverse outcomes in offspring: genetic and environmental sources of covariance. Behav Genet 2014; 44(5): 456–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yao, S, Kuja-Halkola, R, Martin, J, Lu, Y, Lichtenstein, P, Norring, C, et al. Associations between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and various eating disorders: a Swedish nationwide population study using multiple genetically informative approaches. Biol Psychiatry 2019; 86(8): 577–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, LT, Bentler, PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling 1999; 6: 155.Google Scholar
Larsson, H, Chang, Z, D'Onofrio, BM, Lichtenstein, P. The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychol Med 2014; 44: 2223–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polderman, TJ, Benyamin, B, de Leeuw, CA, Sullivan, PF, van Bochoven, A, Visscher, PM, et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet 2015; 47(7): 702–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Adler, L, Barkley, R, Biederman, J, Conners, CK, Demler, O, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 716–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Chiu, WT, Demler, O, Walters, EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 617–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waszczuk, MA, Eaton, NR, Krueger, RF, Shackman, AJ, Waldman, ID, Zald, DH, et al. Redefining phenotypes to advance psychiatric genetics: implications from hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 2020; 129: 143–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, SE. The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2010; 17: 1121.Google Scholar
Insel, TR, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 748–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smoller, JW, Andreassen, OA, Edenberg, HJ, Farone, SV, Glatt, SJ, Kendler, KS. Psychiatric genetics and the structure of psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry 2019; 24(3): 409–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Rydell, M, Lundström, S, Gillberg, C, Lichtenstein, P, Larsson, J. Has the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder phenotype become more common in children between 2004 and 2014? Trends over 10 years from a Swedish general population sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2018; 59(8): 863–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, AM, Ware, EB, Hyde, LW, Schmitz, L, Faul, J, Mitchell, C. Assessing the structure and specificity of polygenic scores for psychiatric disorders in a population-based cohort of older adults. BioRxiv [Preprint] 2019. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1101/601609 [cited 15 Mar 2020].Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive statistics in study sample: age, gender and prevalence rates of psychiatric disorders

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Observed within-individual phenotypic correlations between disorders.

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; OCD, obsessive–compulsive disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder.
Figure 2

Fig. 2 General factor solution: factor loadings and phenotypic correlations between ADHD and the general psychopathology factor, neurodevelopmental-specific and externalising-specific factors.

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; GAD, generalised anxiety disorders; OCD, obsessive–compulsive disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant, conduct and antisocial personality disorders; Stress, reactions to severe stress and adjustment disorders.
Figure 3

Fig. 3 General factor solution: proportion of variance in the subfactors, and in their phenotypic correlations with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), explained by specific and general additive genetic (A), specific and general shared (C) and specific and general non-shared (E) environmental effects. See Supplementary Table 14 for estimates and 95% CIs.

Supplementary material: File

Du Rietz et al. supplementary material

Du Rietz et al. supplementary material

Download Du Rietz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 11.4 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.