We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Barnitz, J. G. (1986). Toward understanding the effects of cross-cultural schemata and discourse structure on second language reading comprehension. Journal of Reading Behavior, 18(2), 95–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, W. S., & Guthrie, L. F. (1980). On the dialect question and reading. In Spiro, R. J., Bruce, B., & Brewer, W. (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kavanagh, J. F., & Venezky, R. L. (Eds.) (1980). Orthography, reading, and dyslexia. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E., May, W. H., & Miron, M. S. (1975). Cross-cultural universals of affective meaning. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Spiro, R. J., Bruce, B., & Brewer, W. (Eds.). (1980). Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, L. (Eds.). (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar