Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:56:41.011Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

High intelligence and the risk of ADHD and other psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Nanda Rommelse*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, and Karakter, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Kevin Antshel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Syracuse University, New York, USA
Stijn Smeets
Affiliation:
Center for Giftedness Research, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands, and Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Corina Greven
Affiliation:
Karakter, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands, Radboud University Medical Center, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Nijmegen, The Netherlands, and King's College London, Medical Research Council Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry, London, UK
Lianne Hoogeveen
Affiliation:
Centre for Giftedness Research, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Stephen V. Faraone
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, State University of New York Upstate Medical University, New York, USA, and K. G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
Catharina A. Hartman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Interdisciplinary Centre of Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation and Research, School of Behavioural and Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Groningen, University Medical Centre Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
*
Dr N. Rommelse, Radboud University Medical Centre, Department of Psychiatry, Reinier Postlaan 10, 6525 GC Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

High intelligence may be associated with positive (adaptive, desired) outcomes, but may also come with disadvantages.

Aims

To contribute empirically to the debate concerning whether a trade-off in IQ scores exists in relation to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and related problems, suggesting that high intelligence – like low intelligence –increases the risk of ADHD.

Method

Curves of the relation between IQ score and ADHD problems were fitted to questionnaire data (parent, teacher, self-report) in a population-based study of 2221 children and adolescents aged 10–12 years. Externalising and internalising problems were included for comparison purposes.

Results

Higher IQ score was most strongly related to fewer attention problems, with more rater discrepancy in the high v. average IQ range. Attention problems – but only minimally hyperactivity/impulsivity problems – predicted functional impairment at school, also in the higher IQ range.

Conclusions

Attention problems in highly intelligent children are exceptional and affect school performance; they are therefore a reason for clinical concern.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2017 

Whether high intelligence represents positive (adaptive, desired) outcomes or may come with disadvantages has been a topic of debate. Reference Gale, Batty, Mcintosh, Porteous, Deary and Rasmussen1Reference Vreeker, Boks, Abramovic, Verkooijen, van Bergen and Hillegers6 On one hand high IQ scores have been associated with excellent behavioural, cognitive and emotional control, coupled with good academic performance, along with better outcomes as regards education, occupation, mental and physical health and mortality (see review by Plomin & Deary). Reference Plomin and Deary5 On the other hand, several studies have shown that high intelligence scores and/or superior performance in (high) school predispose towards developing bipolar disorder, psychosis and/or schizophrenia. Reference Gale, Batty, Mcintosh, Porteous, Deary and Rasmussen1,Reference Karlsson2,Reference MacCabe, Lambe, Cnattingius, Sham, David and Reichenberg4,Reference Vreeker, Boks, Abramovic, Verkooijen, van Bergen and Hillegers6,Reference Tiihonen, Haukka, Henriksson, cannon, Kieseppa and Laaksonen7 It is also clear that a high IQ score by itself is not sufficient to result in high educational and occupational achievement, Reference Sternberg, Grigorenko and Bundy8 suggesting that other factors may undermine the strong positive effect of high intelligence on achievement. From an evolutionary perspective, scoring in the average range of the intelligence continuum may represent a favourable trade-off between advantages and disadvantages of extreme polygenic liabilities, as has been shown for personality traits. Reference Nettle9 Whether such a U-curve exists for IQ scores in relation to problems of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (i.e. both low and high intelligence scores are related to increased levels of ADHD problems) has been vigorously debated but not systematically studied. Some argue that high intelligence (like low intelligence) is inherently related to characteristics indicative of ADHD, such as high activity levels, low impulse control, frustration and boredom, and poor attention span. Reference Budding and Chidekel10Reference Webb, Amend, Webb, Goerss, Beljan and Olenchak12 However, it has also been stated that these problems diverge from those seen in individuals with ADHD of below-average intelligence, by being specific to situations that evoke boredom and frustration rather than pervasive. This may occur in situations of poor matching of school tasks to the student's ability level. This raises the question of whether ADHD problems in highly intelligent students are less predictive of functional impairment (since they depend on the student's specific circumstances) than ADHD problems in students with average IQ scores. Reference Webb, Amend, Webb, Goerss, Beljan and Olenchak12 Conversely, the hypothesis that ADHD is underdiagnosed in highly intelligent individuals derives from the idea that high intellect may mask ADHD problems and associated cognitive deficits, Reference Milioni, Chaim, Cavallet, de Oliveira, Annes and dos Santos13,Reference Moon, Niehart, Reis, Robinson and Moon14 although these nonetheless exert their negative effects on functioning by causing performance at a level below the cognitive abilities of these individuals. Currently, this debate is fuelled by studies that suffer from severe selection biases by including only participants enrolled in special education programmes for highly intelligent students, using various cut-off scores for high intelligence (ranging mostly from 0.1% to 7.5% of the population, with some studies stretching this to even 16%), having a small sample size and/or using invalid measures for assessing ADHD (see for instance Alloway & Elsworth and Richards et al, reviewed by Rommelse et al). Reference Alloway and Elsworth15,Reference Rommelse, van der Kruijs, Damhuis, Hoek, Smeets and Antshel17 Our aim therefore was to address the following questions in a large, unbiased population-based study of children and adolescents aged 10–12 years. First, are high IQ scores – as are low scores – related to increased levels of ADHD and other problems compared with average intelligence? Second, are ADHD symptoms and externalising and internalising problems less pervasive across situations in children with high intelligence scores compared with children with average scores? Third, do ADHD problems in highly intelligent individuals predict functional school impairment to the same degree as in individuals with average IQ scores?

Method

This study is part of the Tracking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS), a Dutch general population cohort study following 2230 children from early adolescence into young adulthood. Participants were recruited from the general population in five municipalities in the northern part of The Netherlands, including both urban and rural areas. All children living in these municipalities and born between October 1989 and September 1990 (two sites) and October 1990 and September 1991 (three sites) were selected (n = 3483). Their date of birth and contact information were obtained through the municipality administrations. The first exclusion criterion was non-participation by the school (9.6% of schools, 338 children). If the school of a selected child was willing to participate, parents were approached with information brochures (one for themselves and one for their children) and a follow-up telephone call in which they were invited to participate. Children were excluded from the study if they were incapable of participating because of intellectual disability or a serious physical illness or handicap; or if no Dutch-speaking parent or parent surrogate was available and it was not feasible to administer part of the measurements in the parent's language (n = 210). Of the 3483 selected children 2935 were eligible for the study, of whom 2230 participated (age range 10–12 years). For the current study 2221 (99.6%) children were included for whom intelligence data were available, of whom 39 (1.8%) were treated with methylphenidate. Further details of TRAILS have been described elsewhere. Reference De Winter, Oldehinkel, Veenstra, Brunnekreef, Verhulst and Ormel18,Reference Huisman, oldehinkel, de Winter, Minderaa, de Bildt and Huizink19 Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their parents. The study was approved by the Dutch Central Medical Ethics Committee and all participants were compensated for their involvement in this study.

Measures

Full-scale IQ score was estimated by combining scores on the two subtests of the Revised Wechsler Intelligence Scales (WISC-R) that show the highest correlation (r = 0.90) with full-scale IQ: Vocabulary and Block Design. Reference Van Haasen, De Bruyn, Pijl, Poortinga, Lutje Spelberg and vander steene20,Reference wechsler21 For the assessment of ADHD and comorbid problems we used the Dutch translations of the parent-reported Child Behavior Checklist for school-age children (CBCL/6–18), the Youth Self-Report and a short version of the Teacher Report Form. Reference De Winter, Oldehinkel, Veenstra, Brunnekreef, Verhulst and Ormel18,Reference Sattler22,Reference Achenbach and Rescorla23 All questionnaires have been found valid and reliable. Reference De Winter, Oldehinkel, Veenstra, Brunnekreef, Verhulst and Ormel18 For the analyses in this review the two DSM-IV sub-dimensions for ADHD were used (attention problems and hyperactivity-impulsivity) as well as the two broad-band scores ‘internalising’ (based on three narrow-band scores: anxious/depressed, somatic complaints, withdrawn/depressed) and ‘externalising’ (based on two narrow-band scores: aggressive behaviour and delinquent behaviour). For all scales the raw mean scores (range 0–2) were used for further analyses. In addition, three measures indicative of functional impairment at school were assessed: school progress (parental report: −1 repeated grade or special education for learning disability, 0 regular, +1 skipped a grade), additional telephone calls because of problems at school (parental report: 0 no, 1 yes) and performance below own capacity (teacher report: 0 no, 1 yes). Socioeconomic status (SES) was based on ratings of occupation and education of both mother and father, as well as income; Z-scores of all five components were calculated and categorised as low (lowest 25%), medium (mid 50%) and high (upper 25%) SES. Ethnicity was parent-reported and dichotomised as Dutch or non-Dutch.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were carried out in SPSS Statistics version 23. The proportion of missing data was below 8% for all variables except teacher ratings (13.5%) and data were not replaced. Main analyses used IQ score as a continuous variable. Post hoc analyses and visualisation of results were performed with IQ-based groups: 55–70 (n = 65), 70–85 (n = 318), 85–100 (n = 805), 100–115 (n = 721), 115–130 (n = 268), 130–145 (n = 39). Five children with an IQ score of 45–55 were excluded from the post hoc tests since they formed too small a group. First, to test whether intelligence was related to increased levels of ADHD and other problems, curves were fitted on the data including linear (Y = b0+(b1*IQ)), inverse (Y = b0+(b1/IQ)), quadratic (Y = b0+(b1*IQ)+ (b2*IQ**2)) and cubic (Y = b0+(b1*IQ)+(b2*IQ**2)+(b3*IQ**3)) terms of IQ as predictor and the questionnaire scores as outcomes (Y). The best-fitting model was selected based on the largest F and R 2 values. Analyses were performed separately for different raters. In the second stage analyses were repeated using the difference score per symptom domain between teacher and parent as dependent measures to examine if rater discrepancy – reflective of situational (non-)pervasiveness of problems – was related to IQ score. In addition, to examine if rater agreement was influenced by IQ, both parent and parent × IQ scores were regressed onto teacher scores. In a final step, it was examined whether ADHD (and other) problems had a differential effect on functional impairment at school depending on IQ score. For this purpose, logistic regression analyses were performed on the three school impairment measures with the main effects (IQ score and symptom domains) and interaction effect (IQ score × symptom domains) included as predictors. Sensitivity analyses were conducted performing analyses separately for girls and boys, and excluding the most extreme IQ scores (plus or minus 2 standard deviations; IQ < 70 and IQ > 130) to test whether results were strongly influenced by the extreme ends of the IQ curve. All analyses were corrected for multiple testing (false discovery rate, α = 0.05). Reference Benjamini and Hochberg24

Results

Demographic and clinical descriptors of the sample are given in Table 1. Estimated IQ score was normally distributed (skewness −0.02, kurtosis 0.03) with a mean of 97.2 (s.d. = 15.0, range 45–149). Scores differed as a function of gender (t = 2.93, P < 0.001), with boys having an overall higher estimated IQ than girls (mean 98.1 v. 96.3). Boys were significantly overrepresented in the highest IQ group (IQ 130+, P = 0.025). Scores were significantly yet weakly associated with age: r = −0.08, P < 0.001. Post hoc tests indicated that the two lowest IQ groups (scores 55–69 and 70–84) were slightly older (1.2 months) than the other IQ groups (P values <0.001 to 0.05). The IQ score was related to SES: F (2,2178) = 159.99, P < 0.001. Low SES was overrepresented in the two lowest IQ groups (P < 0.001), whereas high SES was over-represented in the two highest IQ groups (scores 115–129 and 130+; P < 0.001) and middle SES overrepresented in the two average IQ groups (scores 85–99 and 100–114; P < 0.001). Scores were also related to ethnicity (F (1,2219) = 54.57, P < 0.001): non-Dutch ethnicity was relatively overrepresented in the three lowest IQ groups (scores 55–69, 70–84 and 85–99; P < 0.001) and underrepresented in the three higher IQ groups (scores 100–114, 115–129 and 130+; P <0.001). Scores were significantly related to functional impairment at school (school progress: F (2,2218) = 194.84, P < 0.001; parents receiving telephone call because of problems: F (1,2114) = 33.82, P < 0.001; performing below own capacity: F (1,1909) = 20.95, P < 0.001). On all three indices the two lowest IQ groups (scores 55–69 and 70–84) were more impaired compared with the full sample mean (all P values <0.001) and the two highest groups (scores 115–129 and 130+) were least impaired (all P values <0.001).

Table 1 IQ scores in relation to gender, age, socioeconomic status, ethnicity and functional impairment at school

IQ score
Full sample a 55–69 70–84 85–99 100–114 115–129 130+ Test statistics b
n 2230 65 318 805 721 268 39
Gender, % female 50.8 58.5 54.1 52.4 50.2 45.1 30.8 t = 2.93, P < 0.001
Age, years: mean (s.d.) 11.1 (0.56) 11.3 (0.59) 11.2 (0.56) 11.1 (0.54) 11.1 (0.56) 11.1 (0.56) 11.1 (0.51) r = −0.08, P < 0.001
Ethnicity, % Dutch 86.5 72.3 78.3 84.5 91.4 92.9 89.7 F(1,2219) = 54.57, P < 0.001
SES, % F(2,2178) = 159.99, P < 0.001
    Low 25.3 57.8 48.1 26.9 17.2 7.9 10.3
    Middle 49.5 28.1 42.0 55.5 51.1 43.6 41.0
    High 25.2 14.1 9.9 17.5 31.6 48.5 48.7
School progress, % F(2,2218) = 194.84, P < 0.001
    Regular 75.4 24.6 54.7 72.9 86.5 89.9 89.7
    Repeated grade or
    special education for
    intellectual disability
22.5 75.4 44.7 25.6 11.8 4.1 2.6
    Skipped a grade 2.2 0 0.6 1.5 1.7 6.0 7.7
Parents received
telephone call because of
problems at school, %
17.0 27.0 24.4 17.3 15.1 10.5 5.4 F(1,2114) = 33.82, P < 0.001
Performs below capacity
according to teacher, %
20.2 33.3 23.8 21.3 18.6 13.7 12.5 F(1,909) = 20.95, P < 0.001

SES, socioeconomic status.

a. Full sample includes 5 children with IQ scores of 45–51 who formed too small a group for illustrating group means and s.d.s for psychopathology measures.

b. Analyses performed with estimated IQ score (Vocabulary and Block Design) as continuously distributed predictor.

Relation of IQ to symptoms

Results of analyses of IQ scores in relation to ADHD, externalising and internalising symptom levels are shown in Fig. 1 and online Table DS1. Effects were similar for boys and girls and similar when data for children with IQ scores below 70 or above 130 were excluded from the analyses. Curve estimations showed that of all symptom domains IQ score related most strongly and linearly to attention problems and hyperactivity-impulsivity problems according to parents and teachers, indicating that higher IQ score was related to lower levels of attention problems (4.1% and 8.5% explained variance in parent and teacher ratings, respectively) and hyperactivity-impulsivity problems (3.8% and 2.9% explained variance in parent and teacher ratings respectively). A similar linear (but weaker) association was found between IQ score and externalising problems according to both raters (explaining 3.1% and 2.4% of the variance in parent and teacher ratings respectively) and only teacher ratings of internalising problems related to estimated intelligence (2.0% explained variance) with lower levels of problems in the higher IQ range. No relationship between intelligence and self-reported symptom levels was found.

Fig. 1 Problems rated by (a) parents, (b) teachers and (c) self in relation to estimated IQ score. Problem scores are means plus or minus 2 standard deviations.

Rater discrepancies

Results of analyses of IQ scores in relation to rater discrepancy of ADHD, externalising and internalising symptom levels are shown in Fig. 2 and online Table DS2. Effects were similar when children with scores below 70 or above 130 were excluded from the analyses. Scores had the strongest effect on the rater discrepancy for attention problems (explaining 2.2% of the discrepancy), with less rater discrepancy in the middle IQ range than at the lower and higher ends. The effect remained when controlling for the mean level of attention problems, indicating this was not an artefact of the mean level of scores. Parents reported more attention problems than did teachers in children with high estimated IQ scores, whereas the reverse was found for children with low estimated scores. No such effect was found for rater discrepancy for hyperactivity-impulsivity problems: regardless of IQ score parents reported higher levels of these problems than did teachers and this difference was similar across the IQ range. The scores had no effect on rater discrepancy for externalising problems in girls but did in boys, with less rater discrepancy in the average to higher IQ spectrum than in the lower IQ spectrum (teachers rating more externalising problems than parents). There was a small effect of IQ on rater discrepancy for internalising problems, with less discrepancy in parent-teacher ratings in children with higher IQ scores, explaining 0.5–1.15% of the variance.

Fig. 2 Relation of IQ scores to rater discrepancy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), externalising and internalising symptom scores. Differences are means plus or minus 2 standard deviations.

Parent-teacher correlations were not similar across the IQ range for all problem domains, and were most strongly discrepant for attention problems (online Table DS2). Agreement for attention problems was highest in the middle IQ range as opposed to the low and high ends. Parent-teacher correlations for hyperactivity-impulsivity were also not constant across the IQ range, but no clear pattern of agreement along the IQ continuum emerged. Parent-teacher correlations for externalising problems were highest at the low end of the IQ range, whereas parent-teacher correlations for internalising problems were highest in the mid to high end of the IQ range.

Functional impairment

Potential differential effects of ADHD and other problems on functional impairment at school depending on IQ score are shown in Fig. 3 and online Table DS3. As described in Table 1, IQ score significantly related to all three functional impairment measures regarding school performance. In the logistic regression analyses with parent, teacher and self-reported problems (separate analyses), attention problems were the most common predictor of functional impairment at school according to parents and teachers. Attention problems negatively predicted school progress (according to parents and teachers), extra telephone calls because of problems at school (according to parents and teachers) and performance below own capacity (parents, teachers and self-report). Effects were stronger for girls than for boys according to teacher reports. Performing below own capacity and receiving extra telephone calls as a result of attention problems was moderated by IQ score according to teachers. Post hoc analyses indicated that teacher-rated attention problems predicted performing below own capacity most strongly in the average to higher IQ groups (score 70–84 t = 6.69, P < 0.001; score 85–99 t = 11.01, P < 0.001; score 100–114 t = 12.50, P < 0.001; score 115–129 t = 8.14, P < 0.001; score 130+ contained too few participants with teacher-rated suspected performance below own capacity for post hoc analyses) but less so in the below-average IQ group (score 55–69, t = 2.54, P = 0.02) (Fig. 3). Similar findings were found for extra telephone calls. Problems of hyperactivity-impulsivity did not robustly predict functional impairment at school across raters and impairment indices. One effect was found, with higher levels of parent-rated problems independent of IQ predicting worse school progress. Externalising problems were an important reason for functional impairment at school according to all three raters independent of IQ, predicting extra telephone calls (parent, teacher, self) and performance below own capacity (teacher, self). Internalising problems were the reason for extra telephone calls (according to parents) and performance below own capacity (self) independent of IQ score. Main results were similar when children with scores below 70 or above 130 were excluded from the analyses.

Fig. 3 Effect of teacher-reported attention problems on functional impairment at school for children categorised by IQ score range. Graph indicates whether child performs below own capacity according to teachers: yes, solid line; no, dashed line. Problem scores are means plus or minus 2 standard deviations.

Discussion

This study aimed to contribute empirically to the debate about whether an IQ score trade-off exists in relation to ADHD and related problems by examining whether a high score is related to increased levels of ADHD and other problems, less pervasiveness of problems across situations and less associated functional impairment. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first large, general population-based study employing valid assessment tools for IQ and psychopathology without applying arbitrary cut-offs for both to address this issue. With regard to the first question, of all symptom domains measured IQ score was most strongly and linearly related to attention problems (4.1% explained variance in parent ratings, 8.5% in teacher ratings), with higher scores related to less attention problems compared with lower scores. Similarly (but only according to teachers), smaller effects were found for hyperactivity-impulsivity problems and externalising problems, with lower levels of problems in the high average to high end of the IQ continuum. There was no relationship between IQ score and self-reported problems and only minimally with internalising problems. Thus, results indicate that high IQ scores are related to decreased levels of ADHD problems, predominantly in relation to attention problems. With regard to pervasiveness, of all symptom domains measured IQ score had the strongest influence on rater discrepancy (mean difference in scores) and rater disagreement (correlation between ratings) for attention problems, with more discrepancy and disagreement at the low and high ends of the IQ range v. the mid-range. Parents reported more attention problems than did teachers in children with high estimated IQ scores, whereas the reverse was found for children with low estimated IQ. No such effect was found for rater discrepancy of hyperactivity-impulsivity problems. This suggests that attention problems in highly intelligent children are expressed more and/or noticed more at home than at school compared with children of average intelligence (i.e. less situational pervasiveness) or that attention problems are less easily noticed in these children at school. Finally, with regard to functional impairment, attention problems (followed by externalising problems) were of all symptom domains the strongest predictor of repeating grades and suspected performance below own capacity. Conversely, hyperactivity-impulsivity problems were minimally predictive of functional impairment. Teacher-rated attention problems predicted functional impairments also in the higher IQ range (inattention had functional consequences regardless of IQ level), arguing against the hypothesis that attention problems have fewer functional consequences in highly intelligent children.

Attention v. hyperactivity/impulsivity problems

These results argue against the hypothesis that higher IQ scores may increase the risk of ADHD-like or other problems, as has been argued on the basis mostly of clinical impressions rather than empirical studies. Reference Budding and Chidekel10Reference Webb, Amend, Webb, Goerss, Beljan and Olenchak12 Both ADHD symptom dimensions, as well as externalising and internalising problems, were lower at the high end of the IQ continuum. These data suggest that this holds more true for attention problems than for problems of hyperactivity-impulsivity. This is in line with previous studies showing that high intelligence scores are particularly related to lower attention problems, Reference Alloway and Elsworth15,Reference Richards, Encel and Shute16,Reference Bracken and Brown25,Reference Gere, Capps, Mitchell and Grubbs26 and potentially to a lesser degree to hyperactivity-impulsivity problems. Reference Alloway and Elsworth15,Reference Ackerman27,Reference Bouchet and Falk28 Moreover, teacher-rated attention problems predicted performance below the child's own capacity also at the higher end of the IQ continuum, suggesting these problems are not merely harmless ‘mimics’ of ADHD. The current and previous studies suggest that high levels of psychopathological symptoms (particularly attention problems) are actually more relatively deviant in highly intelligent individuals than in individuals of below-average intelligence, considering the mean level of attention problems in that IQ range.

Pervasiveness of ADHD problems in children with high IQ

A widely held assumption is that ‘ADHD-mimicking’ traits in highly intelligent individuals can be differentiated from ‘true’ symptoms indicative of ADHD by a lack of situational pervasiveness. That is, these traits are thought to be less pervasive across situations in highly intelligent individuals as a consequence of being situationally linked to a mismatch between the child's cognitive ability and offered stimulation. Reference Budding and Chidekel10Reference Webb, Amend, Webb, Goerss, Beljan and Olenchak12 Some support for this view was found. Parent-teacher discrepancy and disagreement were higher in the high IQ range than average IQ range (but note the discrepancy was highest in the lowest IQ range), with more attention problems reported by parents compared with teachers as opposed to similar levels of attention problems in the mid IQ range. It may be proposed that school is a cognitively more stimulating environment than home, evoking less attention problems in highly intelligent children. The argument that attention problems in children with high intelligence may easily go unnoticed by teachers and lead to underachievement seems not to be supported by the data, given that there was a negative linear relationship between IQ score and performing below capacity according to teachers and that there were low levels of attention problems reported by teachers in children with high IQ scores. It is of interest, however, that the findings of parent-teacher rater discrepancy regarding attention problems were not paralleled by rater discrepancies for hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms, which were constant across the IQ range (higher levels reported by parents than teachers). This seems to suggest that situational pervasiveness of hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms is less linked to IQ in comparison with attention problems.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of the study include a large, unselected population-based sample, valid assessments of intelligence and psychopathology, the use of multiple raters (parent, teacher, self), measures of functional impairment at school, and use of IQ scores as a continuous measure instead of working with arbitrary cut-off points (IQ groups were only formed for illustrative purposes and post hoc comparisons). A limitation is the narrow age range of the sample preventing analyses on potential age-moderating effects. In addition, only questionnaire data were used instead of diagnostic interview data. However, parent and teacher ratings revealed similar findings and the use of multi-informant questionnaire data allowed us to test explicitly for pervasiveness of problems across situations. Further, we used only two subtests instead of the full battery to estimate IQ. Given that even full IQ tests are not always reliable representations of intelligence, with the possibility that particularly participants with high levels of ADHD symptoms may obtain lower scores due to their symptoms, replication of the findings is needed. However, using two subsets to estimate full-scale intelligence is commonly done in large epidemiological studies and the currently estimated IQ score appeared to be valid, since it robustly related to school performance and SES. Last, only subtests were used that did not rely on working memory, causing a risk of overestimating the IQ scores of children with high levels of attention problems. However, the inverse relationship between IQ score and attention problems suggest this has not been a substantial confounder.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

S.F has received income, potential income, travel expenses and/or research support from Rhodes, Arbor, Pfizer, Ironshore, Shire, Akili Interactive Labs, CogCubed, Alcobra, VAYA Pharma and NeuroLifeSciences. With his institution he has US patent US20130217707 A1 for the use of sodium-hydrogen exchange inhibitors in the treatment of ADHD.

References

1 Gale, CR, Batty, GD, Mcintosh, AM, Porteous, DJ, Deary, IJ, Rasmussen, F. Is bipolar disorder more common in highly intelligent people? A cohort study of a million men. Mol Psychiatry 2013; 18: 190–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Karlsson, JL. Relation of mathematical ability to psychosis in Iceland. Clin Genet 1999; 56: 447–9.Google Scholar
3 Holahan, CK, Holahan, CJ. Being labeled as gifted, self-appraisal, and psychological well-being: a life span developmental perspective. Int J Aging Hum Dev 1999; 48: 161–73.Google Scholar
4 MacCabe, JH, Lambe, MP, Cnattingius, S, Sham, PC, David, AS, Reichenberg, A, et al. Excellent school performance at age 16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: national cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 196: 109–15.Google Scholar
5 Plomin, R, Deary, IJ. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20: 98108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Vreeker, A, Boks, MP, Abramovic, L, Verkooijen, S, van Bergen, AH, Hillegers, MH, et al. High educational performance is a distinctive feature of bipolar disorder: a study on cognition in bipolar disorder, schizophrenia patients, relatives and controls. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 807–18.Google Scholar
7 Tiihonen, J, Haukka, J, Henriksson, M, cannon, M, Kieseppa, T, Laaksonen, I, et al. Premorbid intellectual functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: results from a cohort study of male conscripts. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 1904–10.Google Scholar
8 Sternberg, RJ, Grigorenko, EL, Bundy, DA. The predictive value of IQ. Merrill Palmer Q 2001; 47: 141.Google Scholar
9 Nettle, D. The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. Am Psychol 2006; 61: 622–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Budding, D, Chidekel, D. ADHD and giftedness: a neurocognitive consideration of twice exceptionality. Appl Neuropsychol Child 2012; 1: 145–51.Google Scholar
11 Lee, KM, Olenchak, FR. Individuals with a gifted/attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder diagnosis: identification, performance, outcomes, and interventions. Gifted Education International 2014; 31: 185–99.Google Scholar
12 Webb, JT, Amend, ER, Webb, NE, Goerss, J, Beljan, P, Olenchak, FR. Misdiagnosis and Dual Diagnoses of Gifted Children and Adults. Great Potential Press, 2005.Google Scholar
13 Milioni, ALV, Chaim, TM, Cavallet, M, de Oliveira, NM, Annes, M, dos Santos, B, et al. High IQ may ‘mask’ the diagnosis of ADHD by compensating for deficits in executive functions in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2017; 21: 455–64.Google Scholar
14 Moon, SM. Gifted children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In The Social and Emotional Development of Gifted Children: What Do We Know? (eds Niehart, M, Reis, S, Robinson, N, Moon, S): 193201. National Association for Gifted Children, 2002.Google Scholar
15 Alloway, TC, Elsworth, M. An investigation of cognitive skills and behavior in high ability students. Learn Individ Differ 2012; 22: 891–5.Google Scholar
16 Richards, J, Encel, J, Shute, R. The emotional and behavioral adjustment of intellectually gifted adolescents: a multi-dimensional, multi-informant approach. High Ability Studies 2003; 14: 153–64.Google Scholar
17 Rommelse, N, van der Kruijs, M, Damhuis, J, Hoek, I, Smeets, S, Antshel, KM, et al. An evidenced-based perspective on the validity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the context of high intelligence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 71: 2147.Google Scholar
18 De Winter, AF, Oldehinkel, AJ, Veenstra, R, Brunnekreef, JA, Verhulst, FC, Ormel, J. Evaluation of non-response bias in mental health determinants and outcomes in a large sample of pre-adolescents. Eur J Epidemiol 2005; 20: 173–81.Google Scholar
19 Huisman, M, oldehinkel, AI, de Winter, A, Minderaa, RB, de Bildt, A, Huizink, AC, et al. Cohort Profile: The Dutch ‘TRacking Adolescents Individual Lives' Survey’. TRAILS. Int J Epidemiol 2008; 37: 1227–35.Google Scholar
20 Van Haasen, PP, De Bruyn, EEJ, Pijl, YL, Poortinga, YH, Lutje Spelberg, HC, vander steene, G, et al. wechsler intelligence Scale for Children – Revised (Dutch edn). Swets ' zeitlinger, 1986.Google Scholar
21 wechsler, D. wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised. Psychological Corporation, 1974.Google Scholar
22 Sattler, JM. Assessment of Children (3rd edn). Jerome M. Sattler, 1992.Google Scholar
23 Achenbach, TM, Rescorla, LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, 2001.Google Scholar
24 Benjamini, Y, Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc Series B 1995; 57: 289300.Google Scholar
25 Bracken, BA, Brown, EF. Behavioral identification and assessment of gifted and talented students. J Psychoeduc Assess 2006; 24: 112–22.Google Scholar
26 Gere, DR, Capps, SC, Mitchell, DW, Grubbs, W. Sensory sensitivities of gifted children. Am J Occup Ther 2009; 64: 288–95.Google Scholar
27 Ackerman, CM. Identifying gifted adolescents using personality characteristics: Dabrowski's overexcitabilities. Roeper Rev 1997; 19: 229–36.Google Scholar
28 Bouchet, N, Falk, RF. The relationship among giftedness, gender, and overexcitability. Gift Child Q 2001; 45: 260–7.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 IQ scores in relation to gender, age, socioeconomic status, ethnicity and functional impairment at school

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Problems rated by (a) parents, (b) teachers and (c) self in relation to estimated IQ score. Problem scores are means plus or minus 2 standard deviations.

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Relation of IQ scores to rater discrepancy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), externalising and internalising symptom scores. Differences are means plus or minus 2 standard deviations.

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Effect of teacher-reported attention problems on functional impairment at school for children categorised by IQ score range. Graph indicates whether child performs below own capacity according to teachers: yes, solid line; no, dashed line. Problem scores are means plus or minus 2 standard deviations.

Supplementary material: PDF

Rommelse et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1-S3

Download Rommelse et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.6 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.