Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:22:23.508Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Short-Term Effects of the Empathic Talk: The Role of Perceived Empathy, Interpersonal Dispositions, and Vagal Tone on Experiencing Depth

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2017

Michele Dufey*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Chile (Chile)
Juan Enrique Wilson
Affiliation:
Universidad de Chile (Chile)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr. Michele Dufey. Assistant professor. Psychology Department. Universidad de Chile. Capitan Ignacio Carrera Pinto, 1045. 1025000. Nunoa Santiago de Chile (Chile). Phone: +56–229772420. E-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]

Abstract

The present study examines the immediate impact of empathic attitudes on the receiver, by comparing an empathic conversation (EC) with a neutral one (NC) on experiencing depth (EXP), perceived empathy and vagal tone. We also evaluate if empathy effects depend on personality dispositions relevant to interpersonal functioning. An experimental, counterbalanced, within-subject design was implemented wherein participants (n = 27, age M = 22.6, SD = 4.0, 52% females) talked about a personal, meaningful topic for 20 minutes under both the EC and NC conditions. At the group level, main results indicated that the EC was more effective than the NC in promoting increases in EXP over time (F(1, 25) = 21.04, p < .001, ηp2 = .457) and perceived empathy in women (F(1, 25) = 9.42, p = .005, ηp2 = .264). At the individual level, the NC particularly precluded EXP gains in people expressing a better interpersonal functioning (βs < –.46, ps < .05 for attachment security and empathy and β = .38, p < .05 for aggression), and who also inhibited their vagal tone throughout this very condition (β = .40, p = .024). We conclude that the empathic context has an immediate positive impact on healthy psychological variables, whereas a more impersonal setting would drive people away from this positive effect. Those results are discussed in the light of the role of interpersonal proximity (being empathic to another’s experience and the lack of this attitude) in psychotherapy and healthy functioning.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

How to cite this article:

Dufey, M., & Wilson, J. E. (2017). Short-term effects of the empathic talk: the role of perceived empathy, interpersonal dispositions, and vagal tone on experiencing depth. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20. e61. Doi:10.1017/sjp.2017.58

References

Allen, J. J. B., Chambers, A. S., & Towers, D. N. (2007). The many metrics of cardiac chronotropy: A pragmatic primer and a brief comparison of metrics. Biological Psychology, 74, 243262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.08.005 Google Scholar
Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology, 8, 291322. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291 Google Scholar
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1986). The Relationship Inventory now: Issues and advances in theory, method, and use. In Greenberg, L. S. & Pinsof, W. M. (Eds.), The psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook (pp. 439476). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183214. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401002012 Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B, & Lang, P. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 3, 276298. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.276 Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 4959. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 Google Scholar
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452 Google Scholar
Carter, C. S., Harris, J., & Porges, S. W. (2009). Neural and evolutionary perspectives on empathy. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 169182), Cambridge, UK: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Castonguay, L. G., Goldfried, M. R., Wiser, S., Raue, P. J., & Hayes, A. M. (1996). Predicting the effect of cognitive therapy for depression: A study of unique and common factors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 497504. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.497 Google Scholar
Celis, A. (1999). Proceso de adaptación del Inventario de la Relación de Ayuda de G. T. Barrett-Lennard [Adaptation of G. T. Barrett Lennard Relationship Inventory]. Revista de Psicología de la Universidad de Chile, 8(1), 163171.Google Scholar
Cheng, Y., Chou, K.-H., Decety, J., Chen, Y.-D., Hung, D., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Lin, C. P. (2009). Sex differences in the neuroanatomy of human mirror-neuron system: A voxel-based morphometric investigation. Neuroscience, 158, 713720. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.10.026 Google Scholar
Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 810832. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.810 Google Scholar
Cook, E. W., & Miller, G. A. (1992). Digital filtering: Background and tutorial for psychophysiologists. Psychophysiology, 29, 350367. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb01709.x Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). From emotional resonance to empathic understanding: A social neuroscience developmental account. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 10531080. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000503 Google Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 Google Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1996). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation and moral development. Annual Review in Psychology, 51, 665697. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665 Google Scholar
Elliot, R., Bohart, A. C., Watson, J. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (2011). Empathy. In Norcross, J. C. (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work. (2nd Ed., Kindle Edition). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fernández, A. M., & Dufey, M. (2015). Adaptation of Collins’ Dimensional Adult Attachment Scale revised to the Chilean context. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 28, 242252. https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528204 Google Scholar
Fernández, A. M., Dufey, M., & Kramp, U. (2011). Testing the psychometric properties of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) in Chile: Empathy in a different cultural context. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 27, 179185. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000065 Google Scholar
Figueroa, P., Ramírez, C. G., & Santis, M. (2005). Adaptación y validación del Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire a la población adolescente, adulta y adulta mayor del gran Santiago [Adaptation and validation of the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire to adolescent, adult and elder populations of Santiago]. (Unpublished undergraduate thesis), Universidad Diego Portales, Chile.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., … Gerber, A. (1996). The relationship of attachment status, psychiatric classification and response to psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 2231. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.22 Google Scholar
Geisler, F. C. M., Kubiak, T., Siewert, K., & Weber, H. (2013). Cardiac vagal tone is associated with social engagement and self-regulation. Biological Psychology, 93, 279286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.013 Google Scholar
Gendlin, E. T. (1990). The small steps of the therapy process: How they come and how to help them come. In Lietaer, G., Rombauts, J., & Van Balen, R. (Eds.), Client-Centered and Experiential Psychotherapy in the nineties (pp. 205224). Leuven, Belgiun: Leuven University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, G. A., & Anderson, T. (2007). Quality of object relations and security of attachment as predictors of early therapeutic alliance. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 111117. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.54.2.111 Google Scholar
Goldman, R. N., Greenberg, L. S., & Pos, A. E. (2005). Depth of emotional experience and outcome. Psychotherapy Research, 15, 248260. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503300512331385188 Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1985). A valid procedure for obtaining self-report of affect in marital interaction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 151160. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.53.2.151 Google Scholar
Greenberg, L. S. (2002). Integrating an emotion-focused approach to treatment into psychotherapy integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 12, 154189. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0479.12.2.154 Google Scholar
Hendicks, M. (2001). Focusing oriented/experiential therapy. In Cain, D. & Seeman, J. (Eds.), Humanistic psychotherapy: Handbook of research and practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Ickes, W., Marangoni, C., & Garcia, S. (1997). Studying empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant context. In Ickes, W. (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 282310). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Klein, M. H., Mathieu, P. L., Gendlin, E. T., & Kiesler, D. J. (1969). The Experiencing Scale: A research and training manual. Madison, WI: Wisconsin Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, M. (2005). When asking ‘‘why’’ does not hurt. Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01600.x Google Scholar
Kurtz, R. R., & Grummon, D. L. (1972). Different approaches to the measurement of therapist empathy and their relationship to therapy outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39, 106115. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033190 Google Scholar
Lambert, M., & Ogles, B. (2004). The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy. In Lambert, M. (Ed.), Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (pp. 139193). New York, NY: John Willey & Sons.Google Scholar
Luborsky, L. (1994). Therapeutic alliances as predictors of psychotherapy outcomes: Factors explaining the predictive success. In Horvath, A. O. & Greenberg, L. S. (Eds.), The working alliance: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 3850). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Malmo, R. M., Boag, T. J., & Smith, A. A. (1956). Physiological study of personal interaction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 19, 105119. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-195703000-00004 Google Scholar
Marci, C. D., Ham, J., Moran, E. B., & Orr, S. P. (2007). Physiological correlates of perceived therapist empathy and socio-emotional process during psychotherapy. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 103111. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.nmd.0000253731.71025.fc Google Scholar
Meredith, P. J., Strong, J., & Feeney, J. A. (2007). Adult attachment variables predict depression before and after treatment for chronic pain. European Journal of Pain, 11, 164170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2006.01.004 Google Scholar
Messina, I., Palmieri, A., Sambin, M., Kleinbub, J. R., Voci, A., & Calvo, V. (2013). Somatic underpinnings of perceived empathy: The importance of psychotherapy training. Psychotherapy Research, 23, 169177. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2012.748940 Google Scholar
Muran, J. C., Segal, Z. V., Samstag, L. W., & Crawford, C. E. (1994). Patient pretreatment interpersonal problems and therapeutic alliance in short-term cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 185190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.62.1.185 Google Scholar
Neumann, S. A., & Waldstein, S. R. (2001). Similar patterns of cardiovascular response during emotional activation as a function of affective valence and arousal and gender. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 50, 245253. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00198-2 Google Scholar
Oliveira-Silva, P., & Gonçalves, O. F. (2011). Responding empathically: A question of heart, not a question of skin. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36, 201207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-011-9161-2 Google Scholar
Orlinsky, D. E., Grawe, K., & Parks, B. K. (1994). Process and outcome in psychotherapy - Noch einmal. In Bergin, A. E. & Garfield, S. L. (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behaviour change (4th Ed., pp. 270376). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Pérez, A., & Sepúlveda, V. (1991). Estandarización de las escalas de evitación y ansiedad social (SAD) y temor a la evaluación negativa (FNE), y relación entre la ansiedad social y los comportamientos asertivo y agresivo [Standardization of Social Anxiety and Distress Scale (SAD) and Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (SAD), and the relationship between social anxiety, assertiveness and aggressive behavior]. (Unpublished undergraduate thesis), Universidad Católica de Chile, Chile.Google Scholar
Porges, S. W. (2003). The Polyvagal Theory: Phylogenetic contributions to social behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 79, 503513. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00156-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (2011). The Polyvagal Theory. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Rainville, P., Bechara, A., Naqvi, N., & Damasio, A. R. (2006). Basic emotions are associated with distinct patterns of cardiorespiratory activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.024 Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 21, 95–103. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045357 Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1975). Empathic: An unappreciated way of being. The Counseling Psychologist, 5, 210. https://doi.org/10.1177/001100007500500202 Google Scholar
Sachse, R., & Elliott, R. (2002). Process–outcome research on humanistic therapy variables. In Cain, D. J. (Ed.), Humanistic psychotherapies: Handbook of research and practice (pp. 83115). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Sandell, R., Lazar, A., Grant, J., Carlsson, J., Schubert, J., & Broberg, J. (2007). Therapist attitudes and patient outcomes: II. Therapist attitudes influence change during treatment. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 196204. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503300600608439 Google Scholar
Toussaint, L., & Webb, J. R. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship between empathy and forgiveness. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145, 673685. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.145.6.673-686 Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 33, 448457. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027806 Google Scholar
Watson, J. C., Goldman, R., & Vanaershot, G. (1998). Empathic. A postmodern way of being? In S.Greenberg, L., Watson, J. C., & Lietaer, G., (Eds.), Handbook of experiential psychotherapy (pp. 6181). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar