Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:49:33.484Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Abnormalities of Response Processing in a Chronic Psychiatric Group

A Possible Predictor of Failure in Rehabilitation Programmes?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

T. Wykes*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF
R. Katz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto
E. Sturt
Affiliation:
Deceased
D. Hemsley
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry
*
Correspondence

Abstract

This study attempted to identify, in a mixed group of chronic patients, a specific measure of cognitive processing that may be of use in predicting dependence on psychiatric care. The measures investigated are derived from reaction-time tasks. Difficulties of response processing seem to account for the largest amount of variance in current service use. When compared with other variables shown to have some predictive power (e.g. social behaviour, symptoms and chronicity), the reaction time measures fare well. Functions derived from discriminant analyses using all the variables correctly classified 90% of those requiring day care and 95% of those requiring night care. Stepwise methods produced lower classification rates but always included reaction-time measures in the predictor set. Patients with continuing cognitive difficulties are likely to remain in more supportive psychiatric settings despite rigorous rehabilitation procedures.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1992 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bellisimo, A. & Steffy, R. (1975) Contextual influences on crossover in the reaction time performance of schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 210220.Google Scholar
Braff, D. (1981) Impaired speed of information processing in non-medicated schizotypal patients. Schizophrenic Bulletin, 7, 499508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broga, M. & Neufeld, R. (1981) Evaluation of information. Sequential aspects of schizophrenic performance. I. Framework and current findings. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169, 558568.Google Scholar
Cancro, R., Sutton, S., Kerr, J., et al (1971) Reaction time in prognosis in acute schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 153, 351359.Google Scholar
Fenton, W. & McGlashan, T. (1987) Prognostic scale for chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 277286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. (1987) The positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of action. Psychological Medicine, 17, 631648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1986) Routes to action in reaction time tasks. Psychological Research, 48, 169177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. & Baker, R. (1983) REHAB, a user's manual. Aberdeen: Vine Publishing.Google Scholar
Held, J., Cromwell, R., Frank, E., et al (1970) Effect of phenothiazines on reaction time in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 7, 209213.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. (1976) Stimulus uncertainty, response uncertainty and stimulus response compatibility as determinants of schizophrenics' reaction time. Bulletin of Psychonomic Science, 8, 425427.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. (1977) What have cognitive deficits to do with schizophrenic symptoms? British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 167173.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. (1978) Limitations of operant procedures in the modification of schizophrenic functioning: the possible relevance of studies of cognitive disturbance. Behaviour Analysis and Modification, 7, 165173.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. (1982) Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. In The Pathology of Cognition (ed. Burton, A.). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Johnstone, E., Owens, D., Frith, C., et al (1985) Institutionalism and the outcome of functional psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 3644.Google Scholar
Kowalski, P., Daley, G. & Gripp, R. I. (1976) Token economy: who responds how? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 372374.Google Scholar
Lyons, M. & Fulkerson, S. (1984) Decisional processing in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenics. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 591601.Google Scholar
Mann, S. & Cree, R. (1976) ‘New’ long-stay psychiatric patients: a national survey of fifteen mental hospitals in England and Wales 1972/3. Psychological Medicine, 6, 603616.Google Scholar
Marshall, W. (1973) Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 413433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, J. & Putchal, C. (1984) Motor control of schizophrenics – II. Manual control and tracking: sensory and motor deficits. Journal of Psychological Research, 18, 287298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neufeld, R. (1977) Components of processing deficit among paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 6064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuechterlein, K. (1977) Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of the data and theories. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 3, 373428.Google Scholar
Nuechterlein, K. & Dawson, M. (1984) Informational processing and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10, 160203.Google Scholar
Oltmanns, T., Ohayon, J. & Neale, J. (1978) The effects of antipsychotic medication and diagnostic criteria on distractibility in schizophrenics. In The Nature of Schizophrenia (eds Wynne, L., Cromwell, R. & Matthyse, S.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Opler, L., Kay, S., Rosado, V., et al (1984) Positive and negative syndromes in chronic schizophrenic inpatients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172, 317325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, D. & Johnstone, E. (1980) The disabilities of chronic schizophrenia: their nature and factors contributing to their development. British Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 384395.Google Scholar
Phillips, L. (1953) Case history data and prognosis in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 117, 515525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, P. & Wing, J. K. (1979) Residential care for the mentally disabled. In Community Care for the Mentally Disabled (eds Wing, J. K. & Olsen, R.). London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spaulding, W., Huntzinger, R., Le Compte, P., et al (1984) Clinical and etiological implications of a specific attention deficit in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172, 279287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SPSS-X (1983) A Users' Guide. Chicago: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Steffy, R. & Galbraith, K. (1980) Relation between latency and redundancy associated deficit in schizophrenia reaction time performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 419427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, S. (1969) The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donder's method. Acta Psychologia, 30, 275315.Google Scholar
Strauss, M. (1973) Behavioural differences between acute and chronic patients. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 271279.Google Scholar
Strauss, J. & Carpenter, W. (1974) The prediction of outcome in schizophrenia – II. Relationships between predictor and outcome variables: a report from the WHO International Pilot Study of Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 3742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, J. & Carpenter, W. (1977) Predictive outcome in schizophrenia – III: Five year outcome and its predictors. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 159163.Google Scholar
Sturt, E. (1981) Hierarchical patterns in the distribution of psychiatric symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 11, 738794.Google Scholar
Sturt, E., Wykes, T. & Creer, C. (1982) Demographic, social and clinical characteristics of the sample. In Long Term Community Care in Camberwell. Psychological Medicine, monograph suppl. No. 2, pp. 514.Google Scholar
Venables, P. H. (1958) Stimulus complexity as a determinant of the reaction time of schizophrenics. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 12, 187190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberger, D. (1985) Clinical–neuropathological correlations in schizophrenia: theoretical implications. In Controversies in Schizophrenia: Changes and Constancies (ed. Alpert, M.), pp. 92106. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
White, C. Farley, J. & Charles, P. (1987) Chronic schizophrenic disorder. II. Reaction time, social performance and arousal. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 374379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickens, C. (1974) Temporal limits of human information processing: a developmental study. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 739755.Google Scholar
Williams, R. M., Alagaratnam, W. & Hemsley, D. (1984) Relationship between subjective self-report of cognitive dysfunction and objective information-processing performance in a group of hospitalized schizophrenic patients. European Archives of Psychiatric Neurological Science, 234, 4853.Google Scholar
Williams, R. M. & Hemsley, D. (1986) Choice reaction time performance in hospitalised schizophrenic patients and depressed patients. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Science, 186, 169173.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K. & Brown, G. (1980) Institutionalism and Schizophrenia. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. & Sartorius, N. (1974) The Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K. & Sturt, E. (1978) The PSD–ID–CATEGO System Supplementary Manual. London: Medical Research Council.Google Scholar
Wishner, J., Stein, M. & Peastrel, A. (1978) Stages of information processing in schizophrenia: Sternberg's paradigm. In The Nature of Schizophrenia (eds Wynn, I., Cromwell, R. & Matthyse, S.). New York: Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Wykes, T. (1982) A hostel-ward for ‘new’ long-stay patients: an evaluative study of a ‘ward in a house’. Psychological Medicine, monograph suppl. No. 2, pp. 5997.Google Scholar
Wykes, T., Sturt, E. & Creer, C. (1982) Reactions of day and residential units in relation to the social behaviour of at tenders. Psychological Medicine, Monograph suppl. No. 2, pp. 1527.Google Scholar
Wykes, T. & Sturt, E. (1986) The measurement of social behaviour in psychiatric patients: an assessment of the reliability and validity of the SBS schedule. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 111.Google Scholar
Zahn, T. & Carpenter, W. (1978) Effects of short-term outcome and clinical improvement on reaction time in acute schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 14, 5968.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.