Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T21:06:45.528Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Comparative and Evolutionary Analysis of the Cultural Cognition of Humans and Other Apes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2017

Andrew Whiten*
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews (UK)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andrew Whiten, Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews. KY16 9JP. St Andrews (UK). Phone: +44–1334462073. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The comparative and evolutionary analysis of social learning and all manner of cultural processes has become a flourishing field. Applying the ‘comparative method’ to such phenomena allows us to exploit the good fortunate we have in being able to study them in satisfying detail in our living primate relatives, using the results to reconstruct the cultural cognition of the ancestral forms we share with these species. Here I offer an overview of principal discoveries in recent years, organized through a developing scheme that targets three main dimensions of culture: the patterning of culturally transmitted traditions in time and space; the underlying social learning processes; and the particular behavioral and psychological contents of cultures. I focus on a comparison between humans, particularly children, and our closest primate relative the chimpanzee, for which we now have much the richest database of relevant observational and experimental findings. Commonalities across these sister-species can be identified in each of the three dimensions listed above and in several subcategories within them, but the comparisons also highlight the major contrasts in the nature of culture that have evolved between ourselves and closest primate relatives.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2015a). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature, 518, 538541. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13998 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2015b). Counting conformity: Evaluating the units of measurement in frequency-dependent social learning. Animal Behaviour, 110, e5e8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: 1. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70, 170. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093718 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berl, R. E. W., & Hewlett, B. S. (2015). Cultural variation in the use of overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLOS ONE, 10, e0120180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120180 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. (2012). Wild cultures. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C., Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B., & Jonlian, F. (1994). Is nutcracking wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour? Journal of Human Evolution, 26, 325338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39, 591614. https://doi.org/10.1086/204785 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnie, K. E., Horner, V., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Spread of arbitrary customs among chimpanzees: A controlled experiment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 274, 367372.Google ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10, 3138. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00630.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. Child Development, 79, 609626. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01146.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychological Science, 20, 14781483. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02469.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. (2001). Body imitation in an enculturated orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). Cybernetics and Systems, 32, 97119. https://doi.org/10.1080/019697201300001821 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 151163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0237-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1994). The social learning of tool use by orangutans. Human Evolution, 9, 297313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). What do chimpanzees know about seeing, revisited: An explanation of the third kind. In Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T., & Roessler, J. (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 4564). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claidière, N., & Whiten, A. (2012). Integrating the study of conformity and culture in humans and non-human animals. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 126145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2010). Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental Psychology, 46, 437445. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017553 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., & Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology, 10, 221243. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.00968.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Custance, D. M., Whiten, A., & Bard, K. A. (1995). Can young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and Hayes 1952 revisited. Behaviour, 132, 837859. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853995X00036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2005). The Ancestor’s Tale. London, UK: Weidenfield and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335, 11141118. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1213969 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Experimental “microcultures” in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development, 83, 911925. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01747.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuhrmann, D., Ravignani, A., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Whiten, A. (2014). Synchrony and motor mimicking in chimpanzee observational learning. Scientific Reports, 4, 5283. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep05283 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. G. (1992). The question of animal culture. Human Nature, 3, 157178. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692251 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Strimling, P., Wrangham, R., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2009). Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task. Current Biology, 19, 18061810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.060 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Reynolds, V., Wrangham, R., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2011). Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 1, 128. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00128 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidle, M. N., Bolus, M., Collard, M., Conard, N. J., Garofolli, D., … Whiten, A. (2015). The nature of culture: An eight-grade model for the evolution and expansion of cultural capacities in hominins and other animals. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 93, 4370. https://doi.org/10.4436/JASS.93011 Google ScholarPubMed
Hara, Y., Imanishi, T., & Satta, Y. (2012). Reconstructing the demographic history of the human lineage using whole-genome sequences from human and three great apes. Genome Biology and Evolution, 4, 11331145. https://doi.org/10.1093/gbe/evs075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lanoble, A., … Roche, H. (2015). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 521, 310315. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14464 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Havard University Press.Google Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Current Biology, 22, 727731. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haun, D. B., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Children conform to the behaviour of peers; other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25, 21602167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, K. J., & Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-reared chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 450459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2015). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species and making us smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00071-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrman, E., Call, J., Hernández-Loreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 13601366. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1146282 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo basin hunters. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 11681178. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0373 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, K. (2009). Animal “culture”? In Laland, K. N., Galef, B. G. (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 269287). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C., Poiso, T., Zuberbühler, K., Hoppit, W., & Gruber, T. (2015). Social network analysis shows direct evidence for social learning of tool use in wild chimpanzees. PLOS Biology, 12, e1001960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoehl, S., Zettersten, M., Schleihauf, H., Grätz, S., & Pauen, S. (2014). The role of social interaction and pedagogical cues for eliciting and reducing overimitation in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 122133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, L. M., Spiteri, A., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2007). Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 73, 10211032. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2008). Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 275, 835840.Google ScholarPubMed
Hopper, L. M., Schapiro, S. J., Lambeth, S. P., & Brosnan, S. F. (2011). Chimpanzees’ socially maintained food preferences indicate both conservatism and conformity. Animal Behaviour, 81, 11951202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2015). The importance of witnessed agency in chimpanzee social learning of tool use. Behavioural Processes, 112, 120129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppitt, W. J. E., Brown, G. R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M. M., & Laland, K. N. (2008). Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 23, 486493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2008.05.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2008). Social processes influencing learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 38, 105165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00003-X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, W. J. E., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods and models. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Horner, K., Proctor, D., Bonnie, K. E., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLoS One, 5, e10625. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010625 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children. Animal Cognition, 8, 164181. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 103, 1387813883. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0606015103 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hrubesch, C., Preuschoft, S., & van Schaik, C. (2009). Skill mastery inhibits adoption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 12, 209216. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0183-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, P. (2015). Technology as human social tradition: Cultural transmission among Hunter-Gatherers. Oakland, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Kendal, R. M., Hopper, L. M., Whiten, A., Brosnan, S. F., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Hoppitt, W. (2015). Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: Implications for cultural diversity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 6572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2011). Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London A, 278, 12391246. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1399 Google ScholarPubMed
Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 392406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behaviour in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, e31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2013). Teaching and life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Human Nature, 24, 351374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9180-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruzen, M., Willems, E. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). Culture and geographic variation in orang-utan behaviour. Current Biology, 21, 18081812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning and Behaviour, 32, 414. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langergraber, K. E., Boesch, C., Inoue, E., Inoue-Murayama, M., Mitani, J. C., Nishida, T., … Vigilant, L. (2011). Genetic and ‘cultural’ similarity in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 408416. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1112 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leadbeater, E., & Chittka, L. (2007). Social learning in insects - from minature brains to consensus building. Current Biology, 17, 703713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, R. A. (1984). Properties of culture: An ethnographic view. In Schweder, R. A., Levine, R. A. (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion (pp. 6787). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Eberly, L. E., & Pusey, A. E. (2004). Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees. Nature, 428, 715716. https://doi.org/10.1038/428715a CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luncz, L. V., & Boesch, C. (2014). Tradition over trend: Neighboring chimpanzee communities maintain differences in cultural behaviour despite frequent immigration of adult females. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 649657. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22259 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luncz, L., Wittig, R. M., & Boesch, C. (2015). Primate archaeology reveals cultural transmission patterns in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 370, 20140348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M., & Keil, F. C. (2011). The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 11581167. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0335 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1975119756. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704452104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall-Pescini, S., & Whiten, A. (2008a). Social learning of nut-cracking behaviour in East African sanctuary-living chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 186194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall-Pescini, S., & Whiten, A. (2008b). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: An experimental approach. Animal Cognition, 11, 449456. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0135-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Ham, R. M., White, L. T. J., Tutin, C. E. G., & Fernandez, M. (1997). Why don’t chimpanzees in Gabon crack nuts? International Journal of Primatology, 18, 353374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 118. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X493115 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuigan, N., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & Horner, V. (2007). Imitation of causally-opaque versus causally-transparent tool use by 3–5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22, 353364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1998). Infant imitation after a one-week delay: Longterm memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24, 470476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 34893501. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0129 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, T. J. H., Laland, K. N., & Harris, P. L. (2015). The development of adaptive conformity in young children: Effects of uncertainty and consensus. Developmental Science, 18, 511524. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12231 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murdock, G. P., Ford, C. S., Hudson, A. E., Kennedy, R., Simmons, L. W., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1987). Outline of cultural materials (5th Rev. Ed.). New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files.Google Scholar
Nagell, K., Olguin, R. S., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) . Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 174186. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.107.2.174 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Over-imitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21, 729736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where culture takes hold: ‘Overimitation’ and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal and Bushmen children. Child Development, 85, 21692184. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12265 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Developmental Science, 5, 722731. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M., & Widjojo, E. (2011). Failure to find over-imitation in captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus ): Implications for our understanding of cross-generation information transfer. In Håkansson, J. (Ed.), Developmental psychology (pp. 153167). New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.Google Scholar
Pagel, M. (2012). Wired for culture: Origins of the human social mind. New York, NY: WW Norton and Co.Google Scholar
Price, E. E., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2009). A potent effect of observational learning on chimpanzee tool construction. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 276, 33773383. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.0640 Google ScholarPubMed
Price, E. E., Wood, L. A., & Whiten, A. (in press). Adaptive cultural transmission biases in children and nonhuman primates. Infant Behavior and Development.Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Bigger knows better: Young children selectively learn rule games from adults rather than from peers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 785798. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151009X479178 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rawlings, B., Davila-Ross, M., & Boysen, S. T. (2014). Semi-wild chimpanzees open hard-shelled fruits differently across communities. Animal Cognition, 17, 891899. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0722-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santorelli, C. J., Schaffner, C. M., Campbell, C. J., Notman, H., Pavelka, M. S., Weghorst, J. A., & Aureli, F. (2011). Traditions in spider monkeys are biased towards the social domain. PLOS ONE, 6, e16863. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016863 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanz, C., Call, J., & Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letter, 5, 293296. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0786 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semaw, S., Rogers, M. J., Quade, J., Renne, P. R., Butler, R. F., Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., … Simpson, S. W. (2003). 2.6-million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution, 45, 169177. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(03)00093-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D. (2011). Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 10501059. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0369 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratchetting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 24052415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE, 5, e10544. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010544 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornton, A., & McCaulliffe, K. (2006). Teaching in wild meerkats. Science, 313, 227229. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1128727 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. J. (2008). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75, 18231836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1990). Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees? In Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 274311). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Davis-Dasilva, M., Camak, L., & Bard, K. (1987). Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees. Human Evolution, 2, 175183. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02436405 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495511. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0003123X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 16881705. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131463 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Waal, E., Renevey, N., Favre, C. M., & Bshary, R. (2010). Selective attention to philopatric models causes directed social learning in wild vervet monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 21052111. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.2260 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Waal, E., Borgeaud, C., & Whiten, A. (2013). Potent social learning and conformity shape a wild primate’s foraging decisions. Science, 6131, 483485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Waal, E., Claidière, N., & Whiten, A. (2015). Wild vervet monkeys copy alternative methods for opening an artificial fruit. Animal Cognition, 18, 617627. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0830-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., & Haun, D. B. M. (2014). A group-specific arbitrary tradition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 17, 14211425. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0766-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Haun, D. B. M., Mundry, R., & Bodamer, M. D. (2012). Neighbouring chimpanzee communities show different preferences in social grooming behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Biological Society B, 279, 43624367. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1543 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., & Haun, D. B. M. (2014). Conformity without majority? The case for demarcating social from majority influences. Animal Behaviour, 96, 187194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.08.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Kendal, R. L., Tennie, C., & Haun, D. B. M. (2015). Conformity and its look-a-likes. Animal Behaviour, 110, e1e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.07.030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., … Merrill, M. (2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102105. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1078004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P., & Burkart, J. M. (2011). Social learning and evolution: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 366, 10081016. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0304 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, M. B., & Andrade, M. G. (1996). Conformity in the Asch test as a function of age. British Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 367372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2005). The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature, 437, 5255. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2011). The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 366, 9971007. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0334 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2012). Social learning, traditions and culture. In Mitani, J., Call, J., Kappeler, P., Palombit, R., & Silk, J. (Eds.), The evolution of primate societies (pp. 681699). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2013). Social cognition: Making us smart, or sometimes making us dumb? Overimitation, conformity, non-conformity and the transmission of culture in ape and child. In Banaji, M., Gelman, S. (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 150154). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2015). Experimental studies illuminate the cultural transmission of percussive technology in Homo and Pan . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 370, 20140359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2017). Social learning and culture in child and chimpanzee. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 129154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Caldwell, C. A., & Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural diffusion in humans and other animals. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 1521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Erdal, D. (2012). The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Pan. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 21192129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Flynn, E. G. (2010). The transmission and evolution of experimental ‘microcultures’ in groups of young children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 16941709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., … Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685. https://doi.org/10.1038/21415 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., … Boesch, C. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 14811516. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853901317367717 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., & Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 21, 239283. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60146-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Laland, K. N., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Culture evolves. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 938948. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0372 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Horner, V., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437, 737740. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04047 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Learning and Behavior, 32, 3652. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2003). Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 92105. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10107 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2005). Selective imitation in child and chimpanzee: A window on the construal of others’ actions. In Hurley, S., Chater, N. (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (pp. 263283). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 24172428. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0069 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Mesoudi, A. (2008). Establishing an experimental science of culture: Animal social diffusion experiments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 34773488. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0134 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Milner, P. (1984). The educational environments of Nigerian infants. In Curran, V. (Eds.), Nigerian children: Developmental perspectives (pp. 3473). London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Spiteri, A., Horner, V., Bonnie, K. E., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Current Biology, 17, 10381043. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 603620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2012). Context-dependent model-based biases in cultural transmission: Children’s imitation is affected by model age over model knowledge state. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 33, 387394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zmyj, N., Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Daum, M. M. (2010). The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds’ imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 208220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zmyj, N., Daum, M. M., Prinz, W., Nielsen, M., & Aschersleben, G. (2012). Fourteen-month-olds’ imitation of differently aged models. Infant and Child Development, 21, 250266. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.750 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwinner, E., & Thornton, A. (2016). Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: Teaching is useful but not essential. Scientific Reports, 5, 16781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar