Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T08:29:24.627Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Comparison of Implicit Memory Tests in Schizophrenic Patients and Normal Controls

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

María José Soler*
Affiliation:
Universitat de València
Juan Carlos Ruiz
Affiliation:
Universitat de València
Inmaculada Fuentes
Affiliation:
Universitat de València
Pilar Tomás
Affiliation:
Centro de Rehabilitación e Inserción Social de Enfermos Mentales de Velluters. Conselleria de Bienestar Social de la Generalitat Valenciana y Eulen Servicios Sociosanitarios
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to María José Soler, Departamento de Psicología Básica., Universidad de Valencia, Avda. Blasco Ibáñez, 21, 46010 – Valencia (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The objective of the current study was to compare the performance of schizophrenic patients and normal controls on implicit memory tests. Two neuropsychological tasks were administered to 29 patients and normal participant samples. The implicit tests were: Word fragment completion and Word production from semantic categories. The priming score was the variable of interest. Priming effects are obtained in normal subjects and schizophrenia patients, regardless of the implicit test used. However, a dissociation in priming between normal and patient groups was observed, depending on the test used. For word fragment test, priming was identical between the two groups. However, for word production, priming obtained in schizophrenics was lower than priming in normal controls. Results confirm a dissociation effect in implicit memory tests. These results could be explained in the context of the Roediger and Blaxton (1987) distinction between data-driven and conceptually-driven processing. This evidence suggests that a complete neuropsychological assessment of memory in schizophrenia should include different kinds of implicit memory tests (procedural, perceptual, and conceptual tasks).

El objetivo de este estudio fue comparar la ejecución de pacientes esquizofrénicos y controles no clínicos en tests de memoria implícita. Se administraron dos pruebas neuropsicológicas a una muestra de 29 pacientes y a dos muestras de participantes no clínicos. Los tests implícitos fueron: Completar fragmentos de palabras y Generación de ejemplares de categorías semánticas. La variable de interés fue la facilitación. Los efectos de facilitación se obtienen en sujetos no clínicos y en pacientes esquizofrénicos, independientemente del test implícito que se utilice. Sin embargo, se ha observado una disociación en la facilitación entre grupos de controles y pacientes, dependiendo del test utilizado. En tests de fragmentos de palabras la facilitación es idéntica en los dos grupos. Sin embargo, en producción de palabras, la facilitación obtenida en esquizofrénicos es menor que la facilitación en controles. Los resultados confirman la existencia de un efecto disociativo en tests de memoria implícita. Estos resultados podrían explicarse en el contexto de la distinción de Roediger y Blaxton (1987) entre procesamiento guiado por los datos y procesamiento guiado conceptualmente. Esta evidencia sugiere que la evaluación neuropsicológica de la memoria en la esquizofrenia debería incluir distintos tipos de pruebas de memoria implícita (procedimentales, perceptuales, y conceptuales).

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Aleman, A., Hijman, R., de Haan, E.H.F., & Kahn, R.S. (1999). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 13581366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blyler, C.R., Gold, J.M., Iannone, V.N., & Buchana, R.W. (2000). Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 46, 209215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brebion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M.J., & Gorman, J.M. (1997). Mechanisms underlying memory impairment in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 27, 383393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clare, L., McKenna, P.J., Mortimer, A.M., & Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Long-term memory in schizophrenia: What is impaired and what is preserved? Neuropsychologia, 31, 12251241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasi, C., Soler, M.J., & Ruiz, J.C. (2004). Normative data on the familiarity and difficulty of 196 Spanish word fragments. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 559563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, L., & O'Carroll, R. (1994). Memory impairment in schizophrenia – a comparison with that observed in the alcoholic Korsakoff syndrome. Psychological Medicine, 24, 155165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elvevag, B., Weinstock, D.M., Akil, M., Kleinman, J.E., & Goldberg, T.E. (2001). A comparison of verbal fluency tasks in schizophrenic patients and normal controls. Schizophrenia Research, 51, 119126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay, S., Isingrini, M., & Clarys, D. (2005). Effects of depth-of-processing and ageing on word-stem and word-fragment implicit memory tasks: Test of the lexical-processing hypothesis. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 785802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, J.M., Randolph, C., & Carpenter, C.J. (1992). The performance of patients with schizophrenia on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 367373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, T.E., Torrey, E.F., Gold, J.M., Ragland, J.D., Bigelow, L.B., & Weinberg, D.R. (1993). Learning and memory in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 23, 7185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granholm, E., Bartzokis, G., Asarnow, R.F., & Marder, S.R. (1993). Preliminary associations between motor and procedural learning, basal ganglia T2 relaxation times, and tardative dyskinesia in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 50, 3344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf, P., & Schacter, D. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 501518.Google ScholarPubMed
Green, M.F., Kern, R.S., & Williams, O. (1997). Procedural learning in schizophrenia: Evidence from serial reaction. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 123134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, R.W., & Zakzanis, K.K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology, 12, 426445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, A., Baumgartner, S., Bodner, T., Edlinger, M., Hummer, H., Kemmler, G., Rettenbacher, M.A., & Fleischhacker, W.W. (2006). Patient outcomes in schizophrenia II: The impact of cognition. European Psychiatry, 20, 395402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazes, M., Berthet, L., Danion, J.M., Amado, I., Willard, D., Robert, P., & Poirier, M.F. (1999). Impairment of consciously controlled use of memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 13, 5461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keri, S., Kelemen, O., Szekeres, G., Bagoczky, N., Erdélyi, A., Antal, G., Benedek, G., & Janka, Z. (2000). Schizophrenics know more than they can tell: Probabilistic classification learning in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 30, 149155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Light, L.L., Prull, M.W., La Voie, D., & Healy, M.R. (2000). Dual-process theories of memory in old age. In Perfect, T.J. & Taylor, E.A. (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 238300). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenna, P.J., Ornstein, T., & Baddeley, A.D. (2002) Schizophrenia. In Baddeley, A.D., Kopelman, M.D., & Wilson, B.A. (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders (pp. 413435). West Sussex, UK: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Pascual, J.P., & Musitu, G. (1980). Normas categoriales. Psicológica, 1, 157174.Google Scholar
Reber, P.J., & Squire, L.R. (1999). Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. Behaviour Neurosciences, 113, 235242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H.L. (1990). Implicit memory. Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 10431056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roediger, H.L., & Blaxton, T.A. (1987). Retrieval modes produce dissociations in memory for surface information. In Gorfein, D. & Hoffman, R.R. (Eds.), Memory and cognitive processes: The Ebbinghaus Centennial Conference (pp. 349379). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roediger, H.L., Srinivas, K., & Weldon, M.S. (1989). Dissociations between implicit measures of retention. In Lewandowsky, S., Dunn, J.C., & Kirsner, K. (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 6784). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rossells, S.L., Shapleske, J., & David, A.S. (1998). Sentence verification and delusions: A content-specific deficit. Psychological Medicine, 28, 11891198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B.L., Rosse, R.B., & Deutsch, S.I. (1993). Limits of the processing view in accounting for dissociations among memory measures in a clinical population. Memory and Cognition, 21, 6372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B.L., Rosse, R.B., Veazey, C., & Deutsch, S.I. (1996). Impaired motor skill learning in schizophrenia: Implications for corticostriatal dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry, 39, 241248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sponheim, S.R., Steele, V.R., & McGuire, K.A. (2004). Verbal memory processes in schizophrenia patients and biological relatives of schizophrenia patients: Intact implicit memory, impaired explicit recollection. Schizophrenia Research, 71, 339348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, J.P. (2000). Nonconscious forms of human memory. In Tulving, E. & Craik, F.I.M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 245261). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Adultos-III. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Wilson, B.A., Alderman, N., & Burgess, P.W. (1998). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Bury St. Edmund, UK: Thames Valley Test Co.Google Scholar
Wilson, B.A., Cockburn, J.M., Baddeley, A.D., & Hiorns, R. (1989). The development and validation of a test battery for detecting and monitoring everyday memory problems. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 855870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B.A., & Zangwill, O. (2002). Assessment of memory disorders. In Baddeley, A.D., Kopelman, M.D., & Wilson, B.A. (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders. Chichester, UK: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar