Cognitive and social deficits pre-date the onset of schizophrenia (Reference Isohanni, Lauronen and MoilanenIsohanni et al, 2005; Reference Cannon, Moffitt and CaspiCannon et al, 2006) but it is unclear whether they reflect genetic liability. We examined the early social and academic functioning of individuals from the Maudsley Family Study to determine whether premorbid impairments in these functional domains are related to familial liability for schizophrenia. We hypothesised that individuals with a higher presumed genetic liability (i.e. those from multiply affected families) would display more prominent social and academic impairment than their counterparts from non-affected families.
METHODS
The recruitment and clinical assessments of the sample are described elsewhere (Reference McDonald, Marshall and ShamMcDonald et al, 2006). Briefly, multiply affected families were defined as having two or more first- and/or second-degree relatives with schizophrenia or another psychotic disorder, and singly affected families were those in which the index individual had no known family history of psychosis as far as their third-degree relatives. None of the control sample had a personal or family history of psychotic illness. All participants were White/Caucasian, aged 18–50 years and gave informed consent for their mother to be interviewed. The study had ethics approval.
Fifty people with ‘familial’ schizophrenia (37 male, 13 female; mean age 32 years, s.d.=6.1), 39 of their unaffected siblings (14 male, 25 female; mean age 34 years, s.d.=7.8), 69 people with ‘non-familial’ (52 male, 17 female; mean age 31 years, s.d.=6.4), 67 of their unaffected siblings (34 male, 33 female; mean age 35, s.d.=7.6) and 83 controls (42 male, 41 female; mean age 31 years, s.d.=7.1) were recruited. Patients fulfilled DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for schizophrenia (n=112), schizo-affective disorder (n=6) or psychotic disorder ‘not otherwise specified’ (n=1). Eleven (2 ‘non-familial’ and 9 ‘familial’) unaffected siblings had had an earlier DSM–IV Axis I non-psychotic psychiatric disorder, predominantly major depressive disorder.
A modified Premorbid Social Adjustment (PSA) scale (Reference Cannon-Spoor, Potkin and WyattCannon-Spoor et al, 1982) was used to examine childhood and adolescent functioning (Reference Foerster, Lewis and OwenFoerster et al, 1991; Reference HollisHollis, 2003). The PSA scale assessed five areas (socialisation, peer relations, academic achievement, school adaptation and hobbies) over two consecutive time periods: 5–11 years (childhood) and 12–16 years (adolescence). The PSA scale was then simplified into two categories: social adjustment (socialisation, peer relations and hobbies); and academic adjustment (academic achievement and school adaptation) (Reference Allen, Frantom and StraussAllen et al, 2005). Higher scores indicated poorer functioning. Any developmental deterioration was calculated as a ‘change score’ by subtracting childhood adjustment from adolescent adjustment.
The scale was administered to the mothers by face-to-face interview (64%) or using a self-report questionnaire (36%). Reliability was established by asking 21 mothers who had completed face-to-face interviews to complete a self-report version of the PSA scale at a later time (on average 4 years). The scores for both time-points were highly consistent (correlation coefficient=0.80).
Multivariate analysis was carried out using STATA version 9.0 with clustered robust standard errors to account for the non-independence of individuals within families and for possible violations of normality and equal variance assumptions. Multiple linear regression was used to compare premorbid adjustment and change scores (dependent variables) of each patient and sibling group (independent variables) with the control group, controlling for age and gender. Scores were log-transformed to normalise the distributions. All tests were two-tailed using a 0.05 level of significance.
RESULTS
The groups did not significantly differ in measures of parental social class (χ2=3.5, d.f.=2, P=0.71) or sibship size (F(2,150)=0.4, P=0.65) but there were significant group differences for age (F(4,151)=2.8, P=0.03) and gender (χ2=22.2, d.f.=4, P=0.002), which were controlled for in subsequent analyses. Siblings of people with non-familial psychosis were older than the control group and there was an excess of males in both patient groups.
Premorbid function scores are presented in Table 1. Compared with controls, both groups with schizophrenia had significantly worse social and academic function in childhood and adolescence, both of which deteriorated over time. The deterioration in social functioning only reached statistical significance for people with ‘familial’ schizophrenia. In a post hoc analysis directly comparing ‘familial’ and ‘non-familial’ schizophrenia, no significant difference was found in either premorbid social functioning or deterioration over time.
Familial schizophrenia (n=50) | Sibling - familial schizophrenia (n=39) | Non-familial schizophrenia (n=69) | Sibling - non-familial schizophrenia (n=67) | Controls (n=83) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
PSA scale score: mean (s.d.) | |||||
Childhood social adjustment | 4.08 (1.16) * | 3.43 (0.64) | 4.33 (1.35) *** | 3.60 (0.97) | 3.52 (0.80) |
Adolescent social adjustment | 4.36 (1.26) *** | 3.51 (0.90) | 4.43 (1.47) *** | 3.39 (0.72) | 3.47 (0.75) |
Change score for social adjustment | 0.28 (0.73) * | 0.05 (0.70) | 0.10 (0.99) | -0.21 (0.69) | -0.05 (0.62) |
Childhood academic adjustment | 3.08 (1.10) ** | 2.51 (0.68) | 3.07 (1.14) *** | 2.58 (0.86) | 2.36 (0.58) |
Adolescent academic adjustment | 3.46 (1.42) *** | 2.84 (0.96) ** | 3.49 (1.32) *** | 2.60 (0.87) | 2.41 (0.66) |
Change score for academic adjustment | 0.38 (0.97) * | 0.33 (0.74) ** | 0.42 (0.99) ** | 0.01 (0.71) | 0.04 (0.44) |
* P<0.05
** P<0.01
*** P<0.001 v. controls in linear regression analysis
Neither unaffected sibling group differed significantly from controls in their social functioning during childhood or adolescence (Table 1). However, siblings of people with familial schizophrenia demonstrated significantly worse academic functioning than controls during adolescence, and had a deterioration in academic functioning between childhood and adolescence. Post hoc analysis demonstrated that siblings of people with familial schizophrenia also had a significantly greater decline in academic functioning when compared directly with ‘non-familial’ siblings (B=–0.31, P=0.02, 95% CI–0.56 to –0.06). Analyses were repeated excluding those 11 unaffected siblings with a history of non-psychotic psychiatric disorders but this made no difference to the results.
DISCUSSION
To our knowledge this is the first study demonstrating that unaffected siblings of people with familial schizophrenia have poor academic functioning during adolescence and deterioration in academic performance between childhood and adolescence compared with controls. This finding, coupled with its absence in siblings of people with non-familial schizophrenia suggests that academic problems may be related to genetic risk for schizophrenia. This is consistent with reports that adult relatives of people with schizophrenia underperform on cognitive tests compared with controls (Reference Snitz, MacDonald and CarterSnitz et al, 2006).
This study has some methodological limitations. Separating families on the basis of family history of psychosis runs the risk that some families may be misclassified. People with non-familial schizophrenia may not represent illness phenocopies but multiply affected families are presumed more likely to carry a greater genetic susceptibility load than those families with only one member affected. This is supported by studies which have found more prominent neurobiological deviations among unaffected relatives from more densely affected families (Reference McDonald, Marshall and ShamMcDonald et al, 2006). It is also possible that recall bias was introduced by the retrospective assessment of behavioural functioning during childhood and adolescence, i.e. that mothers from multiply affected families were more likely to recall negative events in their children. However, maternal ratings across both patient groups (familial and non-familial) were very similar, arguing against such recall bias operating in multiply affected families.
Our finding that people who go on to develop schizophrenia have abnormal premorbid social and academic functioning in childhood and adolescence is in accordance with previous research (e.g. Reference Isohanni, Jones and KemppainenIsohanni et al, 2000), and other studies (Reference Allen, Frantom and StraussAllen et al, 2005) have suggested that academic and social impairment accelerates as people who later develop schizophrenia move from childhood to adolescence.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.