Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:40:35.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Kaleidoscope

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2019

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Kaleidoscope
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2019 

It is a truth universally acknowledged that little gets mental health folk hotter under the collar than interpreting pharmacological trial data, so let's do one on each of antidepressants and antipsychotics. The PANDA trial reports on the clinical effectiveness of sertraline in a pragmatic double-blind placebo-controlled randomised trial (RCT) in primary care.Reference Lewis, Duffy, Ades, Amos, Arayn and Brabyn1 What is interesting about this is that it maps onto a population different to most antidepressant RCTs: 653 participants had symptoms of depression of any severity over the past 2 years, where antidepressant benefit was uncertain. They were titrated to 100 mg of the medication, or placebo, over a fortnight and symptoms were measured over 12 weeks. Of the 550 included in the final analysis, there was no evidence of a clinically meaningful reduction in depressive symptoms by 6 weeks, and weak evidence by 12 weeks. However, there was evidence for a reduction in anxiety, better mental-health-related quality of life and self-reported improvements in mental health. Fascinating as the results have been, almost as interesting has been the diverse range of conclusions drawn from the data. Some have interpreted the study as demonstrating that sertraline is not working as intended as an antidepressant in primary care; others, including the authors, note that it is effecting meaningful change even in circumstances where it being prescribed outside guidelines (namely in many without a clear diagnosis of clinical depression).

Moving swiftly onto a discussion on the relative merits of pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy for schizophrenia. Bighelli et al ask if we have been looking at apples and oranges insofar as the relative trials of each are usually against placebo or treatment as usual and not directly compared with each other.Reference Bighelli, Leucht, Huhn, Reitmeir, Schwermann and Wallis2 They unpicked the underpinning characteristics of 80 studies involving over 18 000 total participants on medication, and 53 studies with over 4000 participants included in psychological interventions. Psychological trials had less severely unwell individuals with shorter illness histories, a longer intervention duration that was administered in addition to medication, and had higher risks of bias. Medication studies included larger number of participants recruited across a greater number of research centres, and participants included more men, in-patients, and older individuals. Psychological trials were largely publically funded, pharmacological ones by the drug manufacturers; however, the two groups were considered equally conflicted insofar as the psychotherapy ones were often run by researchers with an allegiance to the treatment. So, yes, perhaps apples and oranges, but in clinical practice one would not expect individuals who are acutely psychotic to commence a psychological therapy, and the two treatment modalities are not in any way exclusive of each other. We support the authors’ concluding statement that ‘In the interest of patients, psychopharmacologists and psychotherapists should optimise their treatments rather than seeing them in competition’.

It is an intriguing observation that congenital blindness is protective against the development of psychosis; how is this the case, and what does it teach us about neuropathology? The finding is all the more peculiar given how congenital blindness often comes from perinatal traumata or chromosomal disorders that have higher rates of psychosis. Tom Pollak & Phil Corlett review this in a fascinating paper describing a Bayesian prediction error minimisation model.Reference Pollak and Corlett3 In Bayesian inference, prior predictions of the world are combined with actual sensory inputs to create a posterior probability distribution; the relative weight assigned to the prior compared with the sensory data determines how much the latter updates beliefs or gets ignored. Pollak & Corlett give the analogy of speaking with people at a noisy party: it is far easier to talk to someone who is well known to you in such a setting, as you will have an already existing precise model of their speech against which to assimilate the received sensory input. Thus, the precision of your prior beliefs will be high, and one can more easily disregard prediction errors, and the likelihood of misunderstanding is lower. Taking this to psychosis, a Bayesian model has inappropriate weighting of irrelevant sensory stimuli, which erroneously update beliefs about the world. In the case of congenital blindness, they argue that this increases the precision and stability of higher-level ‘priors’ that protect against the computational deficits underpinning psychosis. In other words, such congenitally blind individuals are forced to rely so much more on their other senses such that their model of the world becomes far more resilient to false inferences from ‘noisy’ or ambiguous perceptual input in other modalities, as is seen in schizophrenia. The authors extend their argument to later-life visual impairment that is associated with hallucinations, such as Charles-Bonnet syndrome. Here the brain has ‘developed normally’ in early life, and thus higher-level predictions of reduced visually precise input will be explained confidently but falsely, resulting in visual hallucinations. They take this on to neurodegenerative conditions such as Parkinson's disease and Alzheimer's dementia, which are often associated with complex psychoses including visual abnormalities.

A Lancet commission concluded that improving outcomes in education would be the single most useful intervention to improve health and well-being.Reference Patton4 The ‘growth mindset principle’ evolved out of the work of Carol Dweck and the central principle is that ability and intelligence can be developed, rather than being a fixed trait, by explicitly setting learning as a goal and in parallel with understanding that effort makes one ‘stronger’. It is appealing as a school intervention, but for counterpoint, an interview with geneticist Richard Plomin was far less optimistic.Reference Severs and Henshaw5 An experiment is called for, and in the National Study of Learning Mindsets an online growth mindset intervention was delivered to over 6000 students in 65 state-funded schools in the USA who were low achievers compared with their peers.Reference Yeager, Hanselman, Walton, Murray and Crosnoe6 Interestingly, the authors open their paper with a detailed discussion of heterogeneity of treatment effects and are explicit that they set out to analyse their data to find why the intervention does (or does not) work as a function of the schools in which it was delivered. To guard against bias and false positive outcomes, the intervention and analysis were masked with pre-registered analyses and they pre-specified a priori effect sizes for significance. Further, they used an independent commercial research company to recruit schools so that the sample would be generalisable to the whole US state-funded school population.

The design included just over 11 000 students in ninth grade (14–15 years of age) and they were randomised evenly to either an online mindset intervention (challenging beliefs about intelligence) or no-intervention (a presentation on brain function but no emphasis on beliefs about intelligence). All students completed a self-reported fixed/growth mindset questionnaire (eliciting the students’ beliefs about ability and its flexibility). After randomisation, the first of the two-part intervention was delivered followed by a second session at around a median of 21 or 27 days. After the second session was delivered, all students took a repeat mindset questionnaire and took a maths test. On to the results: first of all, they measured change in mindset (away from ‘fixed’, toward ‘growth’) in 5650 students and found a non-zero effect size with those in the intervention being less ‘fixed’. By looking at the change in grade-point averages (GPAs) in core subjects – for the intervention versus the control groups – they generalise their standardised mean difference effect size to conclude that the intervention would result in 5.3% (of 1.5 million students across the US school system) being prevented from having GPAs representing ‘off track’ target performance. Consistent with their analysis plan to explore heterogeneity across schools, they found that the fixed-mindset change was unaffected when the school was included as a covariate (consistent with the intervention being delivered by the same online platform), but that lower-performing students’ improvement in GPA performance did vary across schools. The authors suggest this represents ‘contextual mechanisms’ that either sustain or negate initial effects of the intervention. Indeed, the interaction of overall school performance with student's change in GPA showed the highest effect in poorly performing rather than high-performing schools. The intervention costs 1 h of student time, appears most effective in lower-performing students in poorly performing schools and has the potential to change the educational trajectory of tens of thousands of students. The authors conclude by speculating on how their study demonstrates the importance of adolescents’ beliefs in the crucial maturational period post-puberty.

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which has been argued to look more like a systemic illness than a ‘purely’ brain-based one, is potentially a great place to explore for a biological signature. Using a simple blood sample and a multi-omic approach, Dean et al worked in collaboration with the US army to generate and validate a biomarker panel for diagnosing combat-related PTSD.Reference Dean, Hammamieh, Mellon, Abu-Amara, Flory and Guffani7 Just over 80 included participants were combat-exposed veterans of the Afghanistan and Iraqi wars, between 20 and 60 years old, and who had been exposed to DSM-IV PTSD Criterion A trauma during deployment, but otherwise quite heterogeneous. Those meeting the full DSM-IV criteria and with Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) scores over 40 were considered PTSD positive; controls had current CAPS scores less than 20. In addition to basic physiological measures, blood samples were assayed for more than 1 million markers including DNA methylation, proteomics, metabolomics, miRNAs, endocrine markers, small molecules and routine clinical lab panels. Using a predictive model familiar to the financial sector called ‘wisdom of crowds’, they narrowed the panel to 343 unique potential marker candidates. To further refine this, after 3 years they invited 55 participants back and ran them through all testing again, settling upon 28 biomarkers representing alterations in DNA methylation, miRNAs, metabolites, physiological and clinical lab measures. To validate the panel, a new, independent, cohort was brought in consisting of 26 participants with PTSD and 26 controls.

The final panel had an accuracy rate of 81% and predicted not only illness status but severity, and tracked changes within participants over time. While some of the biomarkers had been previously linked to PTSD, others were a novel contribution. Interestingly, the predictive PTSD severity scores were most accurate in the subgroup of participants who also met the criteria for major depressive disorders. This panel will need further testing with a series of groups including women, those with civilian trauma, chronic and recovered PTSD, and those with less extreme cases of PTSD. The ultimate aim is for a screening tool that could be used in large populations to triage focused clinical support to those trauma-exposed individuals most likely to benefit.

Finally, links between creativity and psychopathology have long been recognised and continue to intrigue: so what about one creative group that might be close to your heart – successful academics? The broader description is of an inverted-U curve: low levels of psychopathology being associated with creativity, something that declines with greater symptoms. Parnas et al note that many studies looking at this use self-reported occupational role, which is prone to ‘status inflation’, also known as lying about your job responsibilities.Reference Parnas, Sandsten, Vestergaard and Nordgaard8 They get around this by taking a group with assured track records of producing that which is novel, original, useful and valued – tenured academics. Taken from staff across three Danish universities, these were matched with randomly selected controls from the general population. Using the national civil register, first- and second-degree relatives of academics and controls were determined, and everyone's mental health data were analysed from the Psychiatric Central Research Register. The total data-set incorporated over half a million individuals (almost 12 000 tenured academics), and analyses were adjusted for age, gender and educational level. The academics themselves had a considerably lower odds ratio than controls of having been diagnosed with any mental disorder, but their siblings, children and nephews and nieces had significantly increased risk for having schizophrenia; the risk was greater for maternal, but not paternal half-siblings. With regards to bipolar affective disorder, the odds ratio was significantly increased for their parents, grandparents and nephews and nieces. The risk of all other mental disorders was lower than those seen in controls. Schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder are typically seen as the ‘creative’ mental illness-links, and the findings hold here.

References

1Lewis, G, Duffy, L, Ades, A, Amos, R, Arayn, R, Brabyn, S, et al. The clinical effectiveness of sertraline in primary care and the role of depression severity and duration (PANDA): a pragmatic, double-blind, placebo-controlled randomised trial. Lancet Psychiatry 19 Sept 2019 (https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30366-9).Google Scholar
2Bighelli, I, Leucht, C, Huhn, M, Reitmeir, C, Schwermann, F, Wallis, S, et al. Are randomized controlled trials on pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy for positive symptoms of schizophrenia comparable? A systematic review of patient and study characteristics. Schizophr Bull 18 Sept 2019 (https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbz090).Google Scholar
3Pollak, TA, Corlett, PR. Blindness, psychosis, and the visual construction of the world. Schizophr Bull 11 October 2019 (https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbz098).Google Scholar
4Patton, GC, et al. Our future: a Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. Lancet 2016; 387; 24232478.Google Scholar
5Severs, J, Henshaw, C. Exclusive: Growth Mindset is ‘Bullshit’, says Leading Geneticist. TES, 2019 (https://www.tes.com/news/exclusive-growth-mindset-bullshit-says-leading-geneticist).Google Scholar
6Yeager, DS, Hanselman, P, Walton, GM, Murray, JS, Crosnoe, R, et al. A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 2019; 573; 364–9.Google Scholar
7Dean, KR, Hammamieh, R, Mellon, SH, Abu-Amara, D, Flory, JD, Guffani, G, et al. Multi-omic biomarker identification and validation for diagnosing warzone-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Mol Psychiatry 10 Sept 2019 (doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0496-z).Google Scholar
8Parnas, J, Sandsten, KE, Vestergaard, CH, Nordgaard, J. Mental illness among relatives of successful academics: implications for psychopathology-creativity research. World Psychiatry 2019; 18: 362–3.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.