Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T17:36:22.709Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language lateralization in adult bilinguals*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2008

Marjorie B. Wesche
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa
Eta I. Schneiderman
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa

Abstract

Recent experimental studies of bilinguals have provided evidence of less left lateralization (i.e. greater right hemisphere participation) for verbal tasks in the second language than in the first. Other clinical and experimental studies of adults suggest that the normal adult right hemisphere has certain residual language-related capacities and that it plays a role in the early stages of both child and adult language acquisition. These findings lead us to postulate a link between right hemisphere involvement in the early stages of first and second language acquisition.

Two studies were designed to investigate this issue. The subjects of the studies were adult French-English bilinguals, with one group dominant in English and a second in French. A single word dichotic listening paradigm was employed in both studies. Following Obler (1981), the hypotheses were 1) that the second language dichotic task would show less left lateralization (greater right hemisphere involvement) than the first and 2) that greater proficiency in the second language would correlate with a higher degree of left lateralization (less right hemisphere involvement) on the dichotic test in that language. The studies provide some support for these hypotheses and thus, indirectly, for a unified theory of first and second language acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, M., and Obler, L.. 1978. The bilingual brain: neuropsychological and neurolinguistic aspects of bilingualism. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Asher, J.f. 1977. Learning another language through actions. Los Gatos, California: Sky Oaks Productions.Google Scholar
Bakker, D.J. 1981. A set of brains for learning to read. Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude, ed. by Diller, K., 6571. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Barton, M.; Goodglass, H.; and Shai, A.. 1965. Differential recognition of tachistoscopically presented English and Hebrew words in right and left visual fields. Perception and Motor Skills 21. 431437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basser, L.S. 1962. Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain 85.427460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentin, S. 1980. Right hemisphere role in reading a second language. Paper read at the Symposium on Cerebral Lateralization in Bilingualism, BABBLE Conference,Niagara Falls, New York.Google Scholar
Blumstein, S. and Cooper, W.E.. 1974. Hemispheric processing of intonation contours. Cortex 10.146158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryden, M.P. 1978. Strategy effects in the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. Strategies of information processing, ed. by Underwood, G., 117149. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bryden, M.P. 1980. Strategy and attentional influences on dichotic listening and tachistoscopic lateralization assessments. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychology Society,San Francisco, California.Google Scholar
Burt, M.K., and Dulay, H.C.. 1980. On acquisition orders. Second Language development: trends and issues, ed. by Felix, S., 265328. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag.Google Scholar
Carroll, F.W. 1978a. Cerebral dominance for language: a dichotic listening study of Navajo-English bilinguals. The bilingual in a pluralistic society: Proceedings of the Sixth Southwest Area Language and Linguistics Workshop, ed. by Key, H., McCullough, S., and Sawyer, J., 1117. Long Beach: California State University.Google Scholar
Carroll, F.W. 1978b. The other side of the brain and foreign language learning. Paper presented at the TESOL Conference,Mexico City, Mexico.Google Scholar
Cutting, J.E. 1974. Two left hemisphere mechanisms in speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics 16.601612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, M. 1980. Language acquisition in a single hemisphere: semantic organization. Biological studies of mental processes, ed. by Caplan, D., 159186. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennis, M., and Kohn, B.. 1975. Comprehension of syntax in infantile hemiplegics after cerebral hemidecortication: left hemisphere superiority. Brain and Language 2.472482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., and Whitaker, H.A.. 1976. Language acquisition following hemidecortication: linguistic superiority of the left over the right hemisphere. Brain and Language 3.404433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galloway, L. 1980. Clinical evidence: polyglot aphasia. Paper read at the Symposium on Cerebral Lateralization in Bilingualism, BABBLE Conference,Niagara Falls, New York.Google Scholar
Galloway, L., and Krashen, S.. 1980. Cerebral Organization in bilingualism and second language acquisition. Research in second language acquisition, ed. by Krashen, S., and Scarcella, R., 7480. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Galloway, L., and Scarcella, R.. 1979. Cerebral organization in second language acquisition. Paper presented at the Winter Meeting of the Linguistics Society of America,New York, New York.Google Scholar
Gary, J.O. 1978. Why speak if you don't need to? The case for a listening approach to beginning foreign language learning. Second language acquisition research: issues and implications, ed. by Ritchie, W.C., 185199. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gaziel, T.; Obler, L.; Bentin, S.; and Albert, M.. 1977. The dynamics of lateralization in second language learning: sex and proficiency effects. Paper read at Boston University Conference on Language Development,Boston, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M.S., and Hillyard, S.A.. 1971. Language and speech capacity of the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 9.271280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F.; Hamers, J.; Lambert, W.E.; Mononen, L.; Seitz, M.; and Starck, R.. 1978. Language processing in bilinguals. Brain and Language 5.112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, H. and Carmon, A.. 1976. Transfer of dominance in speed of verbal response to visually presented stimuli from right to left hemisphere. Perception and Motor Skills 42.10911100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gott, P.S. 1973. Language after dominant hemispherectomy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 36.10821088.Google ScholarPubMed
Hardyck, C. 1980. Hemisphere differences and language ability. Paper read at the Symposium on Bilingualism and Brain Function, Summer Linguistics Institute, Albuquerque, New Mexico.Google Scholar
Hynd, G.W., and Scott, S.A., (in press). Propositional and appositional modes of thought and differential cerebral speech lateralization in Navaho Indian and Anglo Children. Child Development.Google Scholar
Hynd, G.W.; Teeter, A.; and Stewart, J.. 1980. Acculturation and the lateralization of speech in the bilingual native American. International Journal of Neuroscience 20.001007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotik, B. 1975. Lateralization in bilinguals. Unpublished thesis: Moscow State University, USSR.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. 1973. Lateralization, language learning, and the critical period: some new evidence. Language Learning 23(1).6374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. 1981. Language acquisition and second language learning, Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Krashen, S., and Galloway, L.. 1978. The neurological correlates of language acquisition: current research. SPEAQ Journal 2.2135.Google Scholar
Krashen, S., and Scarcella, R.. 1978. On routines and patterns in language acquisition and performance. Language Learning 28(2).283300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamm, O. 1978. Interhemispheric differences in operating formal symbolic systems. Unpublished M.S. Thesis. Technion Medical School. Haifa, Israel.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. 1967. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley and Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, J., and Trevarthen, C.. 1977. Perceptual, semantic and phonetic aspects of elementary language processes. Brain 100.105118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, J.C.; Caplan, D.; and Holmes, J.M.. 1975. The measure of laterality. Neuro-psychologia 13.315322.Google ScholarPubMed
Maitre, S. 1974. On the representation of second language in the brain. Unpublished M.A. Thesis. University of California at Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Neufeld, G.G. 1977. Language learning ability in adults: a study on the acquisition of prosodic and articulatory features. Working papers on bilingualism 12.4560.Google Scholar
Neufeld, G.G., and Schneiderman, E.I.. 1980. Prosodic and articulatory features in adult language learning. Research in second language acquisition: selected papers of the Los Angeles Second Language Research Forum, ed. by Scarcella, R.C. and Krashen, S.D., 105109. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Nord, J.R. 1980. Developing listening fluency before speaking: an alternative paradigm. System 8.122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obler, L.K. 1981. Right hemisphere participation in second language acquisition. Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude, ed. by Diller, K., 5364. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Obler, L.K. (in press). The neuropsychology of bilingualism. Proceedings of the Biological Perspectives on Language Symposium in Montreal, 1981.Google Scholar
Obler, L.K.; Albert, M.; and Gordon, H.. 1975. Asymmetry of cerebral dominance in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Paper read at Thirteenth Annual Meeting of the Academy of Aphasia,Victoria, Canada.Google Scholar
Obler, L.K.; Zatorre, R.J.; Galloway, L., and Vaid, J.. (in press). Cerebral lateralization in bilinguals: methodological issues. Brain and Language.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. 1977. Bilingualism and aphasia. Studies in neurolinguistics, ed. by H.A. Whitaker and H.A. Whitaker 3.65121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, A. 1977. Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Language 53.560573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postovsky, V.A. 1974. Effects of delay in oral practice at the beginning of second language learning. Modern Language Journal LVIII 5–6.229239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapport, R.L.; Tan, C.T.; and Whitaker, H.A.. 1980. Language function among Chinese polyglots. Paper presented at the Academy of Aphasia, Fall River, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Rogers, L.; Houten, W. Ten; Kaplan, C.D.; and Gardiner, M.. 1977. Hemispheric specialization of language: an EEG study of bilingual Hopi Indian children. International Journal of Neuro-science 17.8992.Google Scholar
Russell, R., and Espir, M.. 1961. Traumatic Aphasia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sanches, M. 1978. Brain function and language acquisition: the evidence from Japanese. Language Sciences 1.3549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneiderman, E.I., and Wesche, M.B.. (in press). The role of the right hemisphere in second language acquisition. Selected papers of the Third Los Angeles Second Language Research Forum, ed. by Bailey, K.M., Long, M.H., and Peck, S.. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Scott, S.A.; Hynd, G.W.; Hunt, L.; and Weed, W.. 1979. Cerebral speech lateralization in the native American Navaho. Neuropsychologia 17.8992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N., and Orr, C.. 1981. How to measure individual differences in brain lateralization: demonstration of a paradigm. Paper presented at the International Neuropsychological Society Meeting,Atlanta, Georgia.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, S.J. 1979. Infant cerebral asymmetries and developmental models of brain lateralization. Paper read at the 40th Annual Meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association,Quebec, Canada.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., and Studdert-Kennedy, M.. 1967. Identification of consonants and vowels presented to left and right ears. Quarterly Journal of Environmental Psychology 19.5963.Google ScholarPubMed
Sibatani, A. 1980. The Japanese brain. Science 1(8).2327.Google Scholar
Silverberg, R.; Bentin, S.; Gaziel, T.; Obler, L.K.; and Albert, M.L.. 1979. Shift of visual field preference for English words in native Hebrew speakers. Brain and Language 8.184190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverberg, R.; Gordon, H.; Pollock, S.; and Bentin, S.. 1980. Shift of visual field preference for Hebrew words in native speakers learning to read. Brain and Language 11.99105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. 1966. Speech and other functions after left (dominant) hemispherectomy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 29.467471.Google ScholarPubMed
Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Shankweiler, D.. 1970. Hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 48.2(2).579594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sussman, H.; Franklin, P.; and Simon, T., (in press). Bilingual speech: bilateral control? Brain and Language.Google Scholar
Vaid, J. (in press). Bilingualism and brain lateralization. Language functions and brain organization, ed. by Segalowitz, S.J.. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vaid, J., and Genesee, F.. (in press). Neuropsychological approaches to bilingualism: a critical review. Canadian Journal of Psychology.Google Scholar
Vaid, J., and Lambert, W.E. 1979. Cerebral involvement in the cognitive functioning of bilinguals. Brain and Language 8.92110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, J., and Zatorre, R.. 1978. Laterality differences for word identification in bilinguals. Brain and Language 2.158167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, H. 1961. Cerebral hemispherectomy in the treatment of infantile hemeplegia. Confinia Neurologica 21.150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witelson, S.F. 1977. Early hemisphere specialization and inter-hemispheric plasticity: an empirical and theoretical review. Language development and neurological theory, ed. by Segalowitz, S.J. and Gruber, F.A.. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Woods, B.T. 1980. Observations on the neurological basis for initial language. Biological studies of mental processes, ed. by Caplan, D., 149158, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zaidel, E. 1973. Linguistic competence and related functions in the right cerebral hemisphere of man following commissurotomy and hemispherectomy. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, California Institute of Technology. Pasadena.Google Scholar
Zaidel, E. 1978. Lexical organization in the right hemisphere. Cerebral correlates of conscious experience, ed. by Buser, P., and Rougeul-Buser, A.. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Zurif, E. 1974. Auditory lateralization. Brain and Language 1.391404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar