Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T18:46:38.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Effect of Content and Physical Properties of Affective Pictures on Emotional Responses

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Juan Pedro Sánchez-Navarro*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia
José María Martínez-Selva
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia
Francisco Román
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia
Ginesa Torrente
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to J.P. Sánchez-Navarro, Departamento de Anatomía Humana y Psicobiología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Murcia, Campus de Espinardo, 30100 Murcia (Spain). Phone: 968 367 707. E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

The aim of this research was to study the influence of both the emotional content and the physical characteristics of affective stimuli on the psychophysiological, behavioral and cognitive indexes of the emotional response. We selected 54 pictures from the IAPS, depicting unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant contents, and used two picture sizes as experimental conditions (120 × 90 cm and 52 × 42 cm). Sixty-one subjects were randomly assigned to each experimental condition. We recorded the startle blink reflex, skin conductance response, heart rate, free viewing time, and picture valence and arousal ratings. In line with previous research (e.g., Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert, & Lang, 2001), our data showed an effect of the affective content on all the measurements recorded. Importantly, effects of the size of the affective pictures on emotional responses were not found, indicating that the emotional content is more important than the formal properties of the stimuli in evoking the emotional response.

El objetivo de esta investigación fue estudiar la influencia del contenido emocional y de las características físicas de los estímulos afectivos sobre los índices psicofisiológicos, conductuales y cognitivos de la respuesta emocional. Seleccionamos 54 imágenes del IAPS, representando contenidos desagradables, neutros y agradables y usamos dos tamaños de imagen como condiciones experimentales (120 × 90 cm y 52 × 42 cm). Los 61 sujetos que conformaban la muestra experimental se asignaron de forma aleatoria a cada condición experimental. Registramos el parpadeo reflejo de sobresalto, la respuesta de conductancia de la piel, la frecuencia cardiaca, el tiempo de visión y las estimaciones de valencia afectiva y arousal de las imágenes. De acuerdo con la investigación previa (p. ej., Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert y Lang, 2001), nuestros datos mostraron un efecto del contenido afectivo en todas las medidas registradas. Nuestros resultados no mostraron, sin embargo, efectos del tamaño de las imágenes afectivas sobre ninguna de las medidas registradas, lo que indica que el contenido afectivo es más importante que las propiedades formales de los estímulos para provocar respuestas emocionales.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bagiella, E., Sloan, R.P., & Heitjan, D.F. (2000). Mixed-effects models in psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 37, 1320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M.M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., & Bernston, G.G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed, pp. 602642). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bradley, M.M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B.N., & Lang, P.J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M.M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D. & Lang, P.J. (2001). Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 300319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N., & Lang, P.J. (1990). Startle reflex modification: Emotion or attention? Psychophysiology, 27, 513522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J.T., & Sandman, C.A. (1981). Psychophysiological functioning, cognitive responding, and attitudes. In Petty, R.E., Ostrom, T.M., & Brock, T.C. (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion (pp. 81104). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention [CESEA-NIHM] (1999). The International Affective Picture System: Digitized photographs. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
Christie, I.C., & Friedman, B.H. (2004). Autonomic specificity of discrete emotion and dimensions of affective space: A multivariate approach. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 51, 143153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B.N., Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (1996). Probing picture perception: Activation or emotion. Psychophysiology, 33, 103111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Detenber, B.H., & Reeves, B. (1996). A bio-informational theory of emotion: Motion and image size effects on viewers. Journal of Communication, 46, 6684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowles, D.C., Christie, M.J., Edelberg, R., Grings, W.W., Lykken, D.T., & Venables, P.H. (1981). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 18, 232239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridlund, A.J., & Cacioppo, J.T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Globisch, J., Hamm, A.O., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1999). Fear appears fast: Temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 36, 6675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, P., Zimmermann, P., Guttormsen-Schär, S., & Danuser, B. (2005). Respiratory responses associated with affective processing of film stimuli. Biological Psychology, 68, 223235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, M.K., Cook, E.W., & Lang, P.J. (1989). Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 5164.Google Scholar
Grimshaw, G.M., Bulman-Fleming, M.B., & Ngo, C. (2004). A signal-detection analysis of sex differences in the perception of emotional faces. Brain and Cognition, 54, 248250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, D.M., & Frijda, N.H. (1994). Modulation of the acoustic startle response by film-induced fear and sexual arousal. Psychophysiology, 31, 565571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P.J. (1968). Fear reduction and fear behavior: Problems in treating a construct. In Schlien, J. (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy (Vol. 3, pp. 205227). Washington: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Lang, P.J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. In , J.B., Sidowski, , Johnson, J.H., & Williams, T.A. (Eds.), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp. 119137). Norwood: Ablex.Google Scholar
Lang, P.J. (1984). Cognition in emotion: Concept and action. In Izard, C.E., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R.B. (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 198228). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, P.J. (1994). The motivational organization of emotion: Affect-reflex connections. In Van Goozen, S.H.M., Van de Poll, N.E., & Sergeant, J.A. (Eds.), Emotions: Essays on emotion theory (pp. 6193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lang, P.J. (1995). The emotion probe. American Psychologist, 50, 372385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., & Cuthbert, B.N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 12481263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P.J., Greenwald, M.K., Bradley, M.M., & Hamm, A.O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavior reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, C.L., Ruffalo, D., Nietert, J.Y., & Davidson, R.J. (2000). Temporal stability of the emotion-modulated startle response. Psychophysiology, 37, 92101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Selva, J.M., Román, F., & Sánchez-Navarro, J.P. (2001, May-June). Modulation of the eyeblink startle reflex and stimulus size. Communication presented at the II International Workshop on Emotion and the Brain, Palma de Mallorca, Spain.Google Scholar
Moltó, J., Montañés, S., Poy, R., Segarra, P., Pastor, M.C., Tormo, M.P., Ramírez, I., Hernández, M.A., Sánchez, M., Fernández, M.C., & Vila, J. (1999). Un nuevo método para el estudio experimental de las emociones: El International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Adaptación española. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 52, 5587.Google Scholar
Mühlberger, A., Herrmann, M.J., Wiedemann, G., Ellgring, H., & Pauli, P. (2001). Repeated exposure of flight phobics to flights in virtual reality. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 10331050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeves, B., Lang, A., Kim, E.Y., & Tatar, D. (1999). The effects of screen size and message content on attention and arousal. Media Psychology, 1, 4967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes del Paso, G.A., & Vila, J. (1998). The continuing problem of incorrect heart rate estimation in psychophysiological studies: An off-line solution for cardiotachometer users. Biological Psychology, 48, 269279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossignol, M., Philippot, P., Douilliez, C., Crommelinck, M., & Campanella, S. (2005). The perception of fearful and happy facial expression is modulated by anxiety: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 377, 115120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Navarro, J.P., Martínez-Selva, J.M., & Román, F. (2005). Emotional response in patients with frontal brain damage: Effects of the affective valence and information content. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 8797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-Navarro, J.P., Martínez-Selva, J.M., & Román, F. (2006). Uncovering the relationship between defence and orienting in emotion: cardiac reactivity to unpleasant pictures. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 3446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, R.F., Detenber, B.H., Reiss, J.E., & Shults, C.W. (2000). Image motion and context: A between- and within-subjects comparison. Psychophysiology, 37, 706710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, R.F., Detenber, B.H., Roedema, T.M., & Reiss, J.E. (1999). Emotion processing in three systems: The medium and the message. Psychophysiology, 36, 619627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venables, P.H., & Christie, M.J. (1980). Electrodermal activity. In Martin, I. & Venables, P.H. (Eds.), Techniques in psychophysiology (pp. 467). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vila, J., Fernández, M.C., Pegalajar, J., Vera, M.N., Robles, H., Pérez, N., Sánchez, M.B., Ramírez, I., & Ruíz-Padial, E. (2003). A new look at cardiac defense: Attention or emotion? The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 6, 6078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vila, J., Sánchez, M., Ramírez, I., Fernández, M.C., Cobos, P., Rodríguez, S., Muñoz, M.A., Tormo, M.P., Herrero, M., Segarra, P., Pastor, M.C., Montañés, S., Poy, R., & Moltó, J. (2001). El sistema internacional de imágenes afectivas (IAPS): Adaptación española. Segunda parte. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 54, 635657.Google Scholar
Vrana, S.R., Spence, E.L., & Lang, P.J. (1988). The startle probe response: A new measure of emotion? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 487491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wald, J., & Taylor, S. (2000). Efficacy of virtual reality exposure therapy to treat driving phobia: A case report. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 31, 249257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed