Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T18:46:16.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Styles of Counterfactual Thoughts in People with and without Signs of Depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 July 2015

Juliana Sarantopoulos Faccioli*
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal de São Carlos (Brazil)
Patricia Waltz Schelini
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal de São Carlos (Brazil)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Juliana Sarantopoulos. Av. Otto Werner Rosel, 1111. Cond. Moradas II. casa 466. Jardim Ipanema. CEP. 13563673. São Carlos (Brazil). Phone: +55–16991537088. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

People in a depressive state frequently believe that things could have happened differently in their lives, which is regarded as counterfactual thought. This study aimed to investigate whether the styles of counterfactual thought shown by people with indicating signs of depression and by people without any of those signs are different. Study participants were 42 adults divided into two groups: those with signs of depression and those without signs of depression. Five stories taken from newspapers and magazines and fictional scenarios were presented to the participants. After reading such stories, participants had to answer questions about how the narrated facts could have been different from what they actually were. Results indicated similar counterfactual thought styles in both groups (with and without depression signs). Most of the thoughts found were categorized as upward, subtractive, self-referent and that modified an aspect referred to action/inaction. A few significant differences between groups were observed, and most of them were obtained through directed modifications, in the last three stories. Some tendencies were shown in the group of people with signs of depression, such as those found for the subtractive category, t(40) = 2.33, p < .05, which can indicate a trend of possible failure in the preparatory function of CT, and in the hetero-referral function, t(40) = 1.97, p < .05, indicating a difficulty in thinking of different forms of action in the future. The removal of negative events may indicate that these experiences are not beneficial to people’s survival. These results may indicate some tendencies about how people in depressive states think about past events.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Associação Americana de Psiquiatria (APA) (2002). DSM-IV-TR- Manual diagnóstico e estatístico de transtornos mentais [Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders] (4 th Ed.), Porto Alegre, Brazil: Artmed.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1997). Terapia cognitiva da depressão [Cognitive therapy of depression] . Porto Alegre, Brazil: Artmed.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The rational imagination. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J., & Girotto, V. (2009). Cognitive processes in counterfactual thinking. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M. P., & Suhr, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. (pp. 151160). Nova York, NY. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cunha, J. A. (2001). Manual da versão em português das Escalas Beck [Portuguese version of the Beck Depression Inventory Manual] . São Paulo, Brazil: Casa do Psicólogo.Google Scholar
Faccioli, J. S., & Schelini, P. W. (2009). A imaginação de crianças com necessidades especiais [The imagination of children with special needs] . (Unpublished monografia). São Carlos, Brazil: Universidade Federal de São Carlos.Google Scholar
Juhos, C., Quelhas, A. C., & Senos, J (2003). Pensamento contrafactual na depressão [Counterfactual thought in depression]. Psychologica, 32, 199215.Google Scholar
Justino, F. L. C., Faccioli, J. S., & Schelini, P. W. (2013, June). Proposta de técnica para avaliação de pensamento contrafactual em adultos [Technical proposal for evaluation of counterfactual thinking in adults]. Paper presented at the VI Congresso Brasileiro de Avaliação Psicológica. Avaliação psicológica: Direito de todos, dever do psicólogo. Maceió, Brazil.Google Scholar
Justino, F. L. C., & Schelini, P. W. (2010). Análise da capacidade de modificação de estórias em crianças, adultos e idosos [Analysis of changing stories capacity in children, adults and seniors]. (Unpublished monografia). Säo Carlos, Brazil: Universidade Federal de São Carlos.Google Scholar
Markman, K. D., Karadogan, F., Lindberg, M. J., & Zell, E. (2009). Counterfactual thinking: Function and dysfunction. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M. P., & Suhr, J. A. (Eds.). Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. (pp. 175195). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
McCloy, R., & Byrne, R. M. J. (2000). Counterfactual thinking about controllable events. Memory & Cognition, 28, 10711078. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209355 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organização Mundial de Saúde (OMS) (1993). Classificação de transtornos mentais e do comportamento da CID-10: Descrições Clínicas e Diretrizes Diagnósticas [Classifications of mental disorders and the ICD-10 behavior: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines] . Porto Alegre, Brazil: Artes Médicas.Google Scholar
Quelhas, A. C., Power, M. J., Juhos, C., & Senos, J. (2008). Counterfactual thinking and functional differences in depression. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 15, 352356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.593 Google Scholar
Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805818. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.66.5.805 Google Scholar
Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 133148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.121.1.133 Google Scholar
Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1993). Self-esteem and counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 199206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.65.1.199 Google Scholar
Roese, N., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Counterfactual thinking: A critical overview. In Roese, N. J. & Olson, J. M. (Eds.). What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 169197). New Jersey, NJ: Lawrense Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Teixeira, J. M. (2001 ). Comorbilidade: Depressão e ansiedade [Comorbidity: Depression and anxiety]. Repositório Aberto - Universidade do Porto, Porto, Portugal. Retrieved from http://www.saude-mental.net/pdf/vol3_rev1_artigo1.pdf Google Scholar
Teng, C. T., Humes, E. C., & Demetrius, F. N. (2005). Depressão e comorbidades clínicas [Depression and clinical comorbidity]. Revista de Psiquiatria Clínica. 32, 149159.Google Scholar
Wells, G. L., & Gavanski, I. (1989). Conterfactual process of normal and exceptional events. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 314325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(89)90025-5 Google Scholar
Wong, E. M., Galinsky, A. D., & Kray, L. J. (2009). The counterfactual mind-set: A decade of research. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M. P., & Suhr, J. A. Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation (pp. 161174). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO) (2011). Depression. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/depression/en Google Scholar