Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T19:57:32.778Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

SCHEMATIC DIAGRAMS IN SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING OF ENGLISH PREPOSITIONS

A BEHAVIORAL AND EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL STUDY

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 January 2020

Helen Zhao
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Shuting Huang
Affiliation:
South China Normal University
Yacong Zhou
Affiliation:
Huanghe University of Science and Technology
Ruiming Wang*
Affiliation:
South China Normal University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ruiming Wang, South China Normal University, School of Psychology, Guangzhou, China 510631. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

In the current study of applied cognitive linguistics (CL), schematic diagrams that represent generalizations of physical-spatial experience were applied in a computer-based tutor that trained English prepositions for second language (L2) learners. Behavioral and electrophysiological (ERP) measures were used to examine whether schematic-diagram feedback provided by the tutor had an instructional advantage over the minimally informed correctness feedback. Behavioral results confirmed this prediction and further revealed that the treatment difference was more striking when the participants had a lower L2 proficiency. The ERP results also supported the prediction. Violation uses of prepositions yielded an N270 and an N400. Schematic-diagram feedback motivated significant changes in brain potentials, whereas correctness feedback failed to do so. Overall, our findings suggest that CL-inspired instruction of a relatively short duration led to significant improvements in learners’ behavioral productive performance and in their sensitivity to semantic violation of preposition use during online sentence processing. The study provided strong neurolinguistic evidence for CL-inspired pedagogy in supporting L2 learning.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This project was funded by the National Social Science Foundation of China (19AYY009). We thank Man Ho Ivy Wong for her help in developing the experimental stimuli, and John Kowalski for his help programming the study.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, J. R. (1995). Developing expertise. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive psychology and its implications (4th ed., pp. 272304). New York, NY: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Arnett, C., & Deifel, K. (2015). A cognitive linguistic approach to two-way prepositions in L2 German. In Masuda, K. et al. (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and sociocultural theory: Applications for second and foreign language teaching (pp. 183201). Berlin, Germany, and Boston, MA: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Becker, A., & Carroll, M. (1997). The acquisition of spatial relations in a second language. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, M. A., Duke, P. A., & Fuggetta, G. (2014). Event-related potential N270 delayed and enhanced by the conjunction of relevant and irrelevant perceptual mismatch. Psychophysiology, 51, 46463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Besson, M., Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C. (1992). An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 132149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boers, F. (2011). Cognitive semantic ways of teaching figurative phrases: An assessment. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9, 227261.Google Scholar
Brugman, C. (1988). The story of “over”: Polysemy, semantic and the structure of the lexicon. New York, NY, and London, UK: Garland.Google Scholar
Buescher, K., & Strauss, S. (2015). A cognitive linguistic analysis of French preposition à, dans, and en and a sociocultural theoretical approach to teaching them. In Masuda, K. et al. (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and sociocultural theory: Applications for second and foreign language teaching (pp. 155181). Berlin, Germany, and Boston, MA: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Celce-Murcia, M., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (1999). The grammar book: An ESL/EFL teacher’s course (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Heinle & Heinle.Google Scholar
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 161174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, D., Dynowska, A., Ritter, W., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2004). Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: Implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. Psychophysiology, 41, 6074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Knop, S., & Dirven, R. (2008). Motion and location events in German, French and English: A typological, contrastive and pedagogical approach. In De Knop, S. & De Rycker, T. (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to pedagogical grammar: A volume in honor of René Dirven (pp. 295324). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dodge, E., & Lakoff, G. (2005). Image schemas: From linguistic analysis to neural grounding. In Beate, H. & Grady, J. E. (Eds.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp. 5791). Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C., Osterhout, L., McLaughlin, J., & Foucart, A. (2008). The effect of phonological realization of inflectional morphology on verbal agreement in French: Evidence from ERPs. Acta Psychologica, 128, 528536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, L., & Meng, G. (2010). A study on the effect of metaphor awareness raising on Chinese EFL learners’ vocabulary acquisition and retention. Canadian Social Science, 6, 110124.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2005). The psychological status of image schemas. In Beate, H. & Grady, J. E. (Eds.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp. 113135). Berlin: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glover, S., Rosenbaum, D. A., Graham, J., & Dixon, P. (2004). Grasping the meaning of words. Experimental Brain Research, 15, 103108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 16311643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huddleston, R., & Pullum, G. (2002). The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hurtienne, J. (2011). Image schemas and design for intuitive use. Berlin, Germany: Doctoral Thesis Techniscie Universitat.Google Scholar
Jacobsen, N. D. (2018). The best of both worlds: Combining cognitive linguistics and pedagogical tasks to teach English conditionals. Applied Linguistics, 39, 668693.Google Scholar
Jiang, N. (2013). Conducting reaction time research in second language studies. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination and reason. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 217265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume II: Descriptive application. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lantolf, J. (2000). Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, H. (2016). Concept-based instruction: Imagistic and metaphorical understanding of phrasal verbs. English Teaching, 71, 167191.Google Scholar
Lindstromberg, S. (2010). English prepositions explained. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ma, S. (2007). Acquisition of English spatial prepositional semantics by Chinese EFL learners. Modern Foreign Languages, 2, 173183.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2012). The logic of the Unified Model. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 211227). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mahpeykar, N., & Tyler, A. (2015). A principled cognitive linguistics account of English phrasal verbs with up and out. Language and Cognition, 7, 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuda, K., & Labarca, A. (2015). Schematic diagram use and languaging quality in learning Japanese polysemous particles ni and de. In Masuda, K. et al. (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and sociocultural theory: Applications for second and foreign language teaching (pp. 203231). Berlin, Germany, and Boston, MA: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. (2012). Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 933947.Google ScholarPubMed
Morimoto, S., & Loewen, S. (2007). A comparison of the effects of image-schema-based instruction and translation-based instruction on the acquisition of L2 polysemous words. Language Teaching Research, 11, 347372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munnich, E., & Landau, B. (2010). Developmental decline in the acquisition of spatial language. Language Learning and Development, 6, 3259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munnich, E., Landau, B., & Dosher, B. (2001). Spatial language and spatial representation: A crosslinguistic comparison. Cognition, 81, 171208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113.Google ScholarPubMed
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pederson, E., Danzinger, E., Wilkins, D., Levinson, S., Kita, S., and Senft, G. (1998). Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization. Language, 74, 557589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London, UK: Longman.Google Scholar
Richardson, D. C., Spivey, M. J., Barsalou, L. W., & McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27, 767780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in L2 learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.Google Scholar
Stanfield, R., & Zwaan, R. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12, 153156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, A. (2012). Cognitive linguistics and second language learning: Theoretical basics and experimental evidence. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A., & Evans, V. (2003). The semantics of English prepositions: Spatial scenes, embodied meanings and cognition . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A., & Jan, H. (2017). Be going to and will: Talking about the future using embodied experience. Language and Cognition, 9, 412445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A., Mueller, C., & Ho, V. (2010). Applying cognitive linguistics to instructed L2 learning: The English modals. AILA Review, 23, 3049.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., Mueller, C., & Ho, V. (2011). Applying cognitive linguistics to learning the semantics of English prepositions to, for, and at: An experimental investigation. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 8, 180205.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., Huang, L., & Jan, H., (Eds.) (2018). What is applied cognitive linguistics? Answers from current SLA research. Berlin, Germany, and Boston, MA: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., Kutas, M., Kluender, R., Mitchiner, M., & McIsaac, H. (1991). Fractioning the word repetition effect with event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 131150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, H., Wang, Y., Kong, J., Cui, L., & Tian, S. (2001). Enhancement of conflict processing activity in human brain under task relevant condition. Neuroscience Letters, 298, 155158.Google ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Tian, S., Wang, H., Cui, L., Zhang, Y., & Zhang, X. (2003). Event-related potentials evoked by multi-feature conflict under different attentive conditions. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 451457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, M. H. I., Zhao, H., & MacWhinney, B. (2018). A cognitive linguistics application for second language pedagogy: The English preposition tutor. Language Learning, 68, 438468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, J., & Wang, Y. (2002). Event-related potentials elicited by stimulus spatial discrepancy in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 326, 7376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zane, E., & Shafer, V. (2018). Mixed metaphors: Electrophysiological brain responses to (un)expected concrete and abstract prepositional phrases. Brain Research, 1680, 7792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, X., Wang, Y., Li, S., Wang, L., & Tian, S. (2005). Distinctive conflict processes associated with different stimulus presentation patterns: An event-related potential study. Experimental Brain Research, 162, 503508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y., Wang, Y., Wang, H., Cui, L., Tian, S., & Wang, D. (2001). Different processes are involved in human brain for shape and face comparisons. Neuroscience Letters, 303, 157160.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhao, H., & MacWhinney, B. (2018). The instructed learning of form-function mappings in the English article system. Modern Language Journal, 102, 99119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zheng, Y., & Cheng, L. (2008). College English Test (CET) in China. Language Testing, 25, 408417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Magliano, J., & Graesser, A. (1995). Dimensions of situated model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 386397.Google Scholar