Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T07:02:26.008Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Morningness-Eveningness in Adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Juan Francisco Díaz-Morales*
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense
María Gutiérrez Sorroche
Affiliation:
I.E.S. Luis García Berlanga
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Juan Francisco Díaz-Morales, Departamento Psicología Diferencial y Trabajo, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas, 28223, MADRID (Spain). Fax: 91 394 3189. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Individual morningness-eveningness differences explain the rhythmic variations of behavioral and biological patterns. Several studies have corroborated that morningness preference increases over adulthood and aging. Adolescents shift their time of day preferences from morningness to eveningness during puberty. The aims of this study were translate to Spanish the Morningness-Eveningness Scale for Children (MESC) and to analyze age and sex differences. A group of 600 adolescents (aged between 12 and 16 years) participated in the study. Psychometric analysis showed that reliability and factor structure were suitable and similar to previous studies. The results indicated a clear decrease of morningness as of 12 years. Boys were more morningness-oriented than girls in several items of the questionnaire. In view of the results, several educational implications are raised.

La tendencia a la matutinidad-vespertinidad permite explicar gran parte de las variaciones rítmicas de patrones biológicos y conductuales. Se ha constatado que con la edad se produce un incremento paulatino de la matutinidad. En población adolescente se ha detectado una mayor tendencia a la vespertinidad. Los objetivos del estudio fueron la traducción y adaptación del Morningness-Eveningness Scale for Children (MESC) a población adolescente, así como el análisis de las diferencias de edad y sexo. En el estudio participaron 600 adolescentes entre 12 y 16 años. Los resultados indicaron que el MESC es un instrumento fiable en población adolescente, siendo su estructura factorial similar a la obtenida en otros estudios. A partir de los 12 años se producía una disminución progresiva de la matutinidad, siendo esta disminución mayor para las mujeres que para los varones en algunos elementos del cuestionario. Se plantean algunas implicaciones educativas a raíz de los resultados obtenidos.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adan, A., Caci, H., & Prat, G. (2005). Reliability of the Spanish version of the Composite Scale of Morningness. European Psychiatry, 20, 503509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adan, A., & Natale, V. (2002). Gender differences in morningness-eveningness preference. Chronobiology International, 19, 709720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baehr, E., Revelle, W., & Eastman, C. (2000). Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: With an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. Journal Sleep Research, 9, 117127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bearpark, H., & Michie, P. (1987). Changes in morningness-eveningness scores during adolescence and their relationship to sleep/wake disturbances. Chronobiologia, 14, 151.Google Scholar
Caci, H., Robert, P., Dossios, C., & Boyer, P. (2005). L'échelle de matinalité pour enfants et adolescents: propriétés psychométriques et effet du mois de naissance. L'Encéphale, 31, 5664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrier, J., & Monk, T. (2000). Circadian rhythms of performance: New trends. Chronobiology International, 17, 719732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carskadon, M.A., & Acebo, C. (1993). A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14, 190195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carskadon, M.A., Vieira, C., & Acebo, C. (1993). Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep, 16, 258262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cofer, L.F., Grice, J.W., Sethre-Hofstad, L., Radi, C.J., Zimmerman, L.K., Palmer-Seal, D., & Santa-Maria, G. (1999). Developmental perspectives on morningness-eveningness and social interactions. Human Development, 42, 169198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, G., Sartori, S., & Åkerstedt, T. (2006). Influence of flexibility and variability of working hours on health and well-being. Chronobiology International, 23, 11251137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C.G., Hasher, L., Djikic, M.Criger, B., & Peterson, J.B. (2007). Morning people are stable people: Circadian rhythm and the higher-order factors of the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 267276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Morales, J.F. (2007). Morning and evening-types: Exploring their personality styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 769778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Morales, J.F., Dávila de León, C., & Gutiérrez, M. (2007). Validity of the Morningness-Eveningness Scale for Children among Spanish adolescents. Chronobiology International, 24, 435447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Díaz-Morales, J.F., & Sánchez-López, M.P. (2004). Composite and Preferences Scales of Morningness: Reliability and factorial invariance in adult and university samples. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 7, 93100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Morales, J.F., & Sánchez-López, M.P. (2005a). Composite Scale of Morningness and Preferences: Preliminary validity data inPeruvian undergraduates. Ergonomics, 4, 354363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Morales, J.F., & Sánchez-López, M.P. (2005b). Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Preferencias y la Escala Compuesta de Matutinidad. Psicothema, 17, 478484.Google Scholar
Gau, S.F., & Soong, W.T. (2003). The transition of sleep-wake patterns in early adolescence. Sleep, 26, 449454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gau, S.F., Soong, W.T., & Merikangas, K.R. (2004). Correlates of sleep-wake patterns among children and young adolescents in Taiwan. Sleep, 27, 512519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, D., Hahn, C.S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U.J., & Zelazo, P.D. (2006). Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 431440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S., Dueker, G.L., Hasher, L., & Goldstein, D. (2002). Children's time of day preference: Age, gender, and ethnic differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 10831090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, C.P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: The costs and a benefit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 142147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, C.P., & Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 363379.Google ScholarPubMed
May, C.P., Hasher, L., & Stoltzfus, E.R. (1993). Optimal time of day and the magnitude of age differences in memory. Psychological Science, 4, 326330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Natale, V., & Bruni, O. (2000). Confronto fra tre questionari per la valutazione della tipologia circadiana in adolescenza. Testing Psicometria Metodologia, 7, 3344.Google Scholar
Natale, V., & Danesi, E. (2002). Gender and circadian typology. Biological Rhythms Research, 33, 261269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, Y.M., Matsumoto, K., Seo, Y.J., & Shinkoda, H. (1999). Sleep and chronotype for children in Japan. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88, 13151329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pisarski, A., Brook, C., Bohle, P., Gallois, C., Watson, B., & Winch, S. (2006). Extending a model of shift work tolerance. Chronobiology International, 23, 13631377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randler, C., & Díaz-Morales, J.F. (2007). Morningness in German and Spanish students: A comparative study. European Journal of Personality, 21, 419427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Randler, C., & Frech, D. (2006). Correlation between morningness-eveningness and final school leaving exams. Biological Rhythm Research, 37, 233239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roenneberg, T., Kuehnle, T., Pramstaller, P.P., Ricken, J., Havel, M., Guth, A., & Merrow, M. (2004). A marker for the end of adolescence. Current Biology, 14, R1038–R1039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roenneberg, T., Wirz-Justice, A., & Merrow, M. (2003). Life between the clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 18, 8090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C., Reilly, C., & Midkiff, K. (1989). Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 728738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SPSS, Inc. (1988). SPSS-X user's guide. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Sheffield, A. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takeuchi, H., Inoue, M., Watanabe, N., Yamashita, Y., Hamada, M., Kadota, G., & Harada, T. (2001). Parental enforcement of bedtime during childhood modulates preference of Japanese junior high school students for eveningness chronotype. Chronobiology International, 18, 823829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tankova, I., Adan, A., & Buela-Casal, G. (1994). Circadian typology and individual differences: A review. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 671684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar