Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:50:32.889Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authors' reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 December 2018

Borwin Bandelow
Affiliation:
psychiatrist, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen, Germany Email: [email protected]
Anne Sagebiel
Affiliation:
psychiatrist Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Göttingen, Germany
Michael Belz
Affiliation:
psychologist, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen, Germany
Yvonne Görlich
Affiliation:
psychologist, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen, Germany
Sophie Michaelis
Affiliation:
psychiatrist, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen, Germany
Dirk Wedekind
Affiliation:
psychiatrist, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Göttingen, Germany.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2018 

We found that gains with psychotherapy were maintained for up to 24 months. We also showed that patients who stopped medication remained stable. This is good news for the affected patients. However, as patients in the placebo groups also did not show deterioration we concluded that enduring effects observed in follow-up studies might be superimposed by spontaneous remission or effects of concurrent treatments.

For detailed inclusion criteria, we had referred to our previous meta-analysis.Reference Bandelow, Reitt, Rover, Michaelis, Görlich and Wedekind1 As there are only a few head-to-head follow-up comparisons of psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy, we decided to calculate pre–post effects. Thus, we were able to include as many as 93 follow-up studies, which also comprised all head-to-head comparisons.

Pre–post effect sizes do not only measure ‘true’ treatment effects, but also natural course and placebo effects. However, when conditions are the same in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy studies, this comparison is fair. Patients are mainly interested in a substantial decline in their anxiety scale scores, and this is what we measure with pre–post effects. Follow-up studies for psychotherapy mostly lack a control group, because a waitlist is used as a control during the active treatment period. After termination, the waitlist patients cannot serve as controls anymore because they will now receive the active treatment. Therefore, the pre–post effect size is the only option to compare the results of follow-up studies.

In every meta-analysis, not only in the ones using pre–post effect sizes, populations may be heterogeneous. However, all the included medication studies were comparisons with psychotherapy. Therefore, it is unlikely that our results were biased because of differences in patient populations.

It is also unlikely that allegiance effects in favour of pharmacotherapy have influenced our results, for 4 reasons: (1) In the data set of our previous meta-analysis,Reference Bandelow, Reitt, Rover, Michaelis, Görlich and Wedekind1 on which the present analysis was performed, we found possible allegiance effects in both psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy studies but were unable to find significant differences between the average effect sizes of studies with or without allegiance effects for both treatment modalities. (2) The vast majority of the studies, including those involving medications, were published by behavioural psychotherapists. (3) The patents of all drugs mentioned in the study have expired for long. (4) We published the raw data of our analysis so that anyone who has the feeling that results might be biased can re-calculate the effect sizes. Allegiance effects are also possible in psychotherapy studies.Reference Munder, Brutsch, Leonhart, Gerger and Barth2 We frankly disclosed our conflicts of interest, but this should also be expected from authors publishing in the field of psychotherapy, in particular when they are strongly promoting certain forms of psychotherapy, such as Dr Leichsenring, who is a fervent advocate for psychoanalysis and has been criticised for possibly biased meta-analyses in the literature.Reference Coyne, Bhar, Pignotti, Tovote and Beck3 Further, the authors seem to have overlooked the part in which we mentioned adverse effects of drugs. Enduring side-effects of medications that are used for anxiety disorders are rare, however.Reference Bandelow, Michaelis and Wedekind4 Furthermore, medication does not lessen cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) gains; we found much higher average effects for CBT plus medication (Cohen's d = 2.12) than for CBT alone (d = 1.22).Reference Bandelow, Reitt, Rover, Michaelis, Görlich and Wedekind1

Footnotes

Declaration of interest In the past 12 months and in the near future, B.B has been/will be on the speakers/advisory board for Hexal, Mundipharma, Lilly, Lundbeck, Pfizer and Servier. D.W. has served on the speakers’ board of AstraZeneca, Essex Pharma, Lundbeck and Servier.

References

1Bandelow, B, Reitt, M, Rover, C, Michaelis, S, Görlich, Y, Wedekind, D. Efficacy of treatments for anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2015; 30: 183–92.Google Scholar
2Munder, T, Brutsch, O, Leonhart, R, Gerger, H, Barth, J. Researcher allegiance in psychotherapy outcome research: an overview of reviews. Clin Psychol Rev 2013; 33: 501–11.Google Scholar
3Coyne, JC, Bhar, SS, Pignotti, M, Tovote, KA, Beck, AT. Missed opportunity to rectify or withdraw a flawed metaanalysis of longer-term psychodynamic psychotherapy. Psychother Psychosom 2011; 80: 53–4.Google Scholar
4Bandelow, B, Michaelis, S, Wedekind, D. Treatment of anxiety disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2017; 19: 93107.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.