Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T22:33:08.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

WHAT CAN L2 WRITERS’ PAUSING BEHAVIOR TELL US ABOUT THEIR L2 WRITING PROCESSES?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 August 2019

Khaled Barkaoui*
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Khaled Barkaoui, Faculty of Education, York University, 253Winters College, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, Ontario, M3J 1P3. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

When responding to a writing task, writers spend a significant amount of their time not writing. These periods of physical inactivity, or pauses, during writing provide observable and measurable cues as to when, where, and how long writers halt to plan and/or revise their texts. Consequently, examining writers’ pausing patterns can provide important insights into the cognitive processes that writers employ when composing and the impact of various individual, task, and contextual factors on those processes. This article discusses theory and research on writers’ pausing behavior; how pause analysis can be used to investigate second language (L2) learners’ writing processes; challenges in researching writers’ pausing behavior (e.g., defining pauses); and some strategies to address these challenges. Next, the article illustrates how L2 writers’ pause data can be collected, analyzed, and interpreted, using keystroke logging data from a research project that aimed to examine the effects of task type, L2 proficiency, and keyboarding skills on L2 learners’ writing processes when writing on the computer. The article concludes with a call for more research on L2 writers’ pausing behavior, particularly how L2 writers’ pausing behavior relates to L2 writing outcomes and development across learners, contexts, and time.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The keystroke data used in this article is from a study funded by Educational Testing Service (ETS) under a Committee of Examiners and TOEFL research grant. ETS does not discount or endorse the methodology, results, implications, or opinions presented by the researcher. The opinions expressed in the report are those of the author.

References

REFERENCES

Abdel Latif, M. (2001). A state-of-the-art review of the real-time computer-aided study of the writing process. International Journal of English Studies, 8, 2950.Google Scholar
Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Chesnet, D., & Fayol, M. (2007). Parallel processing before and after pauses: A combined analysis of graphomotor and eye movements during procedural text production. In Torrance, M., van Waes, L., & Galbraith, D. (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (pp. 1329). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Alves, R. A., Castro, S. L., de Sousa, L., & Stromqvist, S. (2007). Influence of keyboarding skill on pause-execution cycles in written composition. In Torrance, M., van Waes, L., & Galbraith, D. (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (pp. 5565). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Baaijen, V. M., Galbraith, D., & de Glopper, K. (2012). Keystroke analysis: Reflections on procedures and measures. Written Communication, 29, 246277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkaoui, K. (2011). Think-aloud protocols in research on essay rating: An empirical study of their veridicality and reactivity. Language Testing, 28, 5175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkaoui, K. (2014). Examining the impact of L2 proficiency and keyboarding skills on scores on TOEFL iBT writing tasks. Language Testing, 31, 241259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkaoui, K. (2015). Test-takers’ writing activities during TOEFL-iBT writing tasks: A stimulated recall study (TOEFL iBT Research Report No. 25; ETS Research Report No. RR-15-04). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
Barkaoui, K. (2016a). Examining the cognitive processes engaged by Aptis Writing Task 4 on paper and on the computer (Research Report AR-A/2016/1). Retrieved from https://www.britishcouncil.org/examining-cognitive-processes-engaged-aptis-writing.Google Scholar
Barkaoui, K. (2016b). What and when second-language learners revise when responding to timed writing tasks on the computer: The roles of task type, second language proficiency, and keyboarding skills. The Modern Language Review, 100, 320340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boomer, D. S. (1965). Hesitation and grammatical encoding. Language and Speech, 8, 148158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosher, S. (1998). The composing processes of three Southeast Asian writers at the post-secondary level: An exploratory study. Journal of Second Language Writing, 7, 205241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breetvelt, I., Van den Bergh, H., & Rijlaarsdam, G. (1994). Relations between writing processes and text quality: When and how? Cognition and Instruction, 12, 103123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B. (1980). Evidence from pauses in speech. In Butterworth, B. (Ed.), Language production: Speech and talk (pp. 155176). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chanquoy, L., Foulin, J. N., & Fayol, M. (1996). Writing in adults: A real time approach. In Rijlaarsdam, G., van den Bergh, H., & Couzijn, M. (Eds.), Theories, models and methodology in writing research (pp. 3643). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Chenoweth, N., & Hayes, J. R. (2001). Fluency in writing: Generating text in L1 and L2. Written Communication, 18, 8098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chenu, F., Pellegrino, F., Jisa, H., & Fayol, M. (2014). Interword and intraword pause threshold in the writing of texts by children and adolescents: A methodological approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chukharev-Hudilainen, E., Saricaoglu, A., Torrance, M., & Feng, H. (2019). Combined deployable keystroke logging and eyetracking for investigating L2 writing fluency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 583604.Google Scholar
Cumming, A. (1989). Writing expertise and second language proficiency. Language Learning, 39, 81141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deane, P. (2014). Using writing process and product features to assess writing quality and explore how those features relate to other literacy tasks (Research Report No. RR-14-03). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deane, P., & Zhang, M. (2015). Exploring the feasibility of using writing process features to assess text production skills (Research Report No. RR-15-02). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fayol, M. (1999). From online management problems to strategies in written composition. In Torrance, M. & Jeffery, G. (Eds.), The cognitive demands of writing: Processing capacity and working memory effects in text production (pp. 1323). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Field, J. (2004). Psycholinguistics: The key concepts. London, UK: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haas, C. (1989). How the writing medium shapes the writing process: Effects of word processing on planning. Research in the Teaching of English, 23, 181207.Google Scholar
Hall, E. (1991). Variations in composing behaviors of academic ESL writers in test and non-test situations. TESL Canada Journal, 8, 933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 127). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (2006). New directions in writing theory. In MacArthur, C. A., Graham, S., & Fitzgerald, J. (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 2840). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R., & Chenoweth, N. A. (2006). Is working memory involved in the transcribing and editing of texts? Written Communication, 23, 135149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R., & Nash, J. G. (1996). On the nature of planning in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 2955). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 5771). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kormos, J. (2012). The role of individual differences in L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 21, 390403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, H. K. (2004). A comparative study of ESL writers’ performance in a paper-based and a computer-delivered writing test. Assessing Writing, 9, 4-26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leijten, M., & Van Waes, L. (2006). Inputlog: New perspectives on the logging of on-line writing processes in a Windows environment. In Sullivan, K. P. H. & Lindgren, E. (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 7393). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Leijten, M., & Van Waes, L. (2013). Keystroke logging in writing research: Using Inputlog to analyze and visualize writing processes. Written Communication, 30, 358392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leijten, M., Van Waes, L., Schrijver, I., Bernolet, S., & Vangehuchten, L. (2019). Mapping master’s students’ use of external sources in source-based writing in L1 and L2. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 555–582.Google Scholar
Lindgren, E., & Sullivan, K. P. (2003). Stimulated recall as a trigger for increasing noticing and language awareness in the L2 writing classroom: A case study of two young female writers. Language Awareness, 12, 172186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindgren, E., & Sullivan, K. P. H. (2006a). Analysing online revision. In Sullivan, K. P. H. & Lindgren, E. (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 157188). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Matsuhashi, A. (1981). Pausing and planning: The tempo of written discourse production. Research in the Teaching of English, 15, 113134.Google Scholar
Matsuhashi, A. (1987). Revising the plan and altering the text. In Matsuhashi, A. (Ed.), Writing in real time: Modeling production processes (pp. 197223). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 299325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCutchen, D. (2000). Knowledge, processing, and working memory: Implications for a theory of writing. Educational Psychologist, 35, 1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medimorec, S., & Risko, E. F. (2017). Pauses in written composition: On the importance of where writers pause. Reading and Writing, 30, 12671285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2002). Concurrent activation of high- and low-level production processes in written composition. Memory and Cognition, 30, 594600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phinney, M., & Khouri, S. (1993). Computers, revision and ESL writers: The role of experience. Journal of Second Language Writing, 2, 257277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Révész, A., Kourtali, N. E., & Mazgutova, D. (2017). Effects of task complexity on L2 writing behaviors and linguistic complexity. Language Learning, 67, 208241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Révész, A., Michel, M., & Lee, M. (2019). Exploring second language writers’ pausing and revision behaviors: A mixed-methods study. Studies in Seond Language Acquisition, 41, 605–631.Google Scholar
Roca de Larios, J., Manchón, R., Murphy, L., & Marín, J. (2008). The foreign language writers’ strategic behaviour in the allocation of time to writing processes. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17, 3047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, M. (2000). Toward an empirical model of EFL writing processes: An exploratory study. Journal of Second Language Writing, 9, 259291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilperoord, J. (1996). The distribution of pause time in written text production. In Rijlaarsdam, G., van den Bergh, H., & Couzijn, M. (Eds.), Theories, models and methodology in writing research (pp. 2135). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Schilperoord, J., & Sanders, T. (1999). How hierarchical text structure affects retrieval processes: Implications of pause and text analysis. In Torrance, M. & Galbraith, D. (Eds.), Knowing what to write: Conceptual processes in text production (pp. 1230). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Smagorinsky, P. (1994). Think-aloud protocol analysis: Beyond the black box. In Smagorinsky, P. (Ed.), Speaking about writing: Reflections on research methodology (pp. 3-19). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Spelman Miller, K. (2000). Academic writers on-line: Investigating pausing in the production of text. Language Teaching Research, 4, 123148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelman Miller, K. (2006a). Pausing, productivity and the processing of topic in online writing. In Sullivan, K. P. H. & Lindgren, E. (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 131155). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Spelman Miller, K. (2006b). The pausological study of written language production. In Sullivan, K. P. H. & Lindgren, E. (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 1130). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Spelman Miller, K., & Sullivan, K. P. H. (2006). Keystroke logging: An introduction. In Sullivan, K. P. H. & Lingden, E. (Eds.), Computer keystroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 19). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Spelman Miller, K., Lindgren, E., & Sullivan, K. P. H. (2008). The psycholinguistic dimension in second language writing: Opportunities for research and pedagogy using computer keystroke logging. TESOL Quarterly, 42, 433454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Standards Australia. (2001). Keyboarding speed tests (3rd ed.). Sydney: Standards Australia.Google Scholar
Stratman, J., & Hamp-Lyons, L. (1994). Reactivity in concurrent think-aloud editing protocols: Issues for research. In: Smagorinsky, P. (Ed.), Speaking about writing: Reflections on research methodology (pp. 89112). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Torrance, M., & Galbraith, D. (2006). The processing demands of writing. In MacArthur, C. A., Graham, S., & Fitzgerald, J. (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 6780). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Van der Hoeven, J. (1999) Differences in writing performance: Generating as an indicator. In Torrance, M. & Galbraith, D. (Eds.), Knowing what to write: Conceptual processes in text production (pp. 6577). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Van Waes, L., & Leijten, M. (2015). Fluency in writing: A multidimensional perspective on writing fluency applied to L1 and L2. Computers and Composition, 38, 7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Waes, L., & Schellens, P. J. (2003). Writing profiles: The effect of the writing mode on pausing and revision patterns of experienced writers. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 829853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Weijen, D., Van den Bergh, H., Rijlaarsdam, G., & Sanders, T. (2008). Differences in process and process-product relations in L2 writing. ITL - International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 156, 203226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wengelin, A. (2006). Examining pauses in writing: Theory, methods and empirical data. In Sullivan, K. P. H. & Lindgren, E. (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging: Methods and applications (pp. 107130). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wengelin, A. (2007). The word level focus in text production by adults with reading and writing difficulties. In Torrance, M., van Waes, L., & Galbraith, D. (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (pp. 6782). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wengelin, A., Torrance, M., Holmqvist, K., Simpson, S., Galbraith, D., Johansson, V., & Johansson, R. (2009). Combined eyetracking and keystroke-logging methods for studying cognitive processes in text production. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 337351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, C., & Ding, Y. (2014). An exploratory study of pauses in computer-assisted EFL writing. Language Learning and Technology, 18, 8096.Google Scholar
Xu, C., & Qi, Y. (2017). Analyzing pauses in computer-assisted EFL writing: A computer-keystroke-log perspective. Journal of Educational Technology and Society, 20, 2434.Google Scholar
Zhang, M., & Deane, P. (2015). Process features in writing: Internal structure and incremental value over product features (Research Report No. RR-15-02). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar