Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T13:45:15.721Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION IN BILINGUALS

EYE-TRACKING EVIDENCE THAT L2 PROFICIENCY IMPACTS ACCESS OF L1 PHONOTACTICS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2021

Max R. Freeman*
Affiliation:
St. John’s University
Viorica Marian
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Max R. Freeman, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, St. John’s University, 8000 Utopia Parkway, St. John Hall, Room 344, Jamaica, NY 11439. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

A bilingual’s language system is highly interactive. When hearing a second language (L2), bilinguals access native-language (L1) words that share sounds across languages. In the present study, we examine whether input modality and L2 proficiency moderate the extent to which bilinguals activate L1 phonotactic constraints (i.e., rules for combining speech sounds) during L2 processing. Eye movements of English monolinguals and Spanish–English bilinguals were tracked as they searched for a target English word in a visual display. On critical trials, displays included a target that conflicted with the Spanish vowel-onset rule (e.g., spa), as well as a competitor containing the potentially activated “e” onset (e.g., egg). The rule violation was processed either in the visual modality (Experiment 1) or audio-visually (Experiment 2). In both experiments, bilinguals with lower L2 proficiency made more eye movements to competitors than fillers. Findings suggest that bilinguals who have lower L2 proficiency access L1 phonotactic constraints during L2 visual word processing with and without auditory input of the constraint-conflicting structure (e.g., spa). We conclude that the interactivity between a bilingual’s two languages is not limited to words that share form across languages, but also extends to sublexical, rule-based structures.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Best, C. T. (1994). The emergence of native-language phonological influences in infants: A perceptual assimilation modelThe Development of Speech Perception: The Transition From Speech Sounds to Spoken Words167, 233277.Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2, 633660. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960601000746 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013) Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 547567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2013). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.3.47. http://www.praat.org/ Google Scholar
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, M. T. (2018). Making room for second language phonotactics: Effects of L2 learning and environment on first language speech perceptionLanguage and Speech, 61, 598614. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830918767208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, M. T., Goldrick, M., Blasingame, M., & Fink, A. (2016). Navigating conflicting phonotactic constraints in bilingual speech perception. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition , 19, 939954. https://doi.org10.1017/S1366728915000334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chabal, S., & Marian, V. (2015). Speakers of different languages process the visual world differentlyJournal of Experimental Psychology: General144, 539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). PPVT-III: Peabody picture vocabulary test. American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Lugo, P., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Vocabulario en imágenes Peabody (TVIP) . American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dupoux, E., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Navarrete, E., & Peperkamp, S. (2008). Persistent stress “deafness”: The case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition, 106, 682706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkbeiner, M., Forster, K., Nicol, J., & Nakamura, K. (2004). The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzpatrick, I., & Indefrey, P. (2010). Lexical competition in nonnative speech comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 11651178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2016). Phonotactic constraints are activated across languages in bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 702. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2021). First-language influence on second language speech perception depends on task demands. Journal of Language and Speech. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830920983368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giezen, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., Shook, A., Marian, V., & Emmorey, K. (2015). Parallel language activation and inhibitory control in bimodal bilingualsCognition141, 925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J., Midgley, K., & Holcomb, P. J. (2010). Re-thinking the bilingual interactive activation model from a developmental perspective (BIA-d). In Kail, M. & Hickmann, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition across linguistic and cognitive systems (pp. 267284). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallé, P. A., Dominguez, A., Cuetos, F., & Segui, J. (2008). Phonological mediation in visual masked priming: Evidence from phonotactic repair. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 177192.Google ScholarPubMed
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ju, M., & Luce, P. A. (2004). Falling on sensitive ears. Psychological Science , 15, 314318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2007). Non-target language recognition and interference: Evidence from eye-tracking and picture naming. Language Learning, 57, 119163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., and Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception 36 ECVP Abstract Supplement.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lentz, T. O., & Kager, R. W. (2015). Categorical phonotactic knowledge filters second language input, but probabilistic phonotactic knowledge can still be acquiredLanguage and Speech58, 387413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F. (2008). Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. Mental Lexicon, 3, 349374. https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.3.3.06lin CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loebell, H., & Bock, K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 41, 791824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Bartolotti, J., Chabal, S., and Shook, A. (2012). CLEARPOND: Cross-Linguistic Easy Access Resource for Phonological and Orthographic Neighborhood Densities. PLoS ONE, 7, e43230. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043230 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50, 940967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V, & Spivey, M. (2003a). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V, & Spivey, M. (2003b). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within-and between- language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martín, M. C., Macizo, P., & Bajo, T. (2010). Time course of inhibitory processes in bilingual language processing. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 679693. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609X480571 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D. (2014). Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 89117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. CRC Press.Google Scholar
Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differencesJournal of Memory and Language59, 475494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mishra, R. K., & Singh, N. (2016). The influence of second language proficiency on bilingual parallel language activation in Hindi–English bilingualsJournal of Cognitive Psychology28, 396411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism (Vol. 18). John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parlato-Oliveira, E., Christophe, A., Hirose, Y., & Dupoux, E. (2010). Plasticity of illusory vowel perception in Brazilian-Japanese bilingualsThe Journal of the Acoustical Society of America127, 37383748. http://doi.org/10.1121/1.3327792 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PsychCorp. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Duyck, W., Brysbaert, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2009). Semantic and translation priming from a first language to a second and back: Making sense of the findings. Memory and Cognition, 37, 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2013). The bilingual language interaction network for comprehension of speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 304324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2019). Covert co-activation of bilinguals’ non-target language: Phonological competition from translations. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9, 228252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, M. K., Lin, F. C., Ward, S. E., & Fine, J. P. (2013). Composite variables: When and howNursing Research62, 4549. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0b013e3182741948 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sunderman, G., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). First language activation during second language lexical processing: An investigation of lexical form, meaning, and grammatical classStudies in Second Language Acquisition28, 387422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1253012535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Hell, J. G., & Tanner, D. (2012). Second language proficiency and cross-language lexical activation. Language Learning62, 148171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veivo, O., Porretta, V., Hyönä, J., & Järvikivi, J. (2018). Spoken second language words activate native language orthographic information in late second language learnersApplied Psycholinguistics39, 10111032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viviani, P. (1990). Eye movements in visual search: Cognitive, perceptual and motor control aspects. Reviews of Oculomotor Research, 4, 353.Google ScholarPubMed
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2006). First-language phonotactics in second-language listening. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 597607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., and Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities . Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., and Mather, N. (2007). Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities . Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar