Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T23:39:39.420Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive Control on Memory Test Performance across Life: Test of Memory Strategies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2019

María Abellán-Martínez*
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Miguel Ángel Castellanos López
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
María Luisa Delgado-Losada
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Raquel Yubero
Affiliation:
Hospital Quirón (Spain)
Nuria Paúl
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Fernando Maestú Unturbe
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to María Abellán Martínez. Universidad Complutense. Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Procesos Cognitivos y Logopedia. Campus de Somosaguas, Pozuelo de Alarcón, 28223 Madrid (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The ability to generate memory strategies is a key factor in performance of episodic memory tests. There is evidence about the inefficient use of memory strategies in old adults. However, a question remains unresolved: Worse performance on memory test in the older people is due to an inability to mobilize cognitive strategies or to an episodic memory deficit? In this study we tried to answer it by using the Test of Memory Strategies (TMS), which parametrically reduces the need of executive functions on memory tests. The test consists of five experimental conditions (TMS1–5) where a progressive external organization of the material reduces the need to mobilize memory strategies. TMS was applied to a sample of 180 participants (n = 180) divided into three age groups (25–45; 46–65; 66–85). The results showed an increased performance in all groups groups (F(2, 177) = 14.79, p < .001) across conditions (F(3.88,674.04) = 292.48, p < .001), without group differences in those conditions with a maximum reduction of the need of executive functions (F(7.61,674.04) = 1.95, p = .053). However, middle age and older adults showed more difficulties in establishing cognitive strategies, in the initial conditions. These results lead to the conclusion that the typical pattern of low performance on episodic memory tasks in the older population may be due to the deterioration of executive functions and not mainly to a primary decline of memory process.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

How to cite this article:

Abellán-Martínez, M., Castellanos López, M. A., Delgado-Losada, M. L., Yubero, R., Paúl, N., & Maestú Unturbe, F. (2019). Executive control on memory test performance across life: Test of Memory Strategies. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 22. e50. Doi:10.1017/sjp.2019.47

References

Bailey, H. R., Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (2014). Does strategy training reduce age-related deficits in working memory? Gerontology, 60(4), 346356. https://doi.org/10.1159/000356699CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (2017). Memory self-efficacy and memory performance in older adults: The mediating role of task persistence. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 76(1), 2333. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouazzaoui, B., Angel, L., Fay, S., Taconnat, L., Charlotte, F., & Isingrini, M. (2014). Does the greater involvement of executive control in memory with age act as a compensatory mechanism? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(1), 5966. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brickenkamp, R., & Zillmer, E. (1998). The d2 Test of Attention. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.Google Scholar
Burger, L., Uittenhove, K., Lemaire, P., & Taconnat, L. (2017). Strategy difficulty effects in young and older adults’ episodic memory are modulated by inter-stimulus intervals and executive control processes. Acta Psychologica, 175, 5059. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chepenik, L. G., Cornew, L. A., & Farah, M. J. (2007). The influence of sad mood on cognition. Emotion, 7(4), 802811. http://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheke, L. G. (2016). What-where-when memory and encoding strategies in healthy aging. Learning & Memory, 23(3), 121126. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.040840.115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Frias, C. M., Dixon, R. A., & Bäckman, L. (2003). Use of memory compensation strategies is related to psychosocial and health indicators. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(1), 1222. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.1.p12CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabricio, A. T., & Yassuda, M. S. (2011). Use of memory strategies among younger and older adults: Results from objective and subjective measures. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 5(2), 9398. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642011DN05020006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, S. M., Araújo, A. M., Vázquez-Justo, E., Pereira, C., Silva, A., Paul, N., … Maestú, F. (2018). Effects of aging on memory strategies: A validation of the Portuguese version of the test of Memory Strategies. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32, 133151. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2018.1490456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortin, A. & Caza, N. (2014). A validation study of memory and executive functions indexes in French speaking healthy young and older adults. Canadian Journal on Aging/La Revue Canadienne Du Vieillissement, 33(1), 6071. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980813000445CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, M. C., Baldock, Z., Freeman, S. P., & Berry, J. M. (2016). The role of encoding strategy in younger and older adult associative recognition: A think-aloud analysis. Psychology and Aging, 31(5), 471480. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenmolen, N. L., Overdorp, E. J., Fasotti, L., Claassen, J. A. H. R., Kessels, R. P. C., & Oosterman, J. M. (2017). Memory strategy use in older adults with subjective memory complaints. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(5), 10611065. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40520-016-0635-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C. J., & Freshwater, S. M. (2002). The Stroop Color and Word Test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(7), 491505. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, J. W., John, S. E., Adams, J. W., Bussell, C. A., Saurman, J. L., & Gavett, B. E. (2017). The effects of age on the learning and forgetting of primacy, middle, and recency components of a multi-trial word list. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 39(9), 900912. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2017.1278746CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, A. L., & Rebok, G. W. (2011). Memory training and strategy use in older adults: Results from the ACTIVE study. Psychology and Aging, 26(3), 503517. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022687CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harada, C. N., Love, M. C. N., & Triebel, K. L. (2013). Normal Cognitive Aging. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 29(4), 737752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Instituto Nacional de Estadística (INE) (2017). Activos por nivel de formación alcanzado, sexo y grupo de edad [Active persons by educational level reached, sex and age group]. Retrieved from INE website http://www.ine.es/jaxiT3/Datos.htm?t=6363Google Scholar
Kaiser, S., Simon, J. J., Kalis, A., Schweizer, S., Tobler, P. N., & Mojzisch, A. (2013). The cognitive and neural basis of option generation and subsequent choice. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(4), 814829. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0175-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemaire, P., & Reder, L. (1999). What affects strategy selection in arithmetic? The example of parity and five effects on product verification. Memory & Cognition, 27(2), 364382. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, B., Zhu, X., Hou, J., Chen, T., Wang, P., & Li, J. (2016). Combined cognitive training vs. memory strategy training in healthy older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 834. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00834CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lobo, A., Ezquerra, J., Burgada, F. G., Sala, J. M., & Seva, A. (1979). El Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo. Un test sencillo y práctico para detectar alteraciones intelectuales en pacientes médicos [The Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo: A simple and practical test to detect intellectual dysfunctions in medical patients]. Actas Luso-Españolas de Neurología y Psiquiatría, 7,189202Google Scholar
Luszcz, M. A., & Lane, A. P. (2008). Executive function in cognitive, neuropsychological, and clinical aging. In Hofer, S. M. & Alwin, D. F. (Eds.), The handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 193206). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mata, R., Schooler, L. J., & Rieskamp, J. (2007). The aging decision maker: Cognitive aging and the adaptive selection of decision strategies. Psychology and Aging, 22(4), 796810. http://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.796CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rami, L., Valls-Pedret, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Caprile, C., Solé-Padullés, C., Castellví, M., … Molinuevo, J. L. (2011). Cuestionario de reserva cognitiva. Valores obtenidos en población anciana sana y con enfermedad de Alzheimer [Cognitive reserve questionnaire. Scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer’s disease]. Revista de Neurología, 52, 195201. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5204.2010478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8(3), 271276. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). Compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd Ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tanner, J. L., & Arnett, J. J. (2009). The emergence of emerging adulthood: The new life stage between adolescence and young adulthood. In Furlong, A. (Ed.), Handbook of youth and young adulthood: New perspectives and agendas (p. 41). London, UK and New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Votruba, K. L, Persad, C., & Giordani, B. (2016). Cognitive deficits in healthy elderly population with “normal” scores on the mini-mental state examination. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 29(3), 126132. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988716629858CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2012). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos–IV: WAIS–IV [Wechsler Adults Intelligence Scale-IV: WAIS-IV]. Madrid, Spain: Pearson Educación.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2013). Escala de memoria de Wechsler–IV: WMS–IV [Wechsler Memory Scale-IV: WMS-IV]. Madrid, Spain: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). BADS: Bhavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome . Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Company.Google Scholar
Yubero, R., Gil, P., Paul, N., & Maestú, F. (2011). Influence of memory strategies on memory test performance: A study in healthy and pathological aging. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 18(5), 497515. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.597840CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed