Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T01:28:46.505Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Atypical Facial Pain and Depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

R. G. Lascelles*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Guy's Hospital, London, S.E.$1

Extract

Prolonged facial pain has long been a source of frustration to the medical profession, both from the diagnostic and therapeutic standpoints. The difficulty arises not in the “typical” facial pain syndromes such as trigeminal neuralgia, migrainous neuralgia or post-herpetic neuralgia, nor with pain due to diseases of the teeth, throat, nose, eyes and ears, but in the deep, poorly localized vaguely described pain which does not adhere to a strict anatomical distribution. The latter symptom complex has been given many names, but in spite of this is a remarkably uniform syndrome. It is characterized by pain that is felt deep in the soft tissues or the bone, rather than in the superficial tissues as occurs so often in the typical neuralgias, is poorly localized and vaguely described. The pain may be felt in regions supplied by the fifth and ninth cranial nerves and the second and third cervical nerves. Its distribution does not conform to the peripheral distribution of these nerves, but may involve portions of the sensory supply of two or more of them and may cross the mid-line. In general the pain is constant and endures for long periods of weeks to years. It is unusual to find trigger zones or clear-cut precipitating factors, and the pain is rarely excruciating.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1966 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Campbell, A. M. G. and Lloyd, J. K. (1954). Lancet, ii, 1034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costen, J. B. (1934). Ann. Otol. Rhin. and Laryn., 43, 1.Google Scholar
Davies, J. V. S. A. (1961). Brit. med. J., ii, 1527.Google Scholar
Elkington, J. St. C. (1946). In A Textbook of the Practice of Medicine (ed. Price, F.), 7th edition.Google Scholar
Engel, G. L. (1951). Psychosomat. Med., 13, 375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fay, T. (1927). Arch. Neurol. and Psychiat., 18, 309.Google Scholar
Gainsborough, H. and Slater, E. (1946). Brit. med. J., ii, 253.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M. (1960). J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 23, 56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, D. and Hull, S. B. (1962). Lancet, i, 541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, F. W. L. (1960). Proc. Staff Meetings Mayo Clin., 36, 254.Google Scholar
Mayer-Gross, W. Slater, E. and Roth, M. (1954). Clinical Psychiatry. London: Cassell.Google Scholar
McElin, T. W. and Horton, D. T. (1947). Ann. intern. Med., 27, 749.Google Scholar
Neuwirth, E. (1952). Ibid., 37, 75.Google Scholar
Palmer, H. D. and Jones, M. S. (1939). A.M.A. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 41, 856.Google Scholar
Pollitt, J. D. (1960). J. ment. Sci., 106, 93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sargant, W. (1960). Psychosomatics, 1, 14.Google Scholar
Sargant, W. (1961). Brit. med. J., i, 225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shorvon, H. (1946). Proc. Roy. Soc. Med., 39, 779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webb, H. E. and Lascelles, R. G. (1962). Lancet, i, 355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, E. D. and Dally, P. J. (1959). Brit. med. J., i, 1491.Google Scholar
Williams, D. (1963). In Trans. Amer. Neurol. Assoc., 88, 17.Google Scholar
Ziegler, L. H. (1939). Psychiat. Quart., 13, 689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.