Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-03T15:00:36.630Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prespeech Vocalizations and the Emergence of Speech: A Study of 1005 Spanish Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 June 2013

Alexandra Karousou*
Affiliation:
Democritus University of Thrace (Greece)
Susana López-Ornat
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense de Madrid (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alexandra Karousou, Democritus University of Thrace, Department of Preschool Education Sciences, Nea Chili, 68100 Alexandroupoli, Greece. Email: [email protected] or [email protected]

Abstract

This study investigates 12 prespeech vocal behaviors which are taken to reflect children´s phonological, communicative and early symbolic development. It explores their development (onset, duration and extinction) and their relation to early lexical development. A structured parental questionnaire on prespeech vocalizations was developed, validated and used for the evaluation of 1005 Spanish children’s early vocal development (8–30 months). In parallel, the same children’s productive vocabulary was assessed using the vocabulary section of the European-Spanish MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. Results highlight a global inverted U-shaped developmental pattern which emerges from the asynchronous development of the vocal behaviors examined, relating both their emergence and extinction to advances in linguistic development. Moreover, the protracted coexistence of prespeech vocalizations with early speech and their significant correlations with vocabulary size reveal a gradual transition into language. Overall results reinforce and extend previous findings on the development of prespeech vocalizations and establish their relevance as early indexes of linguistic development. Finally, positive evidence on the use of an assisted parental report method for reliably evaluating these developments is provided. Results are discussed within theoretical frameworks that conceive language as the emergent product of complex developmental processes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This work was supported by the Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología, Spain [grant number CICYT-PETRI: PTR1995-0412-OP], the Universidad Complutense de Madrid [grant number GR58/08] and the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación [grant number SEJ2007-67810]. We are grateful to all the Spanish families and to various professionals of early development who collaborated in this study. Special thanks to Pamela Smith, an English friend, for her valuable help in avoiding linguistic and other errors. We also thank the anonymous reviewers of this paper for their useful comments and substantial suggestions.

References

Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450466. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130324 Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205226.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Marchmann, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., & Reznick, J. (1994) Development and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 21, 85123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008680 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, M. C. (1975). Proto-conversations. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 6377). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Non-word repetition as a behavioral marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01420.x Google Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B. (1999). How language comes to children: From birth to two years. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B., & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language, 67, 297319.Google Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B., Hallé, P., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1989). A crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journal of Child Language, 16, 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900013404 Google Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996). Gestures and words during the transition to two word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 645673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008989 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C. (1983). Communication to language: deaf children’s and hearing children’s development compared. Sign Language Studies, 39, 113.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (1990). From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 263277). New York, NY: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Chen-Hafteck, L. (1997) Music and language development in early childhood: Integrating past research in the two domains. Early Child Development and Care, 130, 8597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0300443971300109 Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1986). Prosodic development. In Fletcher, P. J. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Studies in first language development (pp. 174197). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. (1991). The validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 565571.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S., Bates, E., Reznick, J. S., & Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of child language at twenty months. Journal of Child Language, 16, 239249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010394 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delack, J. B., & Fowlow, P. J. (1978). The ontogenesis of different vocalizations: Development of prosodic contrastivity during the first year of life. In Waterson, N. & C.Snow, (Eds.), The development of communication (pp. 93110). London, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Diaz, R., & Berk, L. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dowling, W. J. (1999). The development of music perception and cognition. In Deutsch, D. (Ed.), The psychology of music (2 nd ed., pp. 63625). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Eilers, R. E., & Oller, D. K. (1994). Infant vocalizations and the early diagnosis of severe hearing impairment. Journal of Pediatrics, 124, 199203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3476(94)70303-5 Google Scholar
Elbers, L., & Ton, J. (1985). Play pen monologues: The interplay of words and babble in the first words period. Journal of Child Language, 12, 551565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900006644 Google Scholar
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness –A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., … Reilly, J. S. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (5, Serial No. 242).Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 95116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400001053 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development, 60, 14971510. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130938 Google Scholar
Fernández Flecha, M. (2009). Evolución funcional de las vocalizaciones. Relaciones entre la función y la prosodia. [Functional evolution of vocalizations. Relations between function and prosody] . (Unpublished postgraduate dissertation). Madrid, Spain: Universidad Complutense de Madrid.Google Scholar
Franco, F., & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23, 307336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008813 Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child Development, 80, 636644. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01287.x Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. H., & West, M. J. (1999). Consistent responses of mothers to prelinguistic infants: The effect of prelinguistic repertoire size. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 5258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.52 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., Briesch, J., & Syal, S. (2010) Learning while babbling: Prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate readiness to learn. Infancy, 15, 362391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00020.x Google Scholar
Gratier, M., & Devouche, E. (2011). Imitation and repetition of prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 months. Developmental Psychology, 47, 6776. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020722 Google Scholar
Grimm, H., & Doil, H. (2000). ELFRA: Elternfragebögen für die Früherkennung von Risikokindern [Parent Questionnaires for the Early Detection of Children at Risk] (ELFRA-1, ELFRA-2). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., Goldstein, M. H., & King, A. P. (2006). Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelinguistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 509516. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071914 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. London, UK: Edward Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 466472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, C. G. (1983). Setting the stage for language acquisition: Communication development in the first year. In Golinkoff, R. M. (Ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication: Issues and implications (pp. 93113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hsu, H., & Fogel, A. (2003). Social regulatory effects of infant nondistress vocalization on maternal behavior. Developmental Psychology, 39, 976991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.6.976 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Newton, T., & Conboy, B. (2003). MacArthur Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas. User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: P. H. Brookes,Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E., & Gutierrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development of Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 20, 523549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008461 Google Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, 1132.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941/1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals (A. Keiler, Trans.). The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A., & Redanz, N. (1993). Preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development, 64, 675687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02935.x Google Scholar
Karousou, A. (2004). Análisis de las vocalizaciones tempranas: Su patrón evolutivo y su función determinante en la emergencia de la palabra [Analysis of the early vocalizations: Their developmental pattern and their determinant function in the emergence of the word] . (Published doctoral dissertation). Μadrid, Spain: Publicaciones Electrónicas Universidad Complutense de Madrid.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D., & Murray, A. D. (1982). Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 72, 353365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.388089 Google Scholar
Kokkinaki, T., & Kugiumutzakis, G. (2000). Basic aspects of vocal imitation in infant-parent interactions during the first six months. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 18, 173187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713683042 Google Scholar
Kishon-Rabin, L. Taitelbaum-Swead, R., Ezrati-Vinacour, R., Kronnenberg, J., & Hildesheimer, M. (2004). Pre-first word vocalizations of infants with normal hearing and cochlear implants using the PRISE. In Miyamoto, R.T. (Ed.), Cochlear implants. International congress series (Vol. 1273, pp. 360363). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ics.2004.08.022 Google Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, G. (1999). Genesis and development of early infant mimesis to facial and vocal models . In Nadel, J. & Butterworth, G. (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 127185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, G., Kokkinaki, T., Markodimitraki, M., & Vitalaki, E. (2005). Emotions in early mimesis. In Nadel, J. & Muir, D. (Eds.), Emotional development (pp.161182). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 4252438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.417951 Google Scholar
Law, J., & Roy, P. (2008). Parental report of infant language skills: A review of the development and application of the communicative development inventories. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 13, 198206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-3588.2008.00503.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. (1991). Evidence for language specific rhythmic influences in the reduplicative babbling of French and English learning infants. Language and Speech, 34, 235249.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1988). Variation in human biology and child phonology: A response to Goad and Ingram. Journal of Child Language, 15, 663668. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012617 Google Scholar
Loeb, D. F., & Allen, G. D. (1993). Preschooler’s imitation of intonation contours. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 413.Google Scholar
López Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, P., Karousou, A., Mariscal, S., & Nieva, S. (2003). iLC: Un instrumento de medida del desarrollo comunicativo y linguístico temprano basado en las escalas MacArthur [iLC: An instrument for the evaluation of early communicative and linguistic development, based on the MacArthur Scales]. Boletín de la Asociación Española de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 3, 37.Google Scholar
López Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, P., Karousou, A., Mariscal, S., & Martínez, M. (2005). MacArthur: Inventarios de Desarrollo Comunicativo [MacArthur: Communicative Development Inventories]. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Lynch, M. P., Oller, D. K., Steffens, M. L., Levine, S. L., Basinger, D. L., & Umbel, V. M. (1995). Development of speech-like vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100, 6886.Google Scholar
Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A-M., Leiwo, M., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (1996) Parents as informants of their child’s vocal and early language development. Early Child Development and Care, 126, 1525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0300443961260102 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C., Tesman, J. R., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54, 1103.Google Scholar
Malloch, S. N. (1999/2000). Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue: Rhythms, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication, 2957.Google Scholar
Malloch, S. N., & Trevarthen, C. (2008). Communicative musicality. Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Malloch, S. N., Sharp, D. B., Campbell, A. M., Campbell, D. M., & Trevarthen, C. (1997). Measuring the human voice: Analysing pitch, timing, loudness and voice quality in mother/infant communication. Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics, 19, 495500.Google Scholar
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19, 19941997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.064 Google Scholar
Maneva, B., & Genesee, F. (2002). Bilingual babbling: Evidence for language differentiation in dual language acquisition. In Skarbela, B. et al. (Eds.), The Proceedings of the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 383392). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1993). Transitions in representational ability: Infants’ verbal, vocal, and action imitation during the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 437456.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1995). Infants’ early verbal imitation and their later lexical development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 286306.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 405426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2002.0019 Google Scholar
McCathren, R. B., Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1999). The relationship between prelinguistic vocalization and later expressive vocabulary in young children with developmental delay. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 915924.Google Scholar
McCune, L. (1992). First words: A dynamic systems view. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 331336). Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
McCune, L. (1995). A normative study of representational play at the transition to language. Developmental Psychology, 31, 198206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.2.198 Google Scholar
McCune, L. (2008). How children learn to learn language. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McCune, L., & Vihman, M. M. (2001). Early phonetic and lexical development. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44, 670684. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/054) Google Scholar
Meadows, D., Elias, G., & Bain, J. (2000). Mothers’ ability to identify infants’ communicative acts consistently. Journal of Child Language 27, 393406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900004177 Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(88)90035-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J., & Schultz, M. (1986). Predicting phonological development. In Lindblom, B. & Zetterstrom, R. (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 7994). New York, NY: Stockton Press.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., & Muir, D. (2005). Emotional development: Recent research advances. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nagy, E., & Molnar, P. (2004). Homo imitans or homo provocans? The phenomenon of neonatal initiation. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 5463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.06.004 Google Scholar
Nakata, T., & Trehub, S. E. (2004). Infants’ responsiveness to maternal speech and singing. Infant Behavior & Development, 27, 455464. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.002 Google Scholar
Nathani, S., & Oller, D. K. (2001). Beyond ba-ba and gu-gu: Challenges and potential strategies in coding infant vocalizations. Behavior Research, Methods, Instrumentation and Computers, 33, 321330.Google Scholar
Nazzi, Th., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756766. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.756 Google Scholar
D’ Odorico, L., & Franco, F. (1991). Selective production of vocalization types in different communication contexts. Journal of Child Language, 18, 475499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011211 Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., & Basinger, D. (2001). Intuitive identification of infant vocal sounds by parents. Developmental Science, 4, 4960. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00148 Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (1998). Late onset canonical babbling: A possible early marker of abnormal development. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 103, 249263.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Urbano, R., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (1997). Development of precursors to speech in infants exposed to two languages. Journal of Child Language, 24, 407425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000997003097 Google Scholar
Ostwald, P. F. (1973). Musical behavior in early childhood. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 367375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1973.tb04897.x Google Scholar
Papaeliou, C., Minadakis, G., & Cavouras, D. (2002). Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations expressing emotions and communicative functions. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45, 311317. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/024) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papaeliou, C., & Trevarthen, C. (2006). Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing communication and apprehension. Journal of Child Language, 33, 163178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000905007300 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papoušek, M. (1992). Early ontogeny of vocal communication in parent–infant interactions. In Papousek, H., Jurgens, U., & Papousek, M. (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 230261). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Papoušek, M. (1996). Intuitive parenting: A hidden source of musical stimulation in infancy. In Deliege, I. & Sloboda, J. (Eds.), Musical beginnings. Origins and development of musical competence (pp. 88112). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1981). Musical elements in the infant’s vocalization: Their significance for communication, cognition, and creativity. Advances in Infancy Research, 1, 163–-224.Google Scholar
Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their pre-canonical infants. First Language, 9, 137158.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1977). Language learning strategies: Does the whole equals the sum of the parts? Language, 53, 560573. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/413177 Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., & Goodz, N. (2001). Language differentiation in bilingual infants: Evidence from babbling. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition research (pp. 95106). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ring, E. D., & Fenson, L. (2000). Assessment of language comprehension and production: Child performance versus parent judgment. First Language, 20, 141159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272370002005902 Google Scholar
Rodgon, M. M., & Kurdek, L. A. (1977). Vocal and gestural imitation in 8, 14, and 20 month old children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 131, 115123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1977.10533280 Google Scholar
Schaerlaekens, A., Forrez, G., & Van Bael, L. (1990). Mimicry of intonation in 10- to 12-month-old children: A failure to replicate. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 4, 139144. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/02699209008985477 Google Scholar
Shenfield, T., Trehub, S. E., & Nakata, T. (2003). Maternal singing modulates infant arousal. Psychology of Music, 31, 365375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/03057356030314002 Google Scholar
Siegel, G., Cooper, M., Morgan, J. L., & Sarshad, R. (1990). Imitation of intonation by infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.2.293 Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1989). Imitativeness: A trait or a skill? In Speidel, G. & Nelson, K. (Eds.), The many faces of imitation (pp. 7390). New York, NY: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Snow, D. (2001). Imitation of intonation contours by children with normal and disordered language development. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15, 567584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699200110078168 Google Scholar
Stark, R. E., & Tallal, P. (1988). Language, speech, and reading disorders in children: Neuropsychological studies. Boston, MA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1989). Prespeech and early speech development of two late talkers. First Language, 9, 207224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272378900900607 Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1992). Prelinguistic vocal development: Measurement and predictions. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 439456). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between phonological and lexical development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000425 Google Scholar
Thal, D., Jackson-Maldonado, D., & Acosta, D. (2000). Validity of a parent-report measure of vocabulary and grammar for Spanish-speaking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 10871100.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11, 188195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00240 Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1999-2000). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue: Rhythms, Musical Narrative and the Origins of Human Communication, 157213.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00701 Google Scholar
Uzgiris, I. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/016502548100400101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Puyvelde, M., Vanfleteren, P., Loots, G., Deschuyffeleer, S., Vinck, B., Jacquet, W., & Verhelst, W. (2010). Tonal synchrony in mother–infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 387400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.003 Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1986). Individual differences in babbling and early speech: Predicting to age three. In Lindblom, B. & Zetterström, R. (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 95109). New York, NY: Stockton Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1996). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., DePaolis, R. A., & Keren-Portnoy, T. (2009). Babbling and words: A dynamic systems perspective on phonological development. In Bavin, E. L. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 163182). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & Greenlee, M. (1987). Individual differences in phonological development: Ages one and three years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 503521.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling to speech: A re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language, 61, 397445. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/414151 Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & McCune, L. (1994). When is a word a word? Journal of Child Language, 21, 517542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900009442 Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & Miller, R. (1988). Words and babble at the threshold of lexical acquisition. In Smith, M.D. & Locke, J.L. (Eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child’s development of a linguistic vocabulary (pp. 185222). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1962). Thought and language , Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Kublin, K., & Goldstein, H. (2002). Validity and reliability of the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile with very young children. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 45, 12021218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/097) Google Scholar
Winsler, A. (2009). Still talking to ourselves after all these years: A review of current research on private speech. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 341). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar