Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T22:34:23.423Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hand Gestures and Perceived Influence in Small Group Interaction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Fridanna Maricchiolo*
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi Roma Tre (Italy)
Stefano Livi
Affiliation:
Sapienza Università di Roma (Italy)
Marino Bonaiuto
Affiliation:
Sapienza Università di Roma (Italy)
Augusto Gnisci
Affiliation:
Seconda Università di Napoli (Italy)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Fridanna Maricchiolo, Università degli Studi Roma Tre. Department of Cultural and Educational Studies. Via Milazzo 11B, 00185 Rome (Italy). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

A laboratory study was carried out to establish the relative importance of verbal and gestural behavior, as well as their interaction, for perceived social influence in more or less competitive small groups. Forty women (psychology students) participated in leaderless small group discussions of different sizes (fourmember and eight-member): at the end, each member rated the perceived influence in decision-making of every other member. Verbal dominance coding is based on traditional quantitative conversational dominance (number of talk turns). Gestural coding (conversational, ideational, object-adaptor, self-adaptor gestures) is based on classical gesture classifications. Beside a substantial effect of verbal dominance, the main result is that frequency of object-adaptors and conversational (only in large groups) and ideational (in both small and large groups) gestures increases perceived influence scores particularly when the verbal dominance of the speaker is low.

Se ha realizado un estudio de laboratorio para establecer la importancia de la conducta gestual y verbal, así como su interacción, en la influencia social percibida en grupos pequeños más o menos competitivos. Cuarenta mujeres (estudiantes de psicología) participaron en discusiones de grupo sin líder de diferentes tamaños (cuatro miembros y ocho miembros): al final, cada miembro valoró la influencia percibida en la toma de decisiones de cada uno de los otros miembros. El código de dominancia verbal está basado en la dominancia cuantitativa conversacional (número de turnos de habla). El código gestual (conversacional, ideacional, adaptador-objeto, gestos auto-adaptadores) está basado en clasificaciones gestuales clásicas. Además de un efecto sustancial de la dominancia verbal, el resultado principal es que la frecuencia de adaptadores-objeto y gestos conversacionales (sólo en grupos grandes) e ideacionales (tanto en grupos pequeños como grandes) incrementa los valores de influencia percibida, particularmente cuando la dominancia verbal del hablante es baja.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(2), 169188. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Argentin, G., Ghiglione, R., & Dorna, A. (1990). La gestualité et ses effets dans le discours politique [Gesture and its effect on political discourse]. Psychologie Française, 35, 153161.Google Scholar
Argyle, M. (1972). Nonverbal communication in human social interaction. In Hinde, R. A. (Ed.), Nonverbal Communication (pp. 243269). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Argyle, M. Lalljee, M., & Cook, M. (1968). The effect of visibility on interaction in a dyad. Human Relations, 21, 317. doi:10.1177/001872676802100101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M (1997), Observing interaction. An introduction to Sequential Analysis. (2nd Ed.) Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bales, R. F. (1953). The equilibrium problem in small groups. In Parsons, T., Bales, R. F., & Shils, E. A. (Eds.), Working papers in the theory of action (pp. 111161). New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill's Handbook of Leadership. Theory, Research, and Managerial Applications. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Beattie, G. W., & Shovelton, H. K. (2005). Why the spontaneous images created by the hands during talk can help make TV advertisements more effective. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 2137. doi:10.1348/000712605X103500CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beattie, G. W., & Shovelton, H. K. (2006). When size really matters: How a single semantic feature is represented in the speech and gesture modalities. Gesture, 6, 6384. doi:10.1075/gest.6.1.04beaCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, K. W., & Popelka, G. K. (1971). Extra-facial gestures in relation to speech-reading. Journal of Communication Disorders 3, 302308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonaiuto, M., Gnisci, A., & Maricchiolo, F. (2002). Proposta e verifica di una tassonomia per la codifica dei gesti delle mani in discussioni di piccolo gruppo. [Proposal and test of a taxonomy to code hand gestures in small group discussions]. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 29, 777807.Google Scholar
Bonito, J. A. (2002). The analysis of participation in small groups: Methodological and conceptual issues related to interdependence. Small Group Research, 33, 412438. doi:10.1177/104649640203300402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgoon, J. K., Birk, T., & Pfau, M. (1990). Nonverbal behaviors, persuasion, and credibility. Human Communication Research, 17, 140169. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1990.tb00229.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, D., & Geis, F. L. (1990). Nonverbal affect responses to male and female leaders: Implications for leadership evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 4859. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.58.1.48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B., & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, Speech and Computational Stage: A reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96, 168174. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.96.1.168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carli, L. L., LaFleur, S. L., & Loeber, C. C. (1995). Nonverbal behavior, gender, and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 10301041. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.68.6.1030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E. (1998). Smiles, speech, and body posture: How women and men display sociometric status and power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 209228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caso, L., Maricchiolo, F., Bonaiuto, M., Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2006). The impact of deception and suspicion on different hand movements. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(1), 119. doi:10.1007/s10919-005-0001-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Grada, E., Livi, S., Mannetti, L., & Pierro, A. (2001). Relazioni discorsive e soddisfazione per la propria prestazione e quella del gruppo in un contesto di discussione negoziale [Discourse and performance's satisfaction in group negotiations]. Rassegna Italiana di Psicolinguistica Applicata, 2, 99120.Google Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74118. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.129.1.74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1985). Patterns of visual dominance behavior in humans. In Ellyson, S. L. & Dovidio, J. F. (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal communication (pp. 129149). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior. Semiotica, 1, 4998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. (1983). The interaction order. American Sociological Review, 48, 117. doi:10.2307/2095141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. A., & Argyle, M. (1975). A cross-cultural study of the communication of extra-verbal meaning by gestures. International Journal of Psychology, 10, 5767. doi:10.1080/00207597508247319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadar, U., & Butterworth, B. (1997). Iconic gestures, imagery and word retrieval in speech. Semiotica, 15, 147172. doi:10.1515/semi.1997.115.1-2.147Google Scholar
Hall, J. A., & Friedman, G. B. (1999). Status, gender, and nonverbal behavior: A study of structured interactions between employees of a company. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 10821091. doi:10.1177/01461672992512002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex, and nonverbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Henningsen, D. D., Valde, K. S., & Davies, E. (2005). Exploring the effect of verbal and nonverbal cues on perceptions of deception. Communication Quarterly, 53(3), 359375. doi:10.1080/01463370500101329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollander, E. P. (1985). Leadership and power. In Linzdey, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds.). Handbook of social psychology (Vol. II, pp. 485537). New York, NY: Random Hoese.Google Scholar
Holle, H., & Gunter, T. C. (2007). The role of iconic gestures in speech disambiguation: ERP evidence. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 19, 11751192. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyle, R. H., Georgesen, J. C., & Webster, J. M. (2001). Analyzing data from individuals in groups: The past, the present, and the future. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5, 4147. doi:10.1037//1089-2699.5.1.41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jefferson, G. (1985). On the interactional unpackaging of a “Gloss”. Language & Society, 14, 435466. doi:10.1017/S0047404500011465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2000). The analysis of data from dyads and groups. In Reis, H. & Judd, C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology (pp. 451477). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keating, C. (1985). Human dominance signals: The primate in us. In Ellyson, S. L. & Dovidio, J. F. (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 89108). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal Relations: A Theory of Interdependence. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Manning, S., & Rodak, S. (2008). Gesture gives a hand to language and learning: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology and education. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2, 120. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00067.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1983). Gesture and speech: How they interact. In Wiemann, J. M. & Harrison, R. P. (Eds.), Nonverbal interaction (pp. 1345). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D., & Livi, S. (2009). A componential analysis of leadership using the Social Relations Model. In Yammarino, F., & Danserau, F.. Multi-Level Issues in Organizational Behavior and Leadership. vol. 8, (147191), Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A., Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., Livi, S., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). The statistical analysis of data from small groups. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 83, 126137. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A., & La Voie, L. (1985). Separating individual and group effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 339348. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.48.2.339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leffler, A., Gillespie, D., & Conaty, J. C. (1982). The effect of status differentiation on nonverbal behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 153161. doi:10.2307/3033648CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leigh, T. W., & Summers, J. O. (2002). An initial evaluation of industrial buyers' impressions of salespersons' nonverbal cues. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 22, 4153.Google Scholar
Littell, R. C., Milliken, G. A., Stroup, W. W., & Wolfingen, R. D. (1996). SAS system for mixed models. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.Google Scholar
Livi, S., Battantier, A., Maricchiolo, F. & Mannucci, F. (2005, August). Verbal and nonverbal cues of dominance in small groups discussions. In Noldus, L. P. J. J., Grieco, F., Loijens, L. W. S., & Zimmerman, P. H. (Eds). Proceedings of measuring behavior 2005, 5th International Conference on Methods and Techniques in Behavioral Research (pp. 632634). Wageningen, The Netherlands: Noldus Information Technology.Google Scholar
Livi, S., & Kenny, D. (2009). Thoughts on studying leadership in natural contexts. In Yammarino, F., & Danserau, F.. Multilevel issues in organizational behavior and leadership. (Vol. 8, pp. 215224), Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livi, S., Kenny, D., Albright, L., & Pierro, A. (2008). A social relations analysis of leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 19(2), 235248. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2008.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livi, S., Pierro, A, Mannetti, L. (2000). Self Monitoring, controllo dello spazio conversazionale e dominanza percepita in discussioni di piccoli gruppi in azione. [Self Monitoring, conversational space control and perceived dominance in small group discussion] Rassegna di Psicologia, 17. 127139.Google Scholar
Mannetti, L., Battantier, A., & Pierro, A. (1996). Dominance in group conversations. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 12, 297307.Google Scholar
Maricchiolo, F., Gnisci, A., Bonaiuto, M., & Ficca, G. (2009). Effects of different types of hand gestures in persuasive speech on receivers' evaluations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 239266. doi:10.1080/01690960802159929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92, 350371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A., & Williams, M. (1969). Nonverbal concomitants of perceived and intended persuasiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 3758. doi:10.1037/h0027993CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mignault, A., & Chaudhuri, A. (2002). The many faces of a neutral face: Head tilt and perception of dominance and emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 111132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullen, B., Salas, E., & Driskell, J. E. (1989). Salience, motivation, and artifacts as contributions to the relation participation rate and leadership. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 545559. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(89)90005-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, S. H., Bell, D., & Brooke, M. (1993). Gaining turns and achieving high influence ranking in small conversational groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 265275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, M. T. (1989). Controlling conversations: Turns, topics and interpersonal control. Communication Monographs, 56, 118. doi:10.1080/03637758909390246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimè, B., & Schiaratura, P. (1991). Gesture and speech. In Feldman, R. S. & Rimè, B. (Eds.), Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior (pp. 239281). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; Paris, France: Editions de la Maison des sciences de l'homme.Google Scholar
Riseborough, M. G. (1981). Physiographic gestures as decoding facilitators: Three experiments exploring a neglected facet of communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 5, 172183. doi:10.1007/BF00986134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid Mast, M. (2001). Gender differences and similarities in dominance hierarchies in same-gender groups based on speaking time. Sex Roles, 44, 537556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid Mast, M. (2003). Dominance as expressed and inferred through speaking time: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research, 28, 420430.Google Scholar
Shaw, M. E., (1971). Group dynamics: The psychology of smallgroup behavior. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Tannen, D. (1990). You just don't understand: Women and men in conversation. New York, NY: BallantineGoogle Scholar
Tusing, K. J., & Dillard, J. P. (2000). The sounds of dominance: Vocal precursors of perceived dominance during interpersonal influence. Human Communication Research, 26, 148171. doi:10.1093/hcr/26.1.148Google Scholar