Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T11:11:56.976Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social Comparisons are Associated with Poorer and Riskier Financial Decision Making, no Matter whether Encounters are Sporadic or Repeated

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 September 2016

Alfonso Barrós-Loscertales
Affiliation:
Universitat Jaume I (Spain)
Antonio M. Espín*
Affiliation:
Middlesex University Business School (UK)
José C. Perales
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Antonio M. Espín. Economics Department. Middlesex University Business School. Hendon Campus, The Burroughs. NW4 4BT. London (United Kingdom). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Previous research suggests that social comparisons affect decision making under uncertainty. However, the role of the length of the social interaction for this relationship remains unknown. This experiment tests the effect of social comparisons on financial risk taking and how this effect is modulated by whether social encounters are sporadic or repeated. Participants carried out a computer task consisting of a series of binary choices between lotteries of varying profitability and risk, with real monetary stakes. After each decision, participants could compare their own payoff to that of a counterpart who made the same decision at the same time and whose choices/earnings did not affect the participants’ earnings. The design comprised three between-subjects treatments which differed in the nature of the social interaction: participants were informed that they would be matched with either (a) a different participant in each trial, (b) the same participant across all trials, or (c) a “virtual participant”, i.e., a computer algorithm. Compared to the non-social condition (c), subjects in both social conditions (a and b) chose lotteries with lower expected value (z = –3.10, p < .01) and higher outcome variance (z = 2.13, p = .03). However, no differences were found between the two social conditions (z = 1.15, p = .25 and z = 0.35, p = .73, respectively). These results indicate that social comparison information per se leads to poorer and riskier financial decisions, irrespective of whether or not the referent other is encountered repeatedly.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ai, C., & Norton, E. C. (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters, 80(1), 123129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1765(03)00032-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, P. D. (2009). Fixed effects regression models (Series: Quantitative applications in the social sciences, Vol. 160). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bault, N., Coricelli, G., & Rustichini, A. (2008). Interdependent utilities: How social ranking affects choice behavior. PloS One, 3, e3477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003477 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bault, N., Joffily, M., Rustichini, A., & Coricelli, G. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex and striatum mediate the influence of social comparison on the decision process. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1604416049. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100892108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brañas-Garza, P., Espín, A. M., Exadaktylos, F., & Herrmann, B. (2014). Fair and unfair punishers coexist in the Ultimatum Game. Scientific Reports, 4, 6025. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep06025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camille, N., Coricelli, G., Sallet, J., Pradat-Diehl, P., Duhamel, J. R., & Sirigu, A. (2004). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science, 304, 11671170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1094550 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cikara, M., Botvinick, M. M., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). Us versus them social identity shapes neural responses to intergroup competition and harm. Psychological Science, 22, 306313. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610397667 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. A. (2008). Relative income, happiness, and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46(1), 95144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/jel.46.1.95 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobo-Reyes, R., & Jiménez, N. (2012). The dark side of friendship: ‘Envy’. Experimental Economics, 15, 547570. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-012-9313-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., & Hernán-González, R. (2015). The cognitive basis of social behavior: Cognitive reflection overrides antisocial but not always prosocial motives. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 287. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00287 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dal Bó, P. (2005). Cooperation under the shadow of the future: Experimental evidence from infinitely repeated games. American Economic Review, 95, 15911604. http://dx.doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR34573.v1 Google Scholar
Delgado, M. R., Schotter, A., Ozbay, E. Y., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Understanding overbidding: Using the neural circuitry of reward to design economic auctions. Science, 321, 18491852. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1158860 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Fliessbach, K., Sunde, U., & Weber, B. (2011). Relative versus absolute income, joy of winning, and gender: Brain imaging evidence. Journal of Public Economics, 95, 279285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2010.11.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dvash, J., Gilam, G., Ben-Ze’ev, A., Hendler, T., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2010). The envious brain: The neural basis of social comparison. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 17411750. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20972 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J. R. (2001). Ten difference score myths. Organizational Research Methods, 4, 265287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109442810143005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espín, A. M., Brañas-Garza, P., Herrmann, B., & Gamella, J. F. (2012). Patient and impatient punishers of free-riders. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 49234928. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fareri, D. S., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). Differential reward responses during competition against in-and out-of-network others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 412420. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature02043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817868. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/003355399556151 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fliessbach, K., Weber, B., Trautner, P., Dohmen, T., Sunde, U., Elger, C. E., & Falk, A. (2007). Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum. Science, 318, 13051308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1145876 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gächter, S., Nosenzo, D., & Sefton, M. (2012). The impact of social comparisons on reciprocity. The Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 114, 13461367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9442.2012.01730.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gali, J. (1994). Keeping up with the Joneses: Consumption externalities, portfolio choice, and asset prices. Journal of Money, Credit and Banking, 26(1), 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2078030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiner, B. (2004). An online recruitment system for economic experiments. In Kremer, K. & Macho, V. (Eds.), Forschung und Wissenschaftliches Rechnen GWDG Bericht 63 [Research and Scientific Computation GWDG Report 63]. Göttingen, Germany: Gesellschaft für Wissenschaftliche Datenverarbeitung.Google Scholar
Herrmann, B., & Orzen, H. (2008). The appearance of homo rivalis: Social preferences and the nature of rent seeking. CeDEx Discussion Paper Series (#2008–10). Nottingham, UK: University of Nottingham. Retrieved from http://nottingham.ac.uk/cedex/documents/papers/2008-10.pdf Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Social utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 426441. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.426 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lugovskyy, V., Puzzello, D., & Tucker, S. (2010). An experimental investigation of overdissipation in the all pay auction. European Economic Review, 54, 974997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2010.02.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luttmer, E. F. P. (2005). Neighbors as negatives: Relative earnings and well-being. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 120, 9631002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/qje/120.3.963 Google Scholar
Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489, 427430. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature11467 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheremeta, R. M. (2010). Experimental comparison of multi-stage and one-stage contests. Games and Economic Behavior, 68, 731747. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2009.08.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheremeta, R. M. (2013). Overbidding and heterogeneous behavior in contest experiments. Journal of Economic Surveys, 27, 491514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/joes.12022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Bos, W., Golka, P. J. M., Effelsberg, D., & McClure, S. M. (2013). Pyrrhic victories: The need for social status drives costly competitive behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 189. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00189 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Bos, W., Li, J., Lau, T., Maskin, E., Cohen, J. D., Montague, P. R., & McClure, S. M. (2008). The value of victory: Social origins of the winner’s curse in common value auctions. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Bos, W., Talwar, A., & McClure, S. M. (2013). Neural correlates of reinforcement learning and social preferences in competitive bidding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 21372146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3095-12.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Huizenga, H. M., Somerville, L. H., Delgado, M. R., Powers, A., Weeda, W. D., … Figner, B. (2015). Neural correlates of expected risks and returns in risky choice across development. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 15491560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1924-14.2015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veblen, T. (1899). The theory of the leisure class. New York, NY: The New American Library.Google Scholar
Weber, E. U., Shafir, S., & Blais, A. R. (2004). Predicting risk sensitivity in humans and lower animals: Risk as variance or coefficient of variation. Psychological Review, 111, 430445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.430 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 10491062. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed