Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T18:32:40.243Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comorbid substance use and age at onset of schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Thomas R. E. Barnes*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College Faculty of Medicine, London
Stanley H. Mutsatsa
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College Faculty of Medicine, London
Sam B. Hutton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton
Hilary C. Watt
Affiliation:
Medical Statistics Unit, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London
Eileen M. Joyce
Affiliation:
Institute of Neurology, University College London, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, London, UK
*
Professor Thomas Barnes, Department of Psychological Medicine, Imperial College Faculty of Medicine, Charing Cross Campus, St Dunstan's Road, London W6 8RPUK. e-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Substance use may be a risk factor for the onset of schizophrenia.

Aims

To examine the association between substance use and age at onset in a UK, inner-city sample of people with recent-onset schizophrenia.

Method

The study sample consisted of 152 people recruited to the West London First-Episode Schizophrenia Study. Self-reported data on drug and alcohol use, as well as information on age at onset of psychosis, were collected. Mental state, cognition (IQ, memory and executive function) and social function were also assessed.

Results

In total, 60% of the participants were smokers, 27% reported a history of problems with alcohol use, 35% reported current substance use (not including alcohol), and 68% reported lifetime substance use (cannabis and psychostimulants were most commonly used). Cannabis use and gender had independent effects on age at onset of psychosis, after adjusting for alcohol misuse and use of other drugs.

Conclusions

The strong association between self-reported cannabis use and earlier onset of psychosis provides further evidence that schizophrenia may be precipitated by cannabis use and/or that the early onset of symptoms is a risk factor for cannabis use.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2006 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Substance use in people with schizophrenia is more common than in the general population, and is associated with poor clinical and social outcomes (Reference Blanchard, Brown and HoranBlanchard et al, 2000; Reference Kavanagh, McGrath and SaundersKavanagh et al, 2002). Substance use may also be a risk factor for the onset of the disorder (Reference Smit, Bolier and CuijpersSmit et al, 2004; Reference Semple, McIntosh and LawrieSemple et al, 2005), and the use of cannabis in adolescence has been reported to increase the risk of developing symptoms of schizophrenia in adulthood (Reference Tsapakis, Guillin and MurrayTsapakis et al, 2003). One piece of supportive evidence is that some first-episode studies have found an earlier age at onset for individuals with a history of comorbid substance use (Reference Rabinowitz, Bromet and LavelleRabinowitz et al, 1998; Reference Mastrigt, Addington and AddingtonMastrigt et al, 2004; Reference Veen, Selten and van der TweelVeen et al, 2004), although not all studies have shown this (Reference Cantor-Graee, Nordstr⊘m and McNeilCantor-Graee et al, 2001; Reference Sevy, Robinson and SollowaySevy et al, 2001). The present study was designed to assess the influence of substance use on the age at onset of psychosis in first-episode schizophrenia in West London, and to examine the relationship between a history of comorbid substance use and measures of the nature and severity of symptoms, and social and neuropsychological function.

METHOD

Participants

Individuals were recruited as part of a prospective longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia in West London. Details of the study design and entry criteria can be found elsewhere (Reference Hutton, Puri and DuncanHutton et al, 1998), as can information on symptom severity, adherence to treatment, duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) and cognitive function in samples derived from this study (Reference Barnes, Hutton and ChapmanBarnes et al, 2000; Reference Joyce, Hutton and RobbinsJoyce et al, 2002; Reference Mutsatsa, Joyce and HuttonMutsatsa et al, 2003). The patients who were eligible for inclusion in this study were aged between 16 and 50 years, had received no more than 12 weeks of antipsychotic medication, and in most cases were seen at the time of their first admission to hospital. For each patient, a diagnosis of DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) schizophrenia or schizophreniform disorder was confirmed at diagnostic review by two senior clinicians (E.M.J. and T.R.E.B.).

Assessments

Basic demographic data were collected from the patients and their case notes. Lifetime alcohol and substance use were recorded using the Substance Use Rating Scale, patient version (SURSp; Reference Duke, Pantelis and BarnesDuke et al, 1994). This scale allows the identification of patients with drinking problems, and elicits any evidence of alcohol dependency, as well as collecting information on the nature of current drug use (within the past month) and previous drug use.

Information relating to the date of onset of psychotic symptoms was elicited using a modified questionnaire (Reference Loebel, Lieberman and AlvirLoebel et al, 1992; Reference Barnes, Hutton and ChapmanBarnes et al, 2000). Relevant information was also collected from the clinical case notes, and by questioning the patient and their relatives and/or carers. Any discrepancy between the different sources was discussed within the research team, and the information that was deemed to be most reliable was used. The DUP, namely the time from onset of psychotic symptoms to first treatment with antipsychotic medication, was calculated for each patient.

Global severity of illness was assessed using the Clinical Global Impression Scale (CGI; Reference GuyGuy, 1976). Mental state was assessed using the Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms and the Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SAPS and SANS respectively; Reference Andreasen and AndreasenAndreasen, 1990). Scores for three symptom-derived syndromes of schizophrenia were calculated for each patient by adding together the global sub-scale scores pertaining to each factor, and dividing by the maximum possible score to give a value between 0 and 1 for each factor. The syndromes were categorised as follows: positive syndrome (SAPS hallucinations and delusions); disorganisation syndrome (SAPS bizarre behaviour and positive formal thought disorder); and negative syndrome (all SANS sub-scales). The Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale (CPRS; Reference Asberg, Montgomery and PerrisÅsberg et al, 1978) was also administered, from which a score for the 10-item Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS; Reference Montgomery and AsbergMontgomery & Åsberg, 1979) was calculated.

Social function was assessed using the Social Function Scale (SFS; Reference Birchwood, Smith and CochraneBirchwood et al, 1990), which asks individuals to rate their abilities in seven areas of social function, namely activation–engagement, interpersonal communication, frequency and competence with regard to activities of daily living, participation in social activities, participation in recreational activities, and employment or occupational activity. The Rating of Medication Influences (ROMI; Reference Weiden, Rapkin and MottWeiden et al, 1994) was used to assess positive and negative attitudes towards medication.

Premorbid IQ was estimated using the National Adult Reading Test (NART, revised version; Reference NelsonNelson, 1991), and current, full-scale IQ was assessed using a short form of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (WAIS–R; Reference Missar, Gold and GoldbergMissar et al, 1994) with four sub-tests. The main measures of neurocognitive function were the memory and executive function tests from the computerised Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB; Reference Sahakian and OwenSahakian & Owen, 1992), run on an IBM-compatible PC with a touch-sensitive screen. The memory tests assessed spatial span and pattern recognition memory, and executive function was assessed by means of a planning task (a modified version of the Tower of London test; Reference ShalliceShallice, 1982) and a test of spatial working memory. We have described these tests, the performance measures used and the derivation of the test scores in our patients elsewhere (Reference Hutton, Puri and DuncanHutton et al, 1998).

Data analysis

The data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 12 for Windows. In a comparison of patients with and without a history of substance use, the t-test was employed for continuous data and the χ2 test was used for categorical data. Age at onset of psychosis was used as the outcome variable in a linear regression model, and potentially influential variables (relating to gender and substance use) were entered as independent variables.

RESULTS

Information on lifetime substance use was collected at interview for 152 patients (110 men and 42 women) with DSM–IV schizophrenia or schizophreniform disorder, who were studied at the time of their first presentation to psychiatric services, but the data were incomplete in 6 cases. On the basis of these data, which were obtained using the SURSp, the sample was divided into two subgroups, namely those reporting a history of alcohol misuse (problem drinking, with or without evidence of dependent drinking) and/or substance use (n=110), and those with no such history (n=42). In the former group, most patients described both alcohol misuse and substance use, and only 5 patients reported current or lifetime problem drinking or evidence of dependent drinking in the absence of any other substance use. The clinical characteristics of the two subgroups are listed in Table 1. Further sub-categorisation of the sample on the basis of the reported profile of substances used was not possible because of the overlap in substances used. For example, 90% of the patients with a history of any type of substance use reported the use of cannabis.

Table 1 Clinical characteristics of patients with and without a lifetime history of substance use

Variable No lifetime history of substance use (n=42) Any lifetime history of substance use (n=110) Test statistic P
t χ2
Male/female ratio, n 26/16 84/26 3.17 0.075
Age at first psychiatric contact, years: mean (s.d.) 27.8 (10.4) 24.4 (6.1) 2.54 0.012
Age at onset of psychosis, years: mean (s.d.) 26.1 (9.2) 23.3 (5.9) 2.14 0.033
Duration of untreated psychosis, weeks: mean (s.d.) 51.9 (83.7) 56.4 (83.1) -0.29 0.77
Employed (or student), % 41 37 0.29 0.59

For the total sample, the median DUP was 24 weeks. The mean DUP did not differ significantly between those with and without substance use (see Table 1). However, the patients with a reported lifetime history of substance use were significantly younger at the time of the assessment (t=2.45, d.f.=148, P=0.012), and were also significantly younger at the onset of their psychotic symptoms (t=2.15, d.f.=148, P=0.033). Linear regression analysis was performed with age at onset as the outcome variable and with the following four independent variables: gender; any report of alcohol misuse (problem or dependent drinking); any report of cannabis use; and any report of substance use other than cannabis use. Alcohol misuse and any substance use (other than cannabis use) were not significant in relation to age at onset (estimated mean age 1.6 years older in patients reporting alcohol misuse, 95% CI –1.1 to 4.2, P=0.2; estimated mean age 1.9 years older in users of drugs other than cannabis, 95% CI –0.9 to 4.9, P=0.2). However, gender and cannabis use were found to be significant. The results indicate that in this sample the age at onset of psychosis was on average 4.2 years older for women (95% CI 1.7–6.8, P=0.001) when adjusted for substance use. Cannabis use was significantly associated with a younger onset by a mean value of 5 years (95% CI 1.9–8.1, P=0.002), adjusted for gender and substance use other than cannabis use. This means that those patients in the sample who reported that they had used cannabis had an earlier age at onset of psychosis than other patients who did not report cannabis use but who shared the same profile with regard to the other variables (e.g. comparing men who reported alcohol misuse and use of both cannabis and other drugs with men who had the same characteristics apart from the fact that they had not used cannabis).

We have reported elsewhere (Reference Joyce, Hutton and MutsatsaJoyce et al, 2005) that premorbid IQ (measured by the NART) was associated with earlier onset of psychosis in a sample that was also derived from the West London First-Episode Schizophrenia Study. The NART score was entered as an additional independent variable in the present linear regression analysis, and it was found to be significant, whereas cannabis use remained significant (an estimated increase of 0.24 years in age at onset per unit increase in NART; 95% CI 0.12–0.37, P<0.0002).

Data on the nature and prevalence of alcohol and drug use in those participants who provided a full report are shown in Table 2. Cannabis was the substance most commonly used. In total, 94 patients in the sample had used this drug at some time, of whom only 25 patients (27%) had not used other drugs. The other drugs that had been used, in descending order of reported use, were amphetamines and stimulants (including cocaine), ecstasy, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), opiates (including heroin), barbiturates and benzodiazepines, other substances (e.g. khat) and phencyclidine (PCP). With regard to alcohol misuse, 40 patients reported a history of problem drinking, of whom 29 patients (72.5%) fulfilled the criteria for dependent drinking.

Table 2 Nature and frequency of alcohol and substance use in the total sample (n=148) and in the subsample of patients who reported lifetime alcohol and/or substance use (n=106)

Total sample (%) Subsample alcohol and/or substance use (%)
Smoker 60.1 68.9
Alcohol use
    Problem drinking +evidence of dependence 27.0 37.8
19.6 27.4
Substance use
    In previous month 34.7 48.6
    Lifetime 68.2 95.3
Lifetime use of drugs1
    Cannabis 64.4 90.4
    Stimulants2 37.7 52.9
    Ecstasy 32.9 46.1
    Hallucinogens 30.8 43.3
    Opiates/heroin 11.6 16.3
    Barbiturates/benzodiazepines 8.9 12.5
    Other (including khat) 5.4 7.7
    PCP (angel dust) 4.8 6.7

In Table 3, data for the two patient subgroups are compared with regard to the clinical and neuropsychological assessments that were administered. The patients who were substance users had a significantly higher mean CGI score (t=–2.99, d.f.=138, P=0.003) and there was a trend towards a higher mean score on the positive symptoms syndrome (hallucinations and delusions) (t=–1.85, d.f.=150, P=0.066). There was also a trend towards an association of substance use with male gender (χ2=3.18, d.f.=1, P=0.075). There were no statistically significant differences between the two groups with regard to mean SFS total score, mean ROMI positive and negative scores or mean MADRS total score. Neuropsychological test results were only obtained for a sub-sample of the patients (see Table 3). Analysis of the data revealed no significant differences between substance users and non-users on performance measures for any of the neuropsychological tasks.

Table 3 Mental state and neuropsychological assessment variables for patients with and without a lifetime history of substance use

Variable No lifetime history of substance use Any lifetime history of substance use t P
n 42 110
Symptom-based syndrome scores
    Negative symptoms: mean (s.d.) 0.42 (0.29) 0.42 (0.25) -0.03 0.98
    Hallucinations and delusions: mean (s.d.) 0.60 (0.32) 0.73 (0.41) -1.85 0.066
    Disorganisation: mean (s.d.) 0.39 (0.31) 0.43 (0.29) -0.88 0.38
CGI severity score: mean (s.d.) 4.18 (1.3) 4.82 (1.1) -2.99 0.003
MADRS score: mean (s.d.) 17.1 (9.6) 18.1 (10.1) -0.54 0.59
Social Function Scale score: mean (s.d.) 113.1 (12.9) 111.7 (10.2) 0.63 0.53
ROMI score
    Positive score: mean (s.d.) 12.2 (3.7) 12.9 (4.9) -0.67 0.51
    Negative score: mean (s.d.) 14.3 (5.4) 16.1 (6.2) -1.32 0.19
Neuropsychological tests
    NART
        n 29 73
        mean (s.d.) 99.5 (9.6) 99.5 (10.6) -0.01 0.99
    Full-scale IQ:WAIS—R
        n 30 73
        mean (s.d.) 91.9 (13.1) 92.8 (15.5) -0.29 0.77
    Spatial span score
        n 35 84
        mean (s.d.) 5.6 (1.7) 5.5 (1.3) 0.54 0.59
    Pattern recognition memory: number correct (maximum=24) 36 81
    n 20.1 (2.9) 20.2 (3.1) -0.76 0.94
    mean (s.d.)
    Spatial working memory: total number of errors 34 85
37.9 (21.7) 32.0 (20.7) 1.38 0.17
        n
        mean (s.d.) 34 83
    Planning: number of perfect solutions (maximum=12) 7.7 (2.3) 7.3 (2.0) 1.01 0.34
        n
        mean (s.d.)

DISCUSSION

Earlier age at onset

We found an earlier age at onset of psychosis in patients who reported a lifetime history of comorbid substance use. One explanation for this is that the illness is precipitated by substance use, although it remains uncertain whether this effect is limited to people with a predisposition to psychosis (Reference Smit, Bolier and CuijpersSmit et al, 2004; Reference Henquet, Krabbendam and SpauwenHenquet et al, 2005). Another possible explanation is that the early onset of symptoms is a risk factor for substance use. The experience of symptoms could make patients vulnerable to substance use, perhaps in an attempt to cope with the illness or to self-medicate (Reference Hambrecht and HafnerHambrecht & Häfner, 1996; Reference Addington and Adding tonAddington & Addington, 1998; Reference Degenhardt, Hall and LynskeyDegenhardt et al, 2003). These hypotheses have been explored in previous studies by examining the temporal relationship between the onset of schizophrenia and substance use. The findings have been inconsistent (Hambrecht & Häfner, Reference Hambrecht and Hafner1996, Reference Hambrecht and Hafner2000), and have generally only addressed the relationship between substance use and the onset of psychotic symptoms, and not the possible relevance of prodromal symptoms. Furthermore, the relatively high proportion of patients who reported lifetime substance use in this and other first-episode studies raises the possibility that substance-related symptoms could confound retrospective estimation of DUP and age at onset. For example, drug-induced phenomena may be mistaken for early symptoms of illness or substance use may mask psychotic symptoms (Reference Larsen, McGlashan and JohannessenLarsen et al, 2001). In the latter situation, if patients perceive their early psychotic symptoms to be drug-induced, this may delay their seeking help for illness and thus prolong the DUP. Norman & Malla (Reference Norman and Malla2002) proposed more generally that substance use by people with psychosis may partly reflect denial of the severity of their illness and of the potential benefit of medical intervention, and may thus be associated with a reduced likelihood of seeking treatment soon after the onset of psychosis. However, our findings do not support such a view, insofar as we found no relationship between a history of substance use and DUP.

Our data do not allow us to determine whether or not the use of drugs preceded the onset of psychotic and/or prodromal symptoms in the majority of patients. Furthermore, the marked overlap between patients who were using alcohol, cannabis and other drugs limits analysis of the influence of a particular substance. However, the results of the linear regression analysis suggest that cannabis use is more strongly associated with earlier onset of psychosis than either alcohol misuse or other drug use. This finding is consistent with the results of other studies (Reference Smit, Bolier and CuijpersSmit et al, 2004; Reference Veen, Selten and van der TweelVeen et al, 2004) and it supports the view that cannabis use is a risk factor for the onset of psychosis.

Prevalence and nature of substance use in first-episode schizophrenia

The results of this study confirm the high rates of substance use reported previously (particularly for nicotine, alcohol and cannabis) in individuals presenting to psychiatric services with their first episode of schizophrenia (Reference Hambrecht and HafnerHambrecht & Häfner, 1996; Reference Mastrigt, Addington and AddingtonMastrigt et al, 2004). Both the prevalence and the profile of substances used by the participants in our sample are similar to those reported in previous out-patient and community studies of schizophrenia in the UK (e.g. Duke et al, Reference Duke, Pantelis and Barnes1994, Reference Duke, Pantelis and McPhillips2001; Reference Condren, O'Connor and BrowneCondren et al, 2001), with nicotine, alcohol and cannabis being most commonly used, but stimulants and hallucinogens also being popular. When considering the extent to which our findings can be generalised, it might be argued that the prevalence figures for comorbid substance use in patients with schizophrenia in London tend to be higher than those in many other parts of the UK (Reference Weaver, Madden and CharlesWeaver et al, 2003).

The high prevalence of tobacco smoking that was found in our study is consistent with evidence that this habit is particularly strongly associated with schizophrenia compared with other forms of severe mental illness, and that smoking-related diseases are more common in people with schizophrenia than in the general population (Reference DeLeon, Tracy and McCannDeLeon et al, 2002). The reasons why people with schizophrenia tend to be heavy smokers remain uncertain, but possible explanations relate to a therapeutic effect of nicotine on psychotic symptoms and/or the neurocognitive deficits associated with the disorder, or to an increase in the metabolism of antipsychotic drugs with smoking, causing a reduction in the side-effect burden (Reference Jeste, Gladsjo and LindamerJeste et al, 1996). The latter explanation would not appear to be relevant to the patients in our sample, as they were already smoking when they first presented to psychiatric services, which suggests that there is a relationship with the psychotic illness rather than with the medication.

Assessment of substance use by self-report

Self-reported data on substance use can provide a reliable and valid basis for prevalence estimation (Reference Weaver, Madden and CharlesWeaver et al, 2003), and such an assessment method has the potential to gather information on past use. For example, the self-report time-line follow-back procedure (Reference Sobell, Agrawal and AnnisSobell et al, 2001) for evaluating retrospective self-reported estimates of daily alcohol consumption can be reliably used by clinicians and researchers to collect information on drinking habits up to 12 months before the interview. A limitation of the present study is that our enquiries about past substance use did not include detailed assessment of the frequency and amounts of illicit drugs taken, as well as dependency. It remains uncertain whether precise information on the frequency, intensity and timing of use of the full range of substances reported, sometimes over periods of several years, could have been reliably gathered retrospectively. The lack of such data means that some of the patients with a history of lifetime substance use who were identified in this study may have been infrequent users. However, the evidence suggests that even relatively low levels of comorbid substance use are associated with a poorer outcome in schizophrenia, including a greater risk of relapse and a poorer response to conventional antipsychotic medication (Reference Drake, Brunette and GalanterDrake & Brunette, 1998; Reference Kavanagh, Waghorn and JennerKavanagh et al, 2004), and this seems to hold true for first-episode patients, even in the short term (Reference CaspariCaspari, 1999; Reference Sorbara, Liraud and AssensSorbara et al, 2003).

Symptom and cognitive correlates of comorbid substance use

The finding of a significantly higher mean CGI score in those using substances is consistent with numerous previous reports of more severe illness in people with schizophrenia and comorbid substance use (Reference Brunette, Mueser and XieBrunette et al, 1997). However, there was no significant association with positive psychotic symptoms, which may partly reflect the fact that positive symptoms would have been present in virtually all of the patients in the sample, as the mental state assessments were usually conducted during the first admission to hospital with overt psychosis. Our findings do not suggest that substance use has an effect on the presentation of other symptoms. An association between comorbid substance use and less severe negative symptoms has been reported in several previous studies of patients with schizophrenia (Reference Brunette, Mueser and XieBrunette et al, 1997), but has not consistently emerged in first-episode studies (Reference Linszen, Dingemans and LenoirLinszen et al, 1994; Reference Hambrecht and HafnerHambrecht & Häfner, 1996; Reference Sevy, Robinson and SollowaySevy et al, 2001). We did not find such an association in the present study. Similarly, we found no relationship between substance use and depressive features. Several previous studies of patients with established schizophrenia have more commonly found depressive symptoms in individuals with comorbid substance use, but such an association has not been a feature of studies of first-episode psychosis (Reference Linszen, Dingemans and LenoirLinszen et al, 1994; Reference Sevy, Robinson and SollowaySevy et al, 2001).

Previous findings that substance use was associated with significantly better premorbid cognitive functioning and higher current full-scale IQ prompted the theory that first-episode patients who have used substances may have been better able to obtain them because they had more opportunity, greater resources and possibly better social skills (Reference Salyers and MueserSalyers & Mueser, 2001; Reference Sevy, Robinson and SollowaySevy et al, 2001). This hypothesis is not supported by the findings of our study. Comparison of patients with and without comorbid substance use revealed no significant differences with regard to premorbid IQ (as estimated by the NART), current full-scale IQ or performance on the tests of memory and executive function. Furthermore, the two groups were rated as having similar levels of social function. Our data are consistent with those of Pencer & Addington (Reference Pencer and Addington2003), who also found no association between mild-to-moderate substance misuse and cognitive impairment in patients with first-episode psychosis. They suggested that any deleterious or specific effect of substance use on cognition in people with schizophrenia occurs only after prolonged use. We have previously found that a lower premorbid IQ is associated with an earlier age at onset (Reference Joyce, Hutton and MutsatsaJoyce et al, 2005). In the current patient sample, a regression analysis revealed that premorbid IQ and substance misuse were independent predictors of earlier age at onset.

Adherence to treatment

In patients with established schizophrenia, comorbid substance use has been shown to be a major determinant of adherence to treatment (Reference Olfson, Mechanic and HansellOlfson et al, 2000). The poorer prognosis, particularly the higher relapse rate, in people with schizophrenia who use substances may be partly explained by reduced adherence to the medication regimen. However, in the sample of patients that we studied, we found no relationship between attitudes to medication that are relevant to adherence (as assessed by the ROMI) and substance use. These findings suggest that, among people with schizophrenia, a history of substance use at the time of first presentation to psychiatric services does not reliably identify those individuals who already have negative attitudes to medication and treatment.

Clinical implications

These results confirm the high prevalence of lifetime substance use in first-episode patients with schizophrenia. Although alcohol and cannabis were the substances most commonly used, most patients reported a history of multiple substance use. From a clinical perspective, given that such substance use tends to predict a poorer initial outcome, our findings reinforce the need to routinely assess and consider appropriate intervention for substance use in people with schizophrenia when they first present to pyschiatric services.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Systematic assessment of substance use is warranted in patients with recent-onset schizophrenia.

  2. The use of cannabis and other substances by psychologically vulnerable adolescents should be discouraged.

  3. Retrospective assessment of the duration of untreated psychosis should take account of the possible confounding effect of symptoms related to substance use.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The study population consisted of an urban sample of patients in the UK, and this limits the extent to which the findings can be generalised.

  2. The assessment of substance use did not include frequency of use or dependence.

  3. The assessors who recorded the reports of substance use were not masked to the mental state of the patients.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Dr B. Puri and Dr M. Chapman for help with data collection, and Dr V. Huddy for assistance with data analysis. This study was sup supported by a grant from the Wellcome Trust.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding det alled in the Acknowledgements.

References

Addington, J. & Adding ton, D. (1998) Effect of substance misuse in early psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172 (suppl. 33), 134136.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. (1990) Methods for assessing positive and negative symptoms. In Schizophrenia: Positive and Negative Symptoms and Syndromes. Modern Problems in Pharmacopsychiatry (ed. Andreasen, N.), pp. 7385. Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Asberg, M., Montgomery, S. A., Perris, C., et al (1978) A comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplementum, 271, 527 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, T. R. E., Hutton, S. B., Chapman, M. J., et al (2000) West London first-episode study of schizophrenia. Clinical correlates of duration of untreated psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 207211.Google Scholar
Birchwood, M., Smith, J., Cochrane, R., et al (1990) The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 853859.Google Scholar
Blanchard, J. J., Brown, S. A., Horan, W. P., et al (2000) Substance use disorders in schizophrenia: review, integration and a proposed model. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 207234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunette, M. F., Mueser, K. T., Xie, H., et al (1997) Relationships between symptoms of schizophrenia and substance abuse. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 1320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantor-Graee, E., Nordstr⊘m, L. G. & McNeil, T. F. (2001) Substance abuse in schizophrenia: areview of the literature and a study of correlates in Sweden. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 6982.Google Scholar
Caspari, D. (1999) Cannabis and schizophrenia: results of a follow-up study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 249, 4549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Condren, R. M., O'Connor, J. & Browne, R. (2001) Prevalence and patterns of substance misuse in schizophrenia. A catchment area case–control study. Psychiatric Bulletin, 25, 1720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degenhardt, L., Hall, W. & Lynskey, M. (2003) Tessttiing hypotheses about the relationship between cannabis use and psychosis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 71, 3748.Google Scholar
DeLeon, J., Tracy, J. & McCann, E. (2002) Schizophrenia and tobacco smoking: a replication study in another US psychiatric hospital. Schizophrenia Research, 56, 5565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drake, R. E. & Brunette, M. F. (1998) Complications of severe mental illness related to alcohol and other drug disorders. In Recent Developments in Alcoholism (ed. Galanter, M.), pp. 285299. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Duke, P. J., Pantelis, C. & Barnes, T. R. (1994) South Westminster schizophrenia survey. Alcohol use and its relationship to symptoms, tardive dyskinesia and illness onset. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 630636.Google Scholar
Duke, P. J., Pantelis, C., McPhillips, M. A., et al (2001) Comorbid non-alcohol substance misuse among people with schizophrenia. Epidemiological study in central London. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 509513.Google Scholar
Guy, W. (ed.) (1976) Clinical Global Impression. In: ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology (revised), pp. 217221. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.Google Scholar
Hambrecht, M. & Hafner, H. (1996) Substance abuse and the onset of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 11551163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hambrecht, M. & Hafner, H. (2000) Cannabis, vulnerability and the onset of schizophrenia: an epidemiological perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 468475.Google Scholar
Henquet, C., Krabbendam, L., Spauwen, J., et al (2005) Prospective cohort study of cannabis use, predisposition for psychosis and psychotic symptoms in young people. BMJ, 330, 11.Google Scholar
Hutton, S. B., Puri, B. K., Duncan, L.-J., et al (1998) Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 28, 463473.Google Scholar
Jeste, D. V., Gladsjo, J. A., Lindamer, L. A., et al (1996) Medical comorbidity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 413430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyce, E. M., Hutton, S. B., Robbins, T. W., et al (2002) Executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia and relationship to duration of untreated psychosis. The West London Study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181 (suppl. 43), s38s44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, E. M., Hutton, S. B., Mutsatsa, S. H., et al (2005) Cognitive heterogeneityin first-episode schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 516522.Google Scholar
Kavanagh, D. J., McGrath, J., Saunders, J. B., et al (2002) Substance misuse in patients with schizophrenia: epidemiology and management. Drugs, 62, 743755.Google Scholar
Kavanagh, D. J., Waghorn, G., Jenner, L., et al (2004) Demographic and clinical correlates of comorbid substance use disorders in psychosis: multivariate analyses from an epidemiological sample. Schizophrenia Research, 66, 115124.Google Scholar
Larsen, T. K., McGlashan, T. H., Johannessen, J. O., et al (2001) Shortened duration of untreated first episode of psychosis: changes in patient characteristics at treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 19171919.Google Scholar
Linszen, D. H., Dingemans, P. M. & Lenoir, M. E. (1994) Cannabis abuse and the course of recent-onset schizophrenic disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 273279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loebel, A. D., Lieberman, J. A., Alvir, J. M. J., et al (1992) Duration of psychosis and outcome in firstepisode schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 11831188.Google ScholarPubMed
Mastrigt, S. V., Addington, J. & Addington, D. (2004) Substance misuse at presentation to an early psychosis program. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39, 6972.Google Scholar
Missar, C., Gold, J., Goldberg, T. (1994) WAIIS–R short forms in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 12, 247250.Google Scholar
Montgomery, S. A. & Asberg, M. (1979) Anew depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 382389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutsatsa, S., Joyce, E. M., Hutton, S., et al (2003) Clinical correlates of early medication compliance: the West London First Episode Schizophrenia Study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 439446.Google Scholar
Nelson, H. E. (1991) The National Adult Reading Test (NART), Second Version. Test Manual. Windsor: NFER–Nelson.Google Scholar
Norman, R. M. G. & Malla, A. K. (2002) Examining adherence to medication and substance use as possible confounds of duration of untreated psychosis. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 331334.Google Scholar
Olfson, M., Mechanic, D., Hansell, S., et al (2000) Predicting medication noncompliance after hospital discharge among patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 51, 216222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pencer, A. & Addington, J. (2003) Substance use and cognition in early psychosis. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 28, 4854.Google ScholarPubMed
Rabinowitz, J., Bromet, E. J., Lavelle, J., et al (1998) Prevalence and severity of substance use disorders and onset of psychosis in first-admission psychotic patients. Psychological Medicine, 28, 14111419.Google Scholar
Sahakian, B. & Owen, A. (1992) Computerised assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 85, 399402.Google Scholar
Salyers, M. P. & Mueser, K. T. (2001) Social functioning, psychopathology and medication side-effects in relation to substance use and abuse in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 109123.Google Scholar
Semple, D. M., McIntosh, A. M. & Lawrie, S. M. (2005) Cannabis as a risk factor for psychosis: systematic review. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 19, 187194.Google Scholar
Sevy, S., Robinson, D. G., Solloway, S., et al (2001) Correlates of substance misusein patients with first-episode schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104, 367374.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 298, 199209.Google Scholar
Smit, F., Bolier, L. & Cuijpers, P. (2004) Cannabis use and the risk of later schizophrenia: a review. Addiction, 99, 425430.Google Scholar
Sobell, L. C., Agrawal, S., Annis, H., et al (2001) Cross-cultural evaluation of two drinking assessment instruments: alcohol time-line follow-back and inventory of drinking situations. Substance Use and Misuse, 36, 313331.Google Scholar
Sorbara, F., Liraud, F., Assens, F., et al (2003) Substance use and the course of early psychosis: a 2-year follow-up of first-admitted subjects. Schizophrenia Research, 18, 133136.Google ScholarPubMed
Tsapakis, E.-M., Guillin, O. & Murray, R. M. (2003) Does dopamine sensitization underlie the association between schizophrenia and drug abuse? Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 16 (suppl. 2), 4552.Google Scholar
Veen, N. D., Selten, J.-P., van der Tweel, I., et al (2004) Cannabis use and age at onset of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 501506.Google Scholar
Weaver, T., Madden, P., Charles, V., et al (2003) Comorbidity of substance misuse and mental illness in community mental health and substance misuse services. British Journal of Psychiatry, 183, 304313.Google Scholar
Weiden, P., Rapkin, B., Mott, T., et al (1994) Rating of Medication Influences (ROMI) scale in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 297310.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Clinical characteristics of patients with and without a lifetime history of substance use

Figure 1

Table 2 Nature and frequency of alcohol and substance use in the total sample (n=148) and in the subsample of patients who reported lifetime alcohol and/or substance use (n=106)

Figure 2

Table 3 Mental state and neuropsychological assessment variables for patients with and without a lifetime history of substance use

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.