Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T12:44:36.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comparative Pessimism or Optimism: Depressed Mood, Risk-Taking, Social Utility and Desirability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 March 2015

Isabelle Milhabet*
Affiliation:
Université Nice-Sophia Antipolis (France)
Emmanuelle Le Barbenchon
Affiliation:
Université Savoie Mont Blanc (France)
Laurent Cambon
Affiliation:
Université Nice-Sophia Antipolis (France)
Guylaine Molina
Affiliation:
Aix Marseille Université (France)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Isabelle Milhabet. Laboratoire d’Anthropologie et de Psychologie Cognitives et Sociales. Université de Nice-Sophia Antipolis. Pôle Universitaire Saint-Jean d’Angély. 24, Av. des Diables bleus. 06357. Nice Cedex 4. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Comparative optimism can be defined as a self-serving, asymmetric judgment of the future. It is often thought to be beneficial and socially accepted, whereas comparative pessimism is correlated with depression and socially rejected. Our goal was to examine the social acceptance of comparative optimism and the social rejection of comparative pessimism in two dimensions of social judgment, social desirability and social utility, considering the attributions of dysphoria and risk-taking potential (studies 2 and 3) on outlooks on the future. In three experiments, the participants assessed either one (study 1) or several (studies 2 and 3) fictional targets in two dimensions, social utility and social desirability. Targets exhibiting comparatively optimistic or pessimistic outlooks on the future were presented as non-depressed, depressed, or neither (control condition) (study 1); non-depressed or depressed (study 2); and non-depressed or in control condition (study 3). Two significant results were obtained: (1) social rejection of comparative pessimism in the social desirability dimension, which can be explained by its depressive feature; and (2) comparative optimism was socially accepted on the social utility dimension, which can be explained by the perception that comparatively optimistic individuals are potential risk-takers.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abele, A., & Hermer, P. (1993). Mood influences on health-related judgments: Appraisal of own health versus appraisal of unhealthy behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 613625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420230606 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for the self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 366378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.52.2.366 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B., Fedderly, S. S., Kennedy-Moore, E., & Cohan, C. L. (1998). Dysphoria and social interaction: An integration of behavioral confirmation and interpersonal perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 15661579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1566 Google Scholar
Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 511536. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.324 Google Scholar
Armor, D.-A., & Taylor, S.-E. (1998). Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology. (Vol. 30, pp. 309379). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bougheas, S. (2002). Optimism, education and industrial development. Research in Economics, 2, 199214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/reec.2001.0276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cambon, L. (2006). Désirabilité sociale et utilité sociale: Deux dimensions de la valeur communiquée par les adjectifs de personnalité [Social desirability and social utility: Two evaluative dimensions of Personality Traits]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale/International Review of Social Psychology, 19, 125151.Google Scholar
Cambon, L., Djouary, A., & Beauvois, J. L. (2006). Social judgment norms and social utility: When it is more valuable to be useful than desirable. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 65, 167180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.65.3.167 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., Kus, L. A., & Scheier, M. F. (1994). Effects of good versus bad mood and optimistic versus pessimistic outlook on social acceptance versus rejection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 138151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1994.13.2.138 Google Scholar
Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 813838. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 11521162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.68.6.1152 Google Scholar
Dewberry, C., & Richardson, S. (1990). Effect of anxiety on optimism. Journal of Social Psychology, 130, 731738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1990.9924625 Google Scholar
Dolbier, C. L., Soderstron, M., & Steinhardt, M. A. (2001). The relationships between self-leadership and enhanced psychological, health, and work outcomes. The Journal of Psychology, 135, 469485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223980109603713 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, N., & Beauvois, J.-L. (2005). Normativeness and individualism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 123146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.236 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubois, N., & Beauvois, J.-L. (2011). Are some rabbits more competent and warm than others? The lay epistemologist is interested in object value but not in descriptive parameters. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70, 6373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000040 Google Scholar
Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (1998). A multi-modal analysis of personal negativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 15801591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1580 Google Scholar
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation level theory. New York, NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Helweg-Larsen, M., & Shepperd, J. A. (2001). Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the literature. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 7495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helweg-Larsen, M., Sadeghian, P., & Webb, M. S. (2002). The stigma of being pessimistically biased. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, 92107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.21.1.92.22405 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickman, S. E., Watson, P. J., & Morris, R. J. (1996). Optimism, pessimism, and the complexity of narcissism. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 521525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00223-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
House, R. J., & Shamir, B. (1998). Toward the integration of transformational, charismatic, and visionary theories. In Chemers, M. & Ayman, R. (Eds.), Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and directions (pp. 81107). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 206219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.66.1.206 Google Scholar
Le Barbenchon, E., & Milhabet, I. (2005). L’optimisme: Réponse désirable et/ou socialement utile? [Optimism: A desirable and/or socially useful response?]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale/International Review of Social Psychology, 18, 153181.Google Scholar
Le Barbenchon, E., Milhabet, I., Steiner, D. D., & Priolo, D. (2008). Social acceptance of exhibiting optimism. Current Research in Social Psychology, 14, 5263.Google Scholar
Milhabet, I., Le Barbenchon, E., Molina, G., Cambon, L., & Steiner, D. D. (2012). Comparative optimism, so useful. International Review of Social Psychology, 25, 540.Google Scholar
Mussweiler, T. (2003). “Everything is relative”: Comparison processes in social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 719733. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.169 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3 rd Ed). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Peeters, G. (1971). The positive-negative asymmetry: On cognitive consistency and positivity bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 455474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420010405 Google Scholar
Peeters, G. (2002). From good and bad to can and must: Subjective necessity of acts associated with positively and negatively valued stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 125136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.70 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peeters, G., Czapinski, J., & Hoorens, V. (2001). Comparative optimism, pessimism and realism with respect to adverse events and their relationship with will to live. International Review of Social Psychology, 14, 143162.Google Scholar
Peretti-Watel, P. (2005). La culture du risque, ses marqueurs sociaux et ses paradoxes: Une exploration empirique [Risk culture, social bookmarking an dits paradoxes: An empirical exploration]. Revue Économique 56, 371392. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/reco.562.0371 Google Scholar
Pietromonaco, P. R., & Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative thoughts in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 799807. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.48.3.799 Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling methods for estimating and comparing indirect effects. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1985). Depression and preference for self-focusing stimuli after success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 10661075. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.49.4.1066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1986). Evidence for a depressive self-focusing style. Journal of Research in Personality, 20, 95100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(86)90112-1 Google Scholar
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.122 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 9941001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.52.5.994 Google Scholar
Quadrel, M. J., Fischhoff, B., & Davis, W. (1993). Adolescent (in)vulnerability. American Psychologist, 48, 102116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.48.2.102 Google Scholar
Salovey, P., & Birmaum, D. (1989). Influence of mood on health-relevant cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 539551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.57.3.539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepperd, J. A., Grace, J., Cole, L. J., & Klein, C. (2005). Anxiety and outcome predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 267275. http://dx.doi.org//10.1177/0146167204271322 Google Scholar
Siegel, S. J., & Alloy, L. B. (1990). Interpersonal perceptions and consequences of depressive-significant other relationships: A naturalistic study of college roommates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 361373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0021-843X.99.4.361 Google Scholar
Spire, B., & Peretti-Watel, P. (2008). Sida. Une maladie chronique passée au crible [AIDS. Chronic disease sifted]. Paris, France: Presses de l’EHESP.Google Scholar
Starr, L. R., & Davila, J. (2008). Excessive reassurance seeking, depression, and interpersonal rejection: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 762775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013866 Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.103.2.193 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Portrait of the self-enhancer: Well adjusted and well liked or maladjusted and friendless? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 165176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.84.1.165 Google Scholar
Tice, D. M., Butler, J. L., Muraven, M. B., & Stillwell, A. M. (1995). When modesty prevails: Differential favorability of self-presentation to friends and strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 11201138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.69.6.1120 Google Scholar
Tyler, J. M., & Rosier, J. G. (2009). Examining self-presentation as a motivational explanation for comparative optimism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 716727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016231 Google Scholar
Ucbasaran, D., Westhead, P., Wright, M., & Flores, M. (2010). The Nature of entrepreneurial experience, business failure and comparative optimism. Journal of Business Venturing, 25, 541555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusvent.2009.04.001 Google Scholar
van der Velde, F. W., van der Pligt, J., & Hooykaas, C. (1994). Perceiving AIDS-related risk: Accuracy as a function of differences in actual risk. Health Psychology, 13, 2533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0278-6133.13.1.25 Google Scholar
Vollmann, M., Renner, B., & Weber, H. (2007). Optimism and social support: The providers' perspective. Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 205215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760701409660 Google Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806820. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.39.5.806 Google Scholar
Weinstein, N. D., Marcus, S. E., & Moser, R. P. (2005). Smokers’ unrealistic optimism about their risk. Tobacco Control, 14, 5559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/tc.2004.008375 Google Scholar
Wojciszke, B., & Abele, A. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 11391147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.549 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. New York, NY: Erlbaum Hillsdale.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1990). The Psychophysiology of Sensation Seeking. Journal of Personality, 58, 313345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00918.x Google Scholar