Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:08:10.193Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between schizophrenia and social inequality at birth: case–control study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Glynn Harrison*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Bristol
David Gunnell
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, University of Bristol
Cris Glazebrook
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham
Kim Page
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham
Rosemary Kwiecinski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Nottingham
*
Glynn Harrison, Division of Psychiatry, 41 St Michael's Hill, University of Bristol BS8 3JT, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The association between social inequality at birth and subsequent risk of schizophrenia is uncertain.

Aims

To investigate the relationship between adult-onset schizophrenia and two indicators of social inequality at birth: social class and area of residence.

Method

A matched case–control design was used with data from birth certificates of first-episode cases and age— and gender-matched controls.

Results

Risk increased with increasing levels of deprivation at birth. Subjects whose fathers were social class IV–V or who were born in deprived areas were at increased risk of schizophrenia (odds ratio=2.1; 95% Cl=0.8–5.5). Risk was greater in those with both of these indicators (odds ratio=8.1; 95% CI=2.7–23.9). There was some evidence that associations were stronger in older subjects. Exclusion of African–Caribbeans or cases with positive family history somewhat attenuated the association.

Conclusions

Indicators of social inequality at birth are associated with increased risk of adult-onset schizophrenia, suggesting that environmental factors are important determinants of schizophrenic disorders.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001 

People with schizophrenia are more likely to occupy lower socio-economic positions and reside in areas characterised by higher social deprivation at the time of first diagnosis (Reference Croudace, Bloom and JonesCroudace et al, 2000; Reference Eaton and HarrisonEaton & Harrison, 2001). It is unclear whether these factors contribute to aetiology or whether they result from social segregation caused by prodromal symptoms. Goldberg & Morrison (Reference Goldberg and Morrison1963) examined fathers' social class by occupation in male patients with schizophrenia and appeared to demonstrate that downward social segregation occurred after birth. Evidence is accumulating, however, for an association between risk of schizophrenia and urbanisation of birth-place (Reference Lewis, David and AndreassonLewis et al, 1992; Reference Mortensen, Pedersen and WestergaardMortensen et al, 2000), and obstetric complications, themselves associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia, are also generally socially patterned. We have investigated the relationship between social inequality at birth and the later development of schizophrenia in a first-episode sample. We examined two indicators of socio-economic position recorded at the time of birth and available from routine sources: father's social class; and the social characteristics of the area of residence of the mother.

METHOD

Sample

The design of the first-episode psychosis study on which this analysis is based is described in detail elsewhere (Reference Brewin, Cant well and DalkinBrewin et al, 1997). Briefly, every first episode case of psychosis (n=168) presenting to specialist mental health services over a 2-year period was identified from a defined catchment area covering a population of 600 000 people in Nottingham. Cases were identified using a psychosis screen and subject to in-depth assessments. ‘First-episode’ was defined as a first contact with mental health services for a psychotic illness; there was no ‘cut off’ point for duration of untreated illness, and any cases with prior episodes of undetected illness also were included. Diagnostic categories were assigned using ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1993) following psychopathological assessments with the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN; World Health Organization, 1994). Family history was recorded using the Family History Research Diagnostic Criteria (Reference Andreasen, Endicott and SpitzerAndreasen et al, 1977) based upon multiple sources of information including case notes, patients and, where possible, informants. Cases were allocated to ethnic group on the basis of place of birth of parents: for the purposes of the present analysis, cases were identified as African—Caribbean if one or both parents had been born in the Caribbean (Reference Harrison, Glazebrook and BrewinHarrison et al, 1997).

Study design

A matched case—control design was used. Of 168 incident cases arising over the 24-month study recruitment period, 94 had been born in the study catchment area and these form the basis of this analysis. The birth certificate of each of these cases was located from local birth registers and four age-gender-matched controls were obtained by selecting the next four births registered at the same registry office. Cases and controls thus were matched for date of birth (to within 6 weeks) and area of birth registration. Because our cases were incident episodes of psychotic disorder in people who had been born in Nottingham, to ensure comparability of the source populations for cases and controls we eliminated, before analysis, those controls (43% of the total) who were no longer resident in Nottingham (according to the Family Health Services Authority register) at the time of case ascertainment.

We extracted father's occupation and mother's address from the birth certificates for cases and controls. Father's occupation was assigned to a social class on the basis of the Registrar General's Classification of Occupations (Office of Population Censuses and Surveys, 1991). We used the mother's address to assign each individual to an area measure of socio-economic deprivation, the Nottinghamshire County Council ‘index of need’. This was based mainly upon 1971 census data (Nottinghamshire County Council, 1975), corresponding to the median birth year of the study sample. The county was divided into zones, composed of combinations of enumeration districts, designed to cover areas of broadly similar social character. Each of the zones was scored on 20 indicators representing income, unemployment, crime, housing and other dimensions of deprivation. The scores then were aggregated to give a single (unweighted) summary measure of ‘deprivation’, allowing them to be ranked and stratified into four broad categories. Because of the relatively small numbers in the present study, we decided a priori to create a two-level indicator of deprivation by collapsing these four groups into two broad categories of ‘above average’ or ‘below average’ social need.

Analysis

We analysed associations of the two indicators with all psychotic disorders. Then we assessed separately the associations for non-affective, non-substance-related psychosis (‘broadly defined schizophrenia’, ICD-10 F20-29) and non-schizophrenic psychosis (ICD-10 F30-33 and F1x). Conditional logistic regression analyses were carried out using the Stata software package (StataCorp, 1999). Tests for linear trend across social class categories were based on the continuous measure for social class. Because no information on the ethnicity or family history of controls was available, the potential confounding effects of these factors were assessed by repeating the main analyses but excluding African—Caribbean cases as well as cases with a family history of schizophrenia.

A three-level childhood deprivation score, combining information on place of birth and parental occupation, then was produced as follows: 0, if the father was social class I—III and the mother lived in a non-deprived area; 1, if the father was social class IV/V or the mother lived in a deprived area; 2, if the father was social class IV/V and the mother lived in a deprived area.

Appropriate interaction terms were fitted to the models to investigate whether associations between childhood socio-economic deprivation and schizophrenia differed by gender or age (categorised into two groups — above or below the median age of 25 years for the sample).

RESULTS

The 168 cases identified in the original sample yielded an age-standardised annual incidence rate of 28.7/100 000 population. Of the 94 cases born in Nottingham, information on father's occupation on the birth certificates enabled classification into social class I—V groupings in 88 cases. Data on at least one matched control subject were available for 82 of these cases and these, together with their 198 matched controls, constitute the subjects included in the models presented below. The median age of the cases was 25.5 years (range 16-64) and 61% were males. There was no difference between African—Caribbeans and other cases in the proportion of SCAN ratings that were based upon full respondent interviews (Reference Harrison, Glazebrook and BrewinHarrison et al, 1997). Fifty-eight (71%) were classified into broadly defined schizophrenia (ICD—10, F20-29). Twenty-four (29%) had a non-schizophrenic psychotic illness (ICD—10 F30-33 and F1x). Table 1 shows the distribution of father's social class and mother's residence by deprivation ‘need’ index for the cases of adult-onset psychosis and controls.

Table 1 Distribution of paternal social class and deprivation score of area of residence at birth of incident cases of psychosis with matched controls

Social class of father Controls ‘All psychosis’ cases Total
I 4 3 7
II 17 6 23
III non-manual 16 5 21
III manual 116 25 141
IV 28 28 56
V 17 15 32
Total 198 82 280
Area ‘need’ index1
0 89 27 116
I 109 55 164
Total 198 82 280

Table 2 shows the odds ratios (95% CI) for the association of adult-onset psychosis with paternal social class and area-level deprivation. For all psychoses there were significant associations with the two indicators examined independently: social class and area of residence at birth. When associations with broad schizophrenia and other psychoses were examined separately there was some evidence that associations are stronger in relation to area of residence for broad schizophrenia. The small number of cases limited the power of analyses examining relationships with other psychotic disorders.

Table 2 Odds ratios (95% CI) for association between socio-economic position at birth and adult-onset psychosis (based on the 82 cases and their respective controls where classification of paternal social class into I-V groupings was possible)

All psychoses (n=82) Broad schizophrenia (n=58) Other (non-schizophrenic) psychotic illness (n=24)
Paternal social class
I/II (reference category) 1.0 1.0 1.0
III (nm/m) 0.6 (0.2-1.6) 0.4 (0.1-1.2) 1.5 (0.3-7.6)
IV/V 2.6 (1.0-6.9) 1.8 (0.6-5.8) 4.7 (0.8-29.3)
P value for linear trend P=0.010 P=0.023 P=0.21
Residence in deprived compared 1.9 (1.0-3.7) 2.7 (1.2-6.5) 1.1 (0.4-3.2)
with non-deprived area P=0.046 P=0.021 P=0.86
Combined deprivation score
0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1 1.9 (0.9-4.0) 2.1 (0.8-5.5) 1.5 (0.4-5.4)
2 6.3 (2.7-15.2) 8.1 (2.7-23.9) 3.6 (0.8-16.9)
P value for linear trend P < 0.001 P < 0.001 P=0.11

The combined childhood deprivation score showed a highly significant linear association with all psychoses and for broad schizophrenia, although the association was weaker for non-schizophrenic psychoses. For the latter group, confidence intervals were wide and, as stated above, the analysis lacked statistical power. There was no significant interaction between gender and the combined deprivation score with respect to overall risk of psychosis (P=0.81). There were weak interactions between age and the summary deprivation score with respect to all psychosis (P=0.16) and the broad schizophrenia grouping (P=0.18). In a stratified analysis the odds ratio for broad schizophrenia per unit increase in deprivation score was 4.6 (95% CI=1.9-10.9) for subjects aged >25 years and 2.1 (95% CI=1.0-4.2) for subjects aged <25 years.

Family history of psychosis and African—Caribbean family background

The association between indicators of deprivation at birth and adult-onset psychosis could be explained by family history of psychotic illness. Parental illness would have led to reduced occupation status and geographical segregation in deprived urban areas. There is also an increased risk of psychosis in those with a family background of migration to the UK from the Caribbean (Reference Harrison, Glazebrook and BrewinHarrison et al, 1997), and African—Caribbean families might be more likely to reside in areas of higher deprivation. To assess these possible confounding effects, analyses were repeated for cases without a family history of psychosis in a first-degree relative (Table 3) and cases with neither a family history of psychosis nor African—Caribbean ethnicity (Table 4). Because both of these analyses are based on smaller numbers of cases, they have lower statistical power than those based on the complete data-set. Restriction of the analysis to these subgroups without a positive family history did not alter greatly the strength of the observed associations reported in Table 2, although the magnitude of the odds ratios was attenuated after exclusion of the African—Caribbean cases and their matched controls, suggesting some confounding of the main effects. An analysis excluding only African—Caribbean cases showed a similar degree of attenuation.

Table 3 Odds ratios (95% CI) for association between socio-economic position at birth and adult-onset psychosis, excluding those cases with a family history of schizophrenia and related psychoses

All psychoses (n=71 cases) Broad schizophrenia (n=50) Other (non-schizophrenic) psychotic illness (n=21)
Paternal social class
I/II (reference category) 1.0 1.0 1.0
III (nm/m) 0.7 (0.3-1.9) 0.5 (0.1-1.7) 1.4 (0.3-7.4)
IV/V 2.6 (0.9-7.3) 2.0 (0.6-6.9) 3.9 (0.6-25.0)
P value for linear trend P=0.027 P=0.045 P=0.33
Residence in deprived compared 2.0 (1.0-4.0) 2.9 (1.2-7.2) 1.1 (0.4-3.5)
with non-deprived area P=0.047 P=0.023 P=0.85
Combined deprivation score
0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1 1.6 (0.7-3.5) 2.2 (0.8-6.3) 0.7 (0.2-3.2)
2 5.7 (2.3-14.3) 7.7 (2.4-24.4) 3.3 (0.6-17.3)
P value for linear trend P < 0.001 P < 0.001 P=0.17

Table 4 Odds ratios (95% CI) for association between socio-economic position at birth and adult-onset psychosis, excluding those with a family history of psychosis and those of African—Caribbean ethnicity

All psychoses (n=51 cases) Broad schizophrenia (n=42) Other (non-schizophrenic) psychotic illness (n=19)
Paternal social class
I/II (reference category) 1.0 1.00 1.0
III (nm/m) 0.7 (0.3-2.0) 0.5 (0.2-1.8) 1.2 (0.2-6.7)
IV/V 2.6 (0.9-7.3) 1.7 (0.5-6.0) 5.9 (0.8-42.8)
P value for linear trend P=0.07 P=0.19 P=0.21
Residence in deprived compared 1.7 (0.8-3.4) 2.3 (0.9-6.0) 0.9 (0.3-3.1)
with non-deprived area P=0.16 P=0.08 P=0.92
Combined deprivation score
0 1.0 1.0 1.0
1 1.3 (0.5-3.0) 2.0 (0.7-5.7) 0.4 (0.1-2.6)
2 4.7 (1.8-12.2) 5.1 (1.6-16.9) 4.3 (0.7-30.0)
P value for linear trend P=0.002 P=0.007 P=0.13

DISCUSSION

Our data show that socio-economic deprivation at birth is associated with increased risk of developing schizophrenia and related non-affective psychoses in later life. There was a strong association in cases with broadly defined schizophrenia, but for other (mostly affective) psychoses the case numbers were small and the power of analysis was low. The association with broadly defined schizophrenia persisted but was attenuated slightly after restricting the analysis to those cases who were not of African—Caribbean family origin.

Methodological issues

The strengths of our study design include a well-defined first-episode cohort from a delineated catchment area, with almost complete and comprehensive case ascertainment and assessment. A high proportion of face to face interviews at baseline and follow-up was achieved and standardised assessment tools with high interrater reliability and operationalised criteria for diagnosis were used throughout. The record of family history of psychotic disorder was the weakest aspect of the methodology: we did not carry out a systematic interview with all first-degree relatives, but relied upon interviews with patients, case notes and, where available, an interview with an informant. It is possible that this resulted in some underreporting.

Several other methodological limitations require consideration. Although the study design was a population-based case—control study, case finding was based upon contact with secondary services rather than a systematic community survey. The observed association with social deprivation may have been for risk of making contact with secondary mental health services when developing psychotic symptoms, rather than for risk of psychotic illness per se. Although we cannot eliminate this possibility altogether, we think that this is unlikely in view of evidence from community surveys that most cases make contact with specialist services at some point in the course of the disorder (Reference Link, Dohrenwend, Dohrenwend, Dohrenwend and GouldLink & Dohrenwend, 1980).

The selection of controls required consideration. First, no attempt was made to determine whether controls had schizophrenia. However, because the population prevalence of schizophrenia is low (Office of Population Censuses and Surveys, 1995), the effects of any possible misclassification of controls would be small. Furthermore, such misclassifications would tend to reduce the size of any effect and bias our findings towards the null hypothesis. Second, Nottingham has three birth registry offices and, because both cases and controls would be more likely to register locally, we probably overmatched for geographical area. This also would tend to bias our findings towards the null hypothesis of no difference in area of residence. Finally, confounding could be explored only in a limited way because we were unable to obtain information on the ethnicity and family history of the controls.

A further issue in the interpretation of these data is the potential effect of possibly different migration patterns among cases and potential controls. Although we excluded controls that had migrated out of Nottingham, it is possible that socio-economic factors may influence the migration patterns of people during the prodromal period of psychotic illness: the outward migration from Nottingham of preschizophrenia cases could have been greater among more affluent cases, producing a biased distribution of social class and area of residence among the remaining cases. We believe that this is an unlikely explanation, but the issue remains incapable of being resolved conclusively within the design and sample sizes available in our study.

We based our deprivation score upon available data close to the median birth date of our sample and we do not have data for the longer term stability of this scoring distribution, covering the age range of our sample. There is evidence from other studies, however, that an area's socio-economic characteristics remain relatively stable over long time periods (Reference Dorling, Mitchell and ShawDorling et al, 2000). Finally, it should be recognised that the exclusion of cases that were not born in Nottingham resulted in a sizeable reduction in the number of cases eligible for inclusion in our sample. Our findings therefore only apply to people with schizophrenia who continue to reside in a local area after birth: the association cannot be generalised to all schizophrenic disorders.

Previous studies

Although Goldberg & Morrison (Reference Goldberg and Morrison1963) and Hare et al (Reference Hare, Price and Slater1972) found that social class distribution by paternal background was comparable to that of the general population, our findings are similar to those of Turner & Wagenfield (Reference Turner and Wagenfield1967) and Castle et al (Reference Castle, Scott and Wessely1993), who reported associations with lower paternal social class. Recent British (Reference Done, Crowe and JohnstoneDone et al, 1994; Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994) and Finnish (Reference Makikyro, Isohanni and MoringMakikyro et al, 1997) studies, which take advantage of well-defined birth cohorts followed through part of the risk period for schizophrenia, report an association with high paternal socio-economic status. However, in the study by Jones et al (Reference Jones, Rodgers and Murray1994) the number of cases was small (n=30) and in both Done's (1994) and Makikyro's (1997) studies the subjects were followed for only part of the risk period (to age 28 and age 23 years, respectively) for developing schizophrenia. These analyses thus were confined to early-onset cases. It is possible that any association between parental social class and schizophrenia is stronger for later-onset cases, which would be consistent with our finding of borderline interactions between age and the summary deprivation score with respect to all psychosis. Larger, carefully defined samples are required to clarify these complex issues and, although we developed a combined measure to illustrate accumulated risk (after reporting separate analyses for our two indicators), future studies should continue to examine these (and other) markers separately.

Social inequalities and the aetiology of schizophrenia

Measures of socio-economic position are, at best, proxy markers for factors linked more directly to the risk of schizophrenia. There are a number of causal possibilities, including biological risk factors such as exposure to infections and other toxic agents, and non-biological factors such as social and psychological stressors linked to the degree of social fragmentation and individual social capital (Reference HareHare, 1956; Reference Van Os, Driessen and GuntherVan Os et al, 2000). It is becoming increasingly difficult to distinguish between causes that have a clear material basis and those that might be located in the ‘environment’ or in mental activity. In addition, although our data suggest that there are independent associations with individual-level and area-level characteristics, cumulative effects are likely to accrue over the life course and it is probable that causal relationships in schizophrenia are mediated by complex gene—environment interactions. Whatever the nature of the risk factors involved, our data suggest that environmental factors play a significant role in the aetiology of schizophrenia.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Indicators of social inequality at birth and in early life are associated with increased risk of schizophrenia in later life.

  2. The known association between schizophrenia and social deprivation at the time of diagnosis is not attributable entirely to inter-generation social selection and intrageneration social ‘drift’.

  3. These data add to accumulating evidence for the role of environmental factors in the aetiology of schizophrenia.

LIMITATIONS

  1. Cases were identified from patients making first-ever contact with secondary services, rather than by comprehensive (household) community survey.

  2. The method for measuring family history probably resulted in some under-reporting.

  3. During the period between birth and case identification there may have been different patterns of migration in the source and control populations.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

The study was funded by Trent Regional Health Authority.

References

Andreasen, N. C., Endicott, J., Spitzer, R. L., et al (1977) The family history method using diagnostic criteria. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 12291235.Google Scholar
Brewin, J., Cant well, R., Dalkin, T., et al (1997) The incidence of schizophrenia in Nottingham. A comparison of two cohorts, 1978–80 and 1992–94. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 140144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castle, D. J., Scott, K., Wessely, S., et al (1993) Does social deprivation during gestation and early life predispose to later schizophrenia? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 28, 14.Google Scholar
Croudace, T., Bloom, R., Jones, P., et al (2000) Non linear relationship between a measure of social deprivation, psychiatric admission prevalence and the incidence of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 30, 177185.Google Scholar
Done, D. J., Crowe, T. J., Johnstone, E. C., et al (1994) Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia and affective illness: social adjustment at ages 7 and 11. BMJ, 309, 699703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorling, D., Mitchell, R., Shaw, M., et al (2000) The Ghost of Christmas Past: health effects of poverty in London in 1896 and 1991. BMJ, 321, 15471551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eaton, W. & Harrison, G. (2001) Life chances, life planning and schizophrenia: a review and interpretation of research on social deprivation. International Journal of Mental Health, 30, 5881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, E. M. & Morrison, S. L. (1963) Schizophrenia and social class. British Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 785802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, E. H. (1956) Family setting and the urban distribution of schizophrenia. Journal of Mental Science, 102, 753760.Google Scholar
Hare, E. H., Price, J. S. & Slater, E. (1972) Parental social class in psychiatric patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 121, 515534.Google Scholar
Harrison, G., Glazebrook, C., Brewin, J., et al (1997) Increased incidence of psychotic disorders in African Caribbean migrants to the UK. Psychological Medicine, 17, 799806.Google Scholar
Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., et al (1994) Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet, 344, 13981402.Google Scholar
Lewis, G., David, A. & Andreasson, S. A. P. (1992) Schizophrenia and city life. Lancet, 340, 137140.Google Scholar
Link, G. & Dohrenwend, B. P. (1980) Formulation of hypotheses about the ratio of untreated to treated cases in the true prevalence studies of functional psychiatric disorders in adults in the United States. In Mental Illness in the United States: Epidemiologic Estimates (eds Dohrenwend, B. P., Dohrenwend, B. S., Gould, M. S., et al), pp. 133148. New York: Praeger Google Scholar
Makikyro, T., Isohanni, M., Moring, J., et al (1997) Is a child's risk of early onset schizophrenia increased in the highest social class. Schizophrenia Research, 23 (3), 245252.Google Scholar
Mortensen, P. B., Pedersen, C. B., Westergaard, T., et al (2000) Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine, 340 (8), 603608.Google Scholar
Nottinghamshire County Council (1975) County Deprived Area Study. Nottingham: Nottinghamshire County Council.Google Scholar
Office of Population Censuses and Surveys (1991) Standard Occupation Classification. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Office of Population Censuses and Surveys (1995) The Prevalence of Psychiatric Morbidity among Adults Living in Private Households. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
StataCorp (1999) Stats Statistical Software (release 6.0). College Station, TX: Stata Corporation Google Scholar
Turner, R. J. & Wagenfield, M. O. (1967) Occupational mobility and schizophrenia: an assessment of the social causation and social selection hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 32, 104113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Os, J., Driessen, G., Gunther, N., et al (2000) Neighbourhood variation incidence of schizophrenia. Evidence for person–environment interaction. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 243248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (1993) Tenth Revision of the International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD–10). Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1994) Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry II. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Distribution of paternal social class and deprivation score of area of residence at birth of incident cases of psychosis with matched controls

Figure 1

Table 2 Odds ratios (95% CI) for association between socio-economic position at birth and adult-onset psychosis (based on the 82 cases and their respective controls where classification of paternal social class into I-V groupings was possible)

Figure 2

Table 3 Odds ratios (95% CI) for association between socio-economic position at birth and adult-onset psychosis, excluding those cases with a family history of schizophrenia and related psychoses

Figure 3

Table 4 Odds ratios (95% CI) for association between socio-economic position at birth and adult-onset psychosis, excluding those with a family history of psychosis and those of African—Caribbean ethnicity

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.