Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-01T11:10:29.683Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adult Performance in Naming Spatial Dimensions of Objects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Miguel Ángel Galeote Moreno*
Affiliation:
University of Málaga
Herminia Peraita Adrados
Affiliation:
Open University of Madrid
Elena Checa Ponce
Affiliation:
University of Málaga
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr. Miguel Ángel Galeote Moreno, Dto. de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Facultad de Psicología.Universidad de Málaga. Campus de Teatinos, s/n. 29071 Málaga (Spain). FAX: 34-52 213 26 35. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Most work on acquisition of lexical meaning in developmental psycholinguistics is based on the idea of the relevance of the adult model, which is generally described in relation to certain theoretical semantic analyses. Up to the present, adult behavior itself has not been examined and its validity as a model for children has been taken for granted. This paper analyzes the knowledge of spatial terms, namely dimensional terms, shown by a group of 20 adults. The results show that the adult subjects used in our study - supposedly linguistically competent - committed errors, significantly varying their strategies for naming dimensions from one case to another, and showing a lack of consistency between them. The results are discussed in terms of assumed theoretical validity with regard to theoretical semantic analysis, as well as the methods of research about the acquisition of lexical meaning.

Gran parte del trabajo sobre la adquisición del significado léxico en psicolingüística evolutiva suele tomar como referencia el modelo adulto de respuesta, el cual se define en relación con ciertos modelos semánticos teóricos. Según esto, la conducta adulta no se suele examinar empíricamente, dándose por supuesta su validez como modelo con el que comparar la conducta de los niños. En este artículo se analiza específicamente el conocimiento de términos espaciales, más concretamente, adjetivos dimensionales, mostrado por un grupo de 20 adultos. Los resultados muestran que los sujetos adultos de nuestro estudio, supuestamente competentes desde el punto de vista lingüístico, cometían errores, variaban significativamente sus estrategias al nombrar las distintas dimensiones y mostraban un buen número de inconsistencias entre ellos. Se analizan estos resultados en función de la validez teórica asumida en relación con los modelos semánticos teóricos y su repercusión con respecto a los métodos de investigación en la adquisición del significado léxico.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1999

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abkarian, G. G. (1982). Comprehension of deictic locatives: the object «behind» it. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 229245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and Conceptual Development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Aurnague, M., Borillo, M., & Vieu, L. (1991). A cognitive approach to the semantics of space. Cognitiva-90. Madrid: Afcet.Google Scholar
Bartlett, E. J. (1976). Sizing Things up: the acquisition of the Meaning of Dimensional Adjectives. Journal of Child Language, 3, 205219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berndt, R., & Caramazza, A. (1978). The development of vague modifiers in the language of pre-school children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 279294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bierwisch, M. (1967). Some semantic universals of German adjectivals. Foundations of Language, 3, 136.Google Scholar
Blewitt, P. (1983). “Dog” vs. “collie”: Vocabulary in speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 19, 602609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bomba, P. C. (1984). The development of orientation categories between 2 and 4 months of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 609636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Perceptual anisotropies in infancy: ontogenetic origins and implications of inequality in spatial vision. In Reese, H.W. & Lipsitt, L.P. (Eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 16). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1988). Perceptual development across the life cycle. In Bornstein, M.H. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Developmental Psychology: an advanced text-book (2nd edition, pp. 151204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Callanam, M. A. (1985). How parents label objects for young children: the role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development, 56, 508523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M. A. (1990). Parents' descriptions of objects: potential data for children's inferences about category principles. Cognitive Development, 5, 101122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J., & Miller, G.A. (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1982). Semantic development-State of the art. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. (Eds.), Language Acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T.E. (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1980). Here's the top: nonlinguistic strategies in the acquisition of orientational terms. Child Development, 51, 329338.Google Scholar
Clark, H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore, T.E. (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 2763). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H., & Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and language: an introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Corrales, C. (1977). El campo semántico «dimensión» en español. Sta. Cruz de Tenerife: Ed. Aula de Cultura de Tenerife.Google Scholar
Cox, M. V., & Richardson, J. R. (1985). How do Children Describe Spatial Relationships? Journal of Child Language, 12, 611620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donaldson, M., & Wales, R. (1970). On the acquisition of some relational terms. In Hayes, J.R. (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 235268). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Ellington, J. (1974). The acquisition of word-meaning for dimensional adjectives: the long and short of it. Journal of Child Language, 1, 195204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essock, E. A. (1980). The oblique effect of stimulus identification considered with respect to two classes of oblique effects. Perception, 9, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galeote, M. A. (1995). La adquisición de los adjetivos dimensionales y la estructura del significado léxico. Madrid: Servicio de Investigación de la Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia.Google Scholar
Goede, K. (1989). Language acquisition and development of children's “bigger” and “more” judgments. In Bierwisch, M. & Lang, E. (Eds.), Dimensional adjectives: Grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation (pp. 419432). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greimas, A. J. (1970). La semántica estructural. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Hayes, L. A., & Watson, J. S. (1981). Facial orientation of parents and elicited smiling by infants. Infant Behaviour and Development, 4, 333340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Smiley, P., & Ratner, H. (1983). What do word meanings reveal about conceptual development?. In Selier, T. B. & Wannenmacher, W., Concept development and the development of word meaning. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Lang, E. (1989). The semantics of dimensional designation of spatial objects. In Bierwisch, M. & Lang, E. (Eds.), Dimensional Adjectives: grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1980). Semántica. Barcelona: Ed. Teide, S.A. (2nd edition) (Spanish translation from original: Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1977).Google Scholar
Moliner, M. (1990). Diccionario de uso del español. (1st Edition) Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Piérart, B. (1977). L'acquisition du sens des Marqueurs de Relation Spatiale Devant et Dèrriere. Année Psychologique, 77, 95116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravn, K. E., & Gelman, S. A. (1984). Rule usage in children's understanding of «big» and «little». Child Development, 55, 21412150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, M. M. (1979). Sorting out what's in a word from what's not: evaluating Clark's semantic features acquisition theory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipley, E. F.Kuhn, I.F., & Madden, E. C. (1983). Mothers' use of superordinate category terms. Journal of Child Language, 10, 571588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teller, P. (1969). Some discussion and extension of Manfred Bierwisch's work on German adjectivals. Foundations of Language, 5, 185217.Google Scholar