Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T18:41:53.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Endurance of Uncertainty: Antisociality and Ontological Anarchy in British Psychiatry, 1950–2010

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 February 2014

Martyn Pickersgill*
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Argument

Research into the biological markers of pathology has long been a feature of British psychiatry. Such somatic indicators and associated features of mental disorder often intertwine with discourse on psychological and behavioral correlates and causes of mental ill-health. Disorders of sociality – particularly psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder – are important instances where the search for markers of pathology has a long history; research in this area has played an important role in shaping how mental health professionals understand the conditions. Here, I characterize the multiplicity of psychiatric praxis that has sought to define the mark of antisociality as a form of “ontological anarchy.” I regard this as an essential feature of the search for biological and other markers of an unstable referent, positing that uncertainties endure – in part – precisely because of attempts to build consensus regarding the ontology of antisociality through biomedical means. Such an account is suggestive of the co-production of biomarkers, mental disorder, and psychiatric institutions.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution licence <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/>.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014

Introduction

In Britain, antisociality has long been regarded as an area of potential psychiatric concern.Footnote 1 As such, considerable discussion has revolved around its epistemological and ontological status: how should this pathology be studied? What causes it? Whilst hotly debated, these key questions have proved difficult to resolve. Nevertheless, in spite of this characteristic uncertainty, individual mental health professionals continue to have specific ideas about what might cause disorders of sociality – such as psychopathic personality disorder (psychopathy) and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) – advancing these in contributions to journals and assuming them within their research and practice.

In this paper, I analyze some of the debates pertaining to antisociality as they have played out within British psychiatric discourse. In particular, I am concerned with the place, role, and impact of biological correlates of psychopathy and ASPD.Footnote 2 Twenty-first-century attention to the role of the biological within mental illness continues a long-tradition within British psychiatry (Moncrieff and Crawford Reference Moncrieff and Crawford2001); research into “biomarkers” – biological correlates of pathology that have implications for diagnosis, prognosis, treatment and drug development – is not new. Somatic motifs pattern the narratives of psychiatric discourse, developed through and further developing theory, research, and clinical practice.

Yet, so too do other themes. Indeed, the on-going articulation of biological, psychological, and sociological discourse within the mental health professions complicates attempts to rigidly characterize particular research programs or clinical frameworks as, for instance, “biological” psychiatry. Often, we may see the intertwining of concepts and practices. This renders problematic attempts to understand the social life of biomarkers without also attending to (what mental health professionals call) the “biopsychosocial” spaces (see Pilgrim Reference Pilgrim2002) within which they find mobility, gain traction, and develop salience. Accordingly, in this paper I do not solely analyze discussions of the somatic aspects of disorders associated with antisociality, but rather situate these within a broader examination of the shifting aetiological emphases of psychiatry.

Methodologically, the analysis presented develops from a close reading of research papers, editorials, correspondence, and book reviews pertaining to antisociality within the British Journal of Psychiatry (hereafter, BJP) from 1950–2010 (and other key articles). The BJP has long been and remains one of the key Anglophonic journals of psychiatry, and an interrogation of its contents provides an important window into the changing professional dialogues, debates and disagreements that have played out within UK mental health theory, research and practice.Footnote 3 Attention to ASPD and psychopathy is especially warranted given the relative dearth of historical and sociological literature on these categories; this is all the more striking when the profound effects such labels may have on men and women are borne in mind. Whilst the paper focuses on pathological antisociality, however, I suggest that the wider dynamics characterized herein might likewise be apparent for other contested categories, such as anorexia (Gremillion Reference Gremillion2003), autism (Silverman Reference Silverman2012), depression (Ehrenberg Reference Ehrenberg2010), and schizophrenia (Metzl Reference Metzl2010).

Investigating the shifting understandings of psychopathy, ASPD, and their ascribed psychic and somatic correlates and causes contributes to a more contextualized appreciation of the place and role of biomarkers within mental health. My account highlights the continuities of current research foci on biomarkers within older trajectories, whilst also admitting the distinctiveness of twenty-first-century approaches. Any novelty, however, is itself a function of the heterogeneity that has so long been evident within psychiatry, enabling individual clinicians and researchers to take as their focus different elements constitutive of the biopsychosocial matrix (and, in the process, further disunifying the profession).

The Social Life of Biomarkers and the Anarchy of Science

The potential biological correlates, and perhaps causes, of psychopathy and ASPD have structured debates about these conditions in key ways. Indeed, biomarkers have come to play an especially important role within medicine and the life sciences in general. Used in screening for disease, as targets in pharmaceutical research, and as indicators of exposure to environmental hazards, the idiom of biomarkers can be found across the discursive terrain of science and medicine internationally (Metzler Reference Metzler2010).

A variety of scholars from the social sciences have advanced critical commentary on the ascendance of biomarkers; as Waldby argues, their use to configure individual disease risk “simultaneously expand[s] the potential market for therapeutics while clearly delineating the geographical and demographic borders of that market” (Waldby Reference Waldby2009, 262). More generally, the use of biomarkers constitutes a variety of “ethical tension zones” (Caux et al. Reference Caux, Roy, Gilbert and Viau2007). In psychiatry, there may be particular issues associated with the labelling of individuals as “risky,” which could contribute to the further stigmatization of and use of pharmaceuticals by children and adolescents (Singh and Rose Reference Singh and Rose2009).

Of course, not everyone shares these concerns. As well as holding promissory value for researchers in the life and medical sciences, social scientists too have called for more work that brings together measurements of biomarkers with sociological analysis to produce what Timmermans and Haas (Reference Timmermans and Haas2008) refer to as a “sociology of disease.” Yet, despite the ways in which biomarkers, like other diagnostic “technologies,” have come to be seen by many as a means of offering “a more ‘objective’ approach to diagnosis than reliance on the vagaries of symptom reporting” (Armstrong et al. Reference Armstrong, Lilford, Ogden and Wesseley2007, 581), their use may in fact further complicate the multifaceted nature of illness. As anthropologist Margaret Lock and colleagues (Reference Lock, Freeman, Sharples and Lloyd2006) discuss for the case of Alzheimer's disease, research findings about biomarkers “create uncertainties and destabilization” (Lock et al. Reference Lock, Freeman, Sharples and Lloyd2006, 285), raising profound questions about the ontology of disorder; if pathological indicators may even be present when an individual seems healthy, what, then, is the disease?

Such questions are associated with a long and distinguished philosophical tradition (Canguilhem 1991), and it is not my intention to either summarize or answer them here. Eschewing a normative response to ambiguity within medicine, I instead take a descriptive approach, examining the ways in which disorders of antisociality have been constituted and located within psychiatric theory, research, and practice over the last sixty years. In so doing, I draw in part on the epistemology of Paul Feyerabend (Reference Feyerabend2010, Reference Feyerabend2011). In spite of the normativity of Feyerabend's work, and its focus on ways of knowing, rather than ways of standardizing or defining ontologies, it holds insights for the sociological history I present here. Feyerabend's philosophy is itself historical and empirically grounded; his epistemological positions and proscriptions are based on a careful analysis of the development of the natural sciences. Of particular interest here is his thesis that rationalism is an insufficient theory of knowledge production and circulation. Rather, Feyerabend argues that anarchism has been and needs to be an essential aspect of science; a view he advances with force and feeling:

Unanimity of opinion may be fitting for a rigid church, for the frightened or greedy victims of some (ancient, or modern) myth, or for the weak and willing followers of some tyrant. Variety of opinion is necessary for objective knowledge. And a method that encourages variety is also the only method that is compatible with a humanitarian outlook. (Feyerabend Reference Feyerabend2010, 25, emphasis in original)

These are strong words indeed, and – to be clear – it is not my aim here to suggest that they should necessarily be heeded by psychiatrists or anyone else. However, Feyerabend's thesis of methodological anarchism and the inherent disunity of science – which has also been addressed by philosophers such as John Dupré (Reference Dupré1983) – can also be read as a descriptive statement of how science evolves. It is this empirical claim that I wish to develop. For psychiatry, a kind of ontological anarchy is apparent (at least, for the specific case I examine herein). Through this concept, I intend to capture the diverse theories and practices that have attempted to clarify the murky pathological image that has so long been characteristic of antisociality. Indeed, I suggest that it is at least in part precisely through this diversity that a stable referent for terms such as “psychopathy” or “antisocial personality disorder” has so far escaped the sophisticated attempts of psychiatrists to constitute it. In other words: the variety of theories and experiments associated with pathological antisociality creates a range of parallel trajectories along which research might progress, increasing the breadth of perspectives available to mental health professionals from which to articulate the ontology of conditions like ASPD and psychopathy. This, perhaps paradoxically, makes it more difficult for a working consensus on the nature of these disorders to emerge and from which widely-agreed upon claims about development, identification and treatment can be made. As a consequence, further research and conceptual work is propelled. Ontological anarchy is thus – to a degree – autopoietic: it is a response to the uncertainties inherent to dealing with antisociality in a psychiatric context, as well as an engine powering the generation of yet more ambiguity.

In what follows, I chart some of the uncertainties associated with psychopathy from the origins of the concept, before moving to examine a range of biological understandings of antisociality debated in mid-twentieth-century Britain.Footnote 4 I then discuss how these sat alongside more psychosocial conceptions, and the ways that they were often integrated. Moving forward, into the late twentieth century, I document the great extent to which much earlier uncertainties remained, and the hopes of new technologies of standardization to clarify these ambiguities. In the new millennium, however, doubts and frustrations continue to be apparent, and, as we will see, even the promissory practices of neuroscience and molecular genetics have, so far, failed to provide consensus on the ontology of antisociality.

British Psychopaths

Since at least the 1950s, psychopathy in the UK has been recognized as a severe personality disorder characterized in individuals who have wantonly and repeatedly committed severely antisocial acts (see Berrios Reference Berrios1996; Werlinder Reference Werlinder1978). Historian Henry Werlinder (Reference Werlinder1978) has largely attributed this conception to the influence of David K. Henderson on UK psychiatric thought in the second quarter of the twentieth century (see also, Fox Reference Fox1964; Saß and Herpertz Reference Saß, Herpertz, Berrios and Porter1995; Saß and Felthous Reference Saß, Felthous, Felthous and Saß2008; Shorter Reference Shorter2005). Henderson, a leading Edinburgh psychiatrist and author of Psychopathic States (1939), was influenced by German and American psychiatric traditions, both of which were evident in his highly influential Text-book of Psychiatry, co-authored with Robert D. Gillespie and first published in 1927 (Gelder Reference Gelder, Berrios and Freeman1991; Hilton Reference Hilton2005; Werlinder Reference Werlinder1978). Henderson took psychopathy to be a constitutional inferiority of personality, expressed through criminal behavior (Gelder Reference Gelder, Berrios and Freeman1991; Werlinder Reference Werlinder1978). To assert that psychopathy was constitutional was not, however, to say that it was necessarily an inherited condition, or even biologically determined: the notion of constitutional factors emphasized biology, certainly, but also a contingent interaction between biology and environment (Berrios Reference Berrios1996; Rosenberg Reference Rosenberg2007; Werlinder Reference Werlinder1978).

Werlinder (Reference Werlinder1978) has argued that Henderson's psychopathy was an ambiguous construct: exactly what caused the condition was unclear. Time did little to clarify matters; in the 1950s, when a new Mental Health Act for England and Wales was being developed, psychopathy remained conceptually vague – as commentators like leading social scientist and magistrate Barbara Wootton (Reference Wootton1959) were acutely aware. Critical of the use of the term psychopathy, Wootton wryly observed that one definition of the disorder given by the Royal Medico-Psychological Association (the forerunner of the Royal College of Psychiatrists) essentially reduced to the following statement: “psychopaths are extremely selfish persons and nobody knows what makes them so” (Wootton Reference Wootton1959, 249). Resonating with US debates about psychopathy (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2010), Wootton's doubts about the legitimacy of the concept were closely related to its ambiguity. As she noted: “The volume of the literature on the subject of psychopaths is rivaled only by the depth of confusion in which this literature is steeped” (Wootton Reference Wootton1959, 250).

Prominent clinicians remarked similarly. Aubrey Lewis, widely regarded as one of the most important figures in twentieth-century psychiatry, observed: “Every text-book of psychiatry discusses this abnormality, but almost always ambiguously because the authors do not make clear why it should be regarded as an illness” (Lewis Reference Lewis1953, 119). This was a direct consequence of the dominant means of recognizing psychopathy; essentially, persistent antisocial conduct. This raised a particular problem: that of the individual who is “sociopathic without being psychopathic” (ibid., 116) – a dilemma especially pertinent in medico-legal discussions, within which the ambiguous “quantitative abnormality of impulse” that could be associated with psychopathy was regularly enrolled and “so often urged as a medical explanation for lessened responsibility in crimes of violence” (ibid., 119). Accordingly, “it would seem, then, that until the category is further defined and shown to be characterized by specified abnormality of psychological functions, it will not be possible to consider those who fall within it to be unhealthy, however deviant their social behavior” (ibid.).

As long as the concept of psychopathy has had a place within British psychiatry, then, it has been characterized by uncertainty and confusion. Lewis’ own solution, like those of many psychiatrists over the last 60 years, was to seek to destabilize the diagnostic category through the reflexive production of uncertainty regarding the ontology of its referent, only to appeal to new knowledge practices in order to promote re-reification in the future (cf. Moreira et al. Reference Moreira, May and Bond2009). In essence, only by rendering problematic the taken-for-granted could psychiatric innovation and clinical justification be assured.

The Mark of the Psychopath

In the 1950s, British psychiatry was, in terms of the conceptualization and treatment of mental disorder, a “broad church” (Ingleby Reference Ingleby, Gijtraswijt-Hofstra and Porter1998, 300). Nevertheless, many psychiatrists tended to emphasize its somatic aspects (Micale Reference Micale, Cooter and Pickstone2000), and pharmacological treatments such as chlorpromazine were widely used and associated with shifts in patient care away from the asylums.Footnote 5 Psychopathy (and personality more generally) was not exempt from being understood through a biological framework (Moncrieff and Crawford Reference Moncrieff and Crawford2001; Parker et al. Reference Parker, Georgaca, Harper, McLaughlin and Stowell-Smith1995), and aspects of an individual's soma, including body size and physique, were thought to vary with antisociality and aggressiveness (e.g., Gibbens Reference Gibbens1957; Rees Reference Rees1950a, b, c). The U.S. psychologist William H. Sheldon, whose work focused on “somatotyping” (i.e., linking body-type to personality), was especially influential (Rafter Reference Rafter2007).

In particular, high-profile researchers such as Hans J. Eysenck highlighted and popularized the importance of heredity in determining an individual's personality (Eysenck and Prell Reference Eysenck and Prell1951). Eysenck was a psychologist at the London-based Institute of Psychiatry (IoP), a prestigious center of international excellence in psychiatric research, home to a number of opinion-leaders. A prominent critic of psychoanalysis, Eysenck judged this tradition to have a poor scientific basis (Baruch and Treacher Reference Baruch and Treacher1978; e.g., Eysenck Reference Eysenck1953). His views on personality and its pathologies thus stood in stark contrast to the psychoanalytic model(s) through which many U.S. psychiatrists understood the development of mental disorder (Sclare Reference Sclare1953).Footnote 6

The influence of Eysenck's personality psychology was felt strongly within mid-twentieth-century British psychiatry and psychology (Rafter Reference Rafter2006), and it informed much research into psychopathy. He contributed significantly to the quantitative study of personality using statistical techniques (Buchanan Reference Buchanan2010; Crown Reference Crown1983); to Eysenck, personality was calculable, knowable through tools such as his Maudsley Personality Inventory (a commonly used means of scoring personality traits).Footnote 7 As we will see, such technologies helped to foster the conditions of possibility for the creation and employment of late-twentieth-century personality inventories such as the Hare Psychopathy Checklist.

Eysenck was widely cited by articles in the Journal of Mental Science (JMS; forerunner of the British Journal of Psychiatry), and somatic ideas about psychopathy permeated this periodical. Contributor Alexander Kennedy, for instance, considered psychopathic personality disorder to be “usually” constitutional (Kennedy Reference Kennedy1954, 873), “inborn or due to failure of post-natal development or maturation” (ibid., 876). Kennedy thought that “it is possibly by means of comparatively small structural damage, or by interfering at critical phases of development, to produce an individual who lacks the physical means of being morally aware” (ibid., 879; emphasis in original). Underscoring the ambiguity of the category, Kennedy's psychopaths were a heterogeneous group. Under the rubric of psychopathy came “moral-oligophrenics, antisocial perverts, anarchists, vengeful paranoids, the angry and explosively violent who constantly offend against the law” (ibid., 878). These diverse individuals seemed united only by their acute and essential antisociality. Kennedy thought that psychiatrists might “tame” a beastly psychopath through environmental manipulation, but his “savage heart” would be ultimately unaffected (ibid., 873). His account was thus evocative of Henderson's inter-war constitutional formulation of psychopathy. Not seemingly genetically determinist, Kennedy nevertheless considered that it would be a considerable challenge getting a psychopathic “leopard” to “change his spots” (ibid.).

In line with these essentialist ideas, explicitly cerebral understandings of psychopathy were also apparent in mid-twentieth-century psychiatric discourse. For instance, electroencephalography (EEG), a popular tool for the measurement of the electrical activity of the brain and routine diagnostic aid for neurological conditions such as epilepsy (Borck Reference Borck2008), was investigated as a means of enhancing the reliability of, and decreasing the uncertainty associated with, the diagnosis of psychopathy (Werlinder Reference Werlinder1978).Footnote 8 The hopes and expectations regarding the clinical utility of EEG resonate with more recent anticipatory claims regarding late-twentieth-century neurotechnologies (Borck Reference Borck2008). The ontological imaginaries embedded within the practices of electroencephalography were instantiated in the clinic by the taking of EEG recordings from individuals labeled as psychopathic. These results were used to show how the pattern of brain electrical activity was different between psychopathic and non-psychopathic subjects, implying a cerebral abnormality in the former (D. Hill Reference Hill1956; Gibbens et al. Reference Gibbens, Pond and Stafford-Clark1959).

The common use of EEG underscores the confidence of many psychiatrists that psychopathy was a biological disorder with a clearly defined signature in the brain. Indeed, some viewed leucotomy (a surgical operation on the brain) as an effective means of reducing aggressive behavior in psychopaths (Thorpe and Hardman Reference Thorpe and Hardman1952). However, these endeavors attracted some debate. For instance, on 9 October 1951, Desmond Curran, in his Presidential Address to the Section of Psychiatry at The Royal Society of Medicine, voiced concern that abnormal EEG patterns could be used to give leniency to criminals with no sign of mental disorder (Curran Reference Curran1952). Again, such discussions foreshadow twenty-first-century attempts to employ neuroimaging tools to add empirical flesh to the bones of philosophical discussions about responsibility (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2011a).

Nonetheless, in spite of the efforts of various investigators, scientific research around psychopathy undertaken in the 1950s and 60s was not found to translate easily into clinically useful knowledge – EEG readings did not map easily onto nosological entities (Craft et al. Reference Craft, Fabisch, Stephenson, Burnard and Kerridge1962, 581). Indeed, research detailing disappointing results about the lack of diagnostic value of EEG could be seen more generally in psychiatry at this time (e.g., Turton Reference Turton1958). If EEG was, in theory, a sensible means of rendering the ontology of mental disorder more transparent, in practice its application had the effect of making psychopathology yet more unclear: “The empirical approach to EEG data has produced, not increasing simplification, but increasing complexity and even certain strong points of clinical application have had to be abandoned” (D. Hill Reference Hill1956, 265).

The cerebral conceptions of psychopathy that the use of EEG implies at once recall nineteenth-century ideas about the neurologic origins of mental disorder (Rosenberg Reference Rosenberg2007), whilst also setting the scene for more recent attempts to identify cerebral markers of antisociality (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2009). As we will see, the obfuscation of an already ambiguous construct that was performed through the use of this technology likewise has important parallels with later research.

Disarranging Ontologies

In spite of the somatic slant of much discourse and practice regarding psychopathy in Britain in the 1950s and 60s, some psychiatrists nevertheless recognized a role for the environment in the development of this disorder. As one commentator argued in the JMS: “The grossest psychopaths appear where heredity and environment have conspired jointly to produce deprivation or arrest of the moral and emotional development, a chronic emotional malnutrition as it were over years” (Sands Reference Sands1953, 124). Clearly, whilst biological accounts of antisociality were common, other ways of thinking about the origins of psychopathy were possible.

That developmental narratives drawing upon environmental and psychic themes were considered legitimate to advance was in no small part due to the long tradition of “social psychiatry.” Although the dominant theoretical underpinnings and clinical practices associated with this tradition have shifted over time (Neve Reference Neve2004), the various formulations of social psychiatry all imply a particular ontology of mental illness that emphasizes the important role of “the environment” in the development and expression of psychiatric disorder.Footnote 9 In one clinician's words, “mental illness is to some extent a disease of civilization” (E. Hare Reference Hare1952, 579). Accordingly, “a programme of social psychiatry – the prevention and treatment of mental disease by social measures – must be based” on statistical research into two key questions: “how much of mental disease is caused by the conditions of civilization, and what particular factors are most responsible for causing it?” (ibid.). Environmental and somatic narratives thus played out together in mid-twentieth-century psychiatric discourse. Indeed, some individuals made active efforts to integrate these discourses; Aubrey Lewis, for instance, was especially inspired by the “psychobiology” of Swiss-born, U.S.-based psychiatrist Adolf Meyer.Footnote 10 Such conceptualizations reflected – and furthered – the theoretical and practical eclecticism that Baruch and Treacher (Reference Baruch and Treacher1978) have argued to be longstanding within British psychiatry.

As indicated above, the heterogeneous understandings of psychiatric disorder in general engendered similarly varied accounts of specific conditions. Thus, the aetiological narratives of psychopathy that appeared within journals like the JMS were wide-ranging (Fox Reference Fox1964). Factors such as economic and emotional deprivation as a result of absent parents were implicated in the development of personality disorder and antisocial behavior more broadly (e.g. Brown and Epps Reference Brown and Epps1966; Frommer et al. Reference Frommer, Mendelson and Reid1972; Koller Reference Koller1971; Scott Reference Scott1977; Tennent et al. Reference Tennent, McQuaid, Loughnane and Hands1971; Wolkind Reference Wolkind1974). These sat alongside more organic conceptions throughout the 1960s and 1970s (Nielson Reference Nielson1971), during which time wider debates were being played out regarding the links between an individual's genetics (particularly, the XYY genotype) and aggressiveness.Footnote 11 Correlations between physique and personality type continued to be explored (Borgaonkar et al Reference Borgaonkar, Unger, Moore and Crofton1972; Nielson and Tsuboi Reference Nielson and Tsuboi1970), as were longstanding associations between violence and epilepsy (see Gunn Reference Gunn1974; Gunn and Bonn Reference Gunn and Bonn1971; Hills Reference Hills1971; Kristianson Reference Kristianson1974).Footnote 12

Like the integrationist psychiatry of key figures like Aubrey Lewis, some clinicians and investigators at the coalface of practice and research acted as what might be called “ontological bricoleurs,” molding together different developmental accounts into a complex conceptual framework. For instance, Michael Craft, a consultant psychiatrist at Balderton Hospital in Nottinghamshire, published articles both on EEG analysis of individuals detained within a “Psychopathic Unit” (Craft et al. Reference Craft, Fabisch, Stephenson, Burnard and Kerridge1962), and on the relationship between childhood adversity and personality disorder (Craft et al. Reference Craft, Stephenson and Granger1964). In the latter case, he argued that the psychological and social structural effects of parental absence contributed to the development of psychopathology. The discourse and practice of British mental health professionals complicated historiographical distinctions between “biological” and “psychological” styles of psychiatry (cf. Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2010, Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2011b; Sadowsky Reference Sadowsky2005).Footnote 13

In the mid-twentieth century, aetiological narratives of both psychopathy and antisociality generally were nothing if not diverse. They ranged widely between the ontological realms of psyche, soma and society and the various topologies constitutive of them (e.g., parental absence, dominant mothers; genetics, neurology; childhood abuse, economic deprivation). As such, there was no stable consensus regarding the development of psychopathy; no specific markers – be they biological or otherwise – could be concretely pinned down and agreed upon. Rather, to paraphrase Feyerabend, in the aetiological discourses of psychiatrists, it was almost a case of “anything goes” (Feyerabend Reference Feyerabend2010, 8) – at least, within the broad toolkit of pathological determinants generally available to British psychiatrists to build legitimate models of psychopathy.

In Search of Standards

Disease classifications and diagnostic standards are central features of contemporary biomedicine, ordering knowledge production and helping to delimit uncertainty (Bowker and Star Reference Bowker and Star1999). Alongside the ambiguity regarding the causes of psychopathy were key questions relating to how the disorder should be diagnosed. Answers were not readily forthcoming: since there was “no generally agreed system of classification” (Pritchard and Graham Reference Pritchard and Graham1966, 605), diagnostic practices varied “greatly from one doctor to another” (ibid.).Footnote 14 Particularly problematic was the tendency of psychiatrists to classify antisocial personality most commonly in individuals with “a history of repeated criminal offences or violent and aggressive behaviour” (ibid., 606). This practice made it harder to establish firm boundaries between individuals with a psychiatric disorder, and those who were “merely” extremely antisocial. There was agreement at least that psychopathy was socially, as well as clinically, problematic: “a community problem, not only to themselves but to their family doctors, social workers, and others who become involved in their affairs and treatment” (Craft Reference Craft1968, 813). However, a consensus regarding diagnostic practice was far from imminent.

Little had changed by the 1970s; as one contributor to the BJP noted:

Although the term ‘psychopath’ continues to be used widely in clinical practice and research, doubts are frequently expressed as to whether the concept has a substantive referent. As the history of the concept indicates, it has gradually come to be used to identify a class of antisocial individuals by reference to psychological abnormality, but difficulties have arisen since the necessary and sufficient conditions of membership in the class have not been made explicit. (Blackburn Reference Blackburn1975, 456)

Psychopathy was still commonly considered, as famously stated by Sir Aubrey Lewis, a “most elusive category” (Lewis Reference Lewis1974). However, this was not just a problem for psychiatrists and other health professionals. Psychopathy also occupied an important position within mental health law, and therefore presented a challenge for policymakers and lawyers. Indeed, a key 1975 report on mentally disordered offenders (the Butler Report) recommended the category be abandoned altogether and be replaced with the broader and somewhat less controversial term, “personality disorder” (McCallum Reference McCallum2001; Wootton Reference Wootton1980). Nevertheless, the use of the psychopathy construct persisted; as criminologist Philip Bean complained, “Years of [clinical and legal] deliberation about the psychopathic patient appear to have led nowhere” (Bean Reference Bean1979, 102).

The “perennial problem” (ibid.) of psychopathy endured into the 1980s, and mental health practitioners and researchers continued to find its ambiguity troubling (Grounds Reference Grounds1987). In 1981, psychologists William Davies and Philip Feldman noted that “while there are many signs used to justify such a label there is no clear agreement on which, if any, are crucial ones” (W. Davies and Feldman Reference Davies and Feldman1981, 329). Accordingly, the validity of the psychopathy concept was “increasingly questioned, and its use little examined, in spite of the fact that when a person is diagnosed as a psychopath by a forensic specialist it can have very severe consequences for him” (ibid.). Davies and Feldman conducted a study examining how psychiatrists used the term psychopathy in practice. They found that though it was commonly deemed useful, clinicians “recognized a large number of diagnostic signs, though with much disagreement about the exact importance of each” (ibid.). Thus, if the ontology of psychopathy remained uncertain, the means of identifying individuals with the disorder was perhaps even more so.

Towards Standardization

In the 1980s, significant changes were taking place within British psychiatry, bringing with them a new climate of diagnostic certitude. As with the U.S., institutional shifts were made towards a more openly “biological” psychiatry (Busfield Reference Busfield, Gijtraswijt-Hofstra and Porter1998; Pilgrim Reference Pilgrim2002). Such a somatic emphasis within the psychiatric imaginary matched the biomedical framework of the third editions of the American Psychiatric Association (APA) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III), released in 1980, which facilitated the uptake of the nosology throughout the world.Footnote 15 As historian and psychiatrist David Healy (Reference Healy2002) has shown, the endorsement of the DSM by pharmaceutical companies contributed to the speedy translation of U.S. frameworks for diagnosis into British discourse, and it did not take unduly long for the text to be used in the training of U.K. psychiatrists (Macaskill et al. Reference Macaskill, Geddes and Macaskill1991). At the same time as the DSM-III was released, debates were being held regarding what would become of the 1983 Mental Health Act of England and Wales. With its precise descriptions of psychiatric disorder, the DSM-III was, it seems, alluring to U.K. clinicians in search of specific and legitimate standards for contested mental disorders.

One such “American” diagnostic entity was “antisocial personality disorder” (ASPD).Footnote 16 A clinically-useful shorthand for the pathological antisociality that the psychopathy construct had long been used to try and capture, ASPD proved appealing to many psychiatrists. In marked contrast to pychopathy, explicit criteria and guidelines for the recognition and diagnosis of ASPD were set out within the DSM, and the subsequent development of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III personality Disorders (SCID-II) helped to make the disorder “visible.”Footnote 17 ASPD could therefore be standardized, ensuring that the DSM-III was the “right tool for the job” (Clarke and Fujimura Reference Clarke and Fujimura1992) of generating stable biomedical knowledge regarding antisociality. “Modern medicine,” historian Ilana Löwy has pointed out, is “a trans-national endeavour” (Löwy Reference Löwy2007, 466); with ASPD, British clinicians and investigators could also more firmly ensconce themselves within international research and debate on personality disorder.

The DSM did not stand alone, however. Alongside the new category of ASPD was the World Health Organization's 1978 International Classification of Diseases (ICD-9) in 1978, which contained a category of “dissocial personality disorder,” similar to ASPD. However, as noted earlier, the ICD appeared to have less traction within UK forensic research than the DSM-III.Footnote 18 More importantly still was a novel means of standardizing psychopathy. In 1980, Canadian psychologist Robert Hare released the first version of his influential Psychopathy Checklist (PCL): a tool for the characterization of psychopathy, based on the concept of the disorder advanced by Hervey Cleckley in mid-twentieth century North America (R. Hare Reference Hare1980). Hare's Checklist became important for research internationally, including (neuro)biological studies. The 1991 Revision (PCL-R) is today commonly regarded as the “gold standard” for scientific investigations into the disorder. Both Hare's and the APA's standards came to have significant traction within U.K. research, feeding into clinical discourse and leading to fresh debate regarding how these categories relate to each other – and, thus, how the ontology of pathological antisociality can best be bounded (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2011c).Footnote 19

Despite the uptake of the DSM personality disorders within the literature, some professionals – including various influential figures – contested the criteria and the ontological assumptions underpinning them. Psychiatrists influenced by theoretical traditions within personality psychology, for instance, recoiled against the categorical diagnostic within the DSM. These psychiatrists thought personality disorders lay “at the extreme of a multidimensional continuum” (Tyrer and Alexander Reference Tyrer and Alexander1979, 166) of (statistically) normal personality; the notion that there were discrete personality disorder types that individuals either were or were not was rejected. Others, such as IoP faculty members Glyn Lewis and Louis Appleby, argued that DSM definitions of personality disorders were vague, making them unreliable diagnostic categories. Lewis and Appleby even proposed that “the concept be abandoned” (Lewis and Appleby Reference Lewis and Appleby1988, 45).

However, whilst many held strong views on personality disorders, few appeared to feel that these diagnostics should be completely omitted from the psychiatric vernacular. As one response to Lewis and Appleby argued, “Only when it has been shown that the concept of personality disorder holds no validity should it be discarded” (Cook Reference Cook1988, 701). Indeed, it is precisely because many viewed personality disorder diagnostics to be administratively necessary and “clinically important” (Rutter Reference Rutter1987, 454) that such debate raged.

Thus, the DSM personality disorders might have been standards, but they were not, many felt, necessarily very good ones. ASPD was particularly contested. A “questionable diagnostic entity” (ibid., 452), it cast into sharp relief the tensions mental health professionals had long faced when adjudicating between normality and pathology. Of particular concern was the wide-range of individuals who might legitimately be diagnosed with ASPD: “both transitory delinquents and continuous antisocials” (Stone Reference Stone1993, 307) stood under the umbrella of this diagnostic category.Footnote 20 ASPD could thus apply to the merely criminal as well as the truly personality disordered, pathologizing “normal” social deviancy.

Such disquiet had long been associated with attempts to formulate antisociality within a medical rubric; as we have seen, Barbara Wootton (Reference Wootton1959) highlighted the problematic circular logic of psychopathy (i.e., extreme antisocial acts may be taken to be indicative of mental disorder, an assertion which is itself then validated by pointing to the unusually antisocial behavior of the individual labeled psychopathic).Footnote 21 The endurance of these concerns suggests the degree to which some psychiatrists were reflexive about their conceptualizations of pathology, whilst also underscoring how regularly and easily “philosophical” concerns about personality disorders associated with antisocial behavior were side-lined. Such “practical uncertainty work” (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2011c) was essential in enabling clinical and scientific professionals to go about the job of managing patients and undertaking research without being paralyzed by fundamental ontological questions.

If ASPD attracted similar kinds of critique as psychopathy had in the past, it was not, however, because psychiatrists necessarily took these categories to be “the same.” Rather, the relationship between ASPD and psychopathy was problematic. Though both classified antisociality, the degree to which they could be said to capture the same psychopathology was not self-evident. On the one hand, some considered ASPD and psychopathy to be markedly different entities. Psychopathy was viewed as a severe form of personality disorder associated with traits such as lack of empathy, whereas ASPD applied to individuals who were indisputably delinquent, but lacked the severe antisociality characteristic of psychopathy. On the other hand, as psychologist Ronald Blackburn (Reference Blackburn1988) argued, in practice, ASPD and psychopathy could be taken to be overlapping entities (see also, more recently, Cawthra and Gibb Reference Cawthra and Gibb1998).

Such uncertainties were not solely clinical concerns; they were also construed as a potential impediment to the advancement of scientific knowledge concerning personality disorder. Though at least some investigators (e.g., Virkunnen 1983) considered that the increased “accuracy” of the DSM diagnostic criteria for standardizing personality disorder had paved the way for better, more scientific studies, others thought the use of similar behavioral proxies (such as criminality) for both psychopathy and ASPD was concerning (see, for instance, Dolan Reference Dolan1994).

Boundaries between psychopathy and ASPD were thus not easily erected. Since a consensus about the nature of psychopathy had always escaped British psychiatry, definite boundaries were difficult, if not impossible, to secure. This in turn blurred the edges of ASPD, which the authors of the DSM had invested considerable energy in sharpening as much as possible (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill, Flear, Farrell, Hervey and Murphy2013a). That boundaries were deemed necessary to police at all, however, is telling: though ASPD (or even dissocial personality disorder) might simply have replaced psychopathy within the psychiatric idiom, the coexistence of these concepts draws our attention to the significant traction of the latter category in spite of its uncertain ontology.

Structuring Aetiology

A key set of developments within biomedicine in the last century was the proliferation and increased sophistication of molecular genetics techniques (Keller Reference Keller2000). In psychiatry, such approaches were popularized by influential figures like psychologist Robert Plomin (Plomin et al. Reference Plomin, DeFries and McClearn1980; Plomin Reference Plomin1986; Plomin et al. Reference Plomin, DeFries and Fulker1988; Plomin and McClearn Reference Plomin and McClearn1993; Plomin Reference Plomin1994), a professor of behavioral genetics at the IoP since 1994. Genetic motifs were evident in discourse on personality disorder; some British psychiatrists advanced genetic ideas and research in journals like the BJP (McGuffin and Thapar Reference McGuffin and Thapar1992), as did clinicians and researchers from abroad (Virkkunen Reference Virkkunen1983). Indeed, genetic perspectives on antisocial behavior and violence broadly seemed increasingly visible in the late twentieth century. In particular, the research of psychologist Avshalom Caspi (Professor of Personality Development, also at the IoP) investigating gene-environment interactions in violent individuals came to be especially influential (e.g. Caspi et al. Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig, Taylor and Poulton2002).Footnote 22 Genetic science was clearly an important ontological anchor for various kinds of interests in antisociality.

Alongside their interests in genetics, British psychiatrists – like their colleagues in the U.S. – were also turning ever more towards the neurosciences. The use of brain imaging technologies such as Positron Emission Tomography (PET) and Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) helped to constitute new forms of objectivity (Beaulieu Reference Beaulieu2001, Beaulieu Reference Beaulieu2002), and neuroimages had a broad valence in scientific, clinical and popular cultures (Dumit Reference Dumit2004; Joyce Reference Joyce2008). Psychiatrists increasingly drew on neurobiological explanations for the aetiology and expression of mental disorder, and interventions that acted directly on the brain through neurotransmitters were widely accepted and used (Healy Reference Healy2004; Micale Reference Micale, Cooter and Pickstone2000; Rose Reference Rose2007).

Neuroscience was construed as a powerful way through which the ontology of psychopathology could be rendered transparent; as an editorial in a BJP supplement entitled “Brain imaging in psychiatry” concluded:

The application of neuroimaging techniques in the study of psychiatric disorders has provided one of the most dramatic developments in biological psychiatry. There is little doubt that integrated, structural-functional techniques will lead to major advances in the understanding of the biology of psychiatric disorders, refining their classification and guiding management. (Abou-Saleh Reference Abou-Saleh1990, s9)

The brain was thus constructed as what Brown and Kraft (Reference Brown and Kraft2006) might call “promissory matter,” a bodily substance around which numerous anticipatory claims revolved. Key amongst these was that knowledge of the brain would improve understanding of psychiatric disorders and, consequently, enhance practice. Nevertheless, this was not a radical shift so much as an amplification of established neurologic hopes in British psychiatry. As explored earlier, EEG technologies had routinely been used in hospitals housing individuals characterized as psychopathic, and the practice continued into the late twentieth century. Psychologist Richard Bentall recalls: “When I worked in a High Security Hospital after qualifying as a clinical psychologist in the mid-1980s, EEGs were routinely taken from patients. The reports made by the electroencephalographer usually made vague reference to ‘possible’ and ‘non-specific’ abnormalities and were almost always useless for clinical purposes” (Bentall Reference Bentall2003, 161).

Papers drawing on EEG methodologies to investigate psychopathy were also still reported in the BJP; for example, Howard and colleagues documented in 1984 that individuals categorized under the Mental Health Act as psychopathic had significantly different EEG results from offenders not recognized as psychopaths (Howard et al. Reference Howard, Fenton and Fenwick1984; see also Shagrass et al. Reference Shagrass, Roemer, Straumanis and Josiassen1984).Footnote 23 At the same time, techniques such as PET and MRI began to be increasingly employed by researchers interested in the organic correlates of antisociality. U.S. psychiatric researchers Nora D. Volkow and Lawrence Tancredi (1987), for instance, reported a study in which they used PET to elucidate neural substrates of violent behavior. Although neuro-technologies and the physiological assumptions implicit within them have shifted over time, neurologic constructions of personality disorder have thus been a persistent feature of British discourse. Nevertheless, it seems clear that their use and influence have been gradually extended over the last 20–30 years.

However, just because researchers operated within a neurologic framework did not mean that the disorders they studied were reduced solely to the isolated brain. As Volkow and Tancredi concluded in their paper on the neurobiology of antisociality:

It is important to emphasize that we do not attribute the violent behaviour of these cases solely to brain abnormalities, but rather that the occurrence of the violent outbursts are facilitated by the cerebral dysfunction. Most of the violent behaviour seen in patients probably represents complex interactions between various neural systems, neurotransmitters, hormones, environmental stimuli, and learned responses. (Volkow and Tancredi Reference Volkow and Tancredi1987, 672)

Psychiatrist and clinical researcher Mairead Dolan (Reference Dolan1994) also sought to re-introduce a neuroscientific perspective to the understanding of psychopathy; she too considered that though there was “evidence of a genetic influence” (ibid., 152) in the development of the disorder, an individual's genetic profile did not necessarily predetermine pathological antisociality, or that this had an exclusively somatic aetiology: “It is unlikely that either congenital or acquired [neurobiological] deficits alone could account for the development of psychopathy, but they may be interacting with adverse environmental circumstances to predispose the individual to act in an antisocial way” (ibid.). Neuroscientific analyses of constructs such as ASPD and psychopathy therefore granted ontological primacy to biology, but nevertheless positioned “the environment” as playing a part in the development and expression of these disorders (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2009, Reference Pickersgill2011b; cf. Hedgecoe Reference Hedgecoe2001; Shostak Reference Shostak2003; Rose Reference Rose2007).

Such psychosocial sentiments on behalf of primarily “biological” researchers must, however, be read within the context of a backlash against “reductionism” that was occurring even as biomedical approaches to theory, research and practice within British psychiatry were in ascendancy. “Biological psychiatry” was alarming to many who favored more “holistic” approaches to understanding and intervening in psychiatric disorder. Psychiatrists averse to neuroscience interpreted neurobiological models of aetiology and psychopharmaceutical therapies as “reductionist,” arguing that by foregrounding biological aspects of aetiology, the importance of psychological and sociological factors would be lessened – an approach that would ultimately be detrimental to therapy. The U.S. psychiatrist Leon Eisenberg, for instance, remarked in his 1986 Eli Lilly Lecture before the Royal College of Psychiatrists:

The peril is that psychiatry may come to focus exclusively on the brain as an organ and to overlook the experience of the patient as a person. We have been for so long pilloried by our medical and surgical colleagues as witch-doctors and woolly-minded thinkers that it is tempting to seek professional respectability by adhering to a reductionistic model of mental disorder. We may trade the one-sidedness of the “brainless” psychiatry of the past for that of a “mindless” psychiatry of the future. (Eisenberg Reference Eisenberg1986, 500)Footnote 24

In his lecture, Eisenberg cast light on why many psychiatrists found biological approaches alluring (e.g., they imbued “respectability” on practitioners), whilst also illuminating the dangers of reducing patient distress to nothing more than dysfunctional soma. As others have noted, such critique is international in its nature (Beaulieu Reference Beaulieu2003; Leibing Reference Leibing2008), and as longstanding as its referent (Rosenberg Reference Rosenberg2007); certainly, evidence of “anti-reductionism” has appeared in a number of forms within the BJP over the last fifty years (e.g. Bannister Reference Bannister1972; Krauss Reference Krauss1972; Lake Reference Lake1972; Walton Reference Walton1966). That such sentiments are so readily apparent even within the pages of such an “establishment” journal underscores the sometimes uneasy co-existence of “techno-somatic” psychiatry (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2010) with other styles of thought and practice. “Biological” epistemologies and the ontologies they constituted continued to sit alongside other approaches that emphasized the importance of psyche and society (as also reflected upon by Ingleby Reference Ingleby, Gijtraswijt-Hofstra and Porter1998; Gittins Reference Gittins1998; Moncrieff and Crawford Reference Moncrieff and Crawford2001; Porter Reference Porter2002; Samson Reference Samson1995).

With this in mind, it is not surprising that some contributors to the BJP not only gave credence to psychosocial factors in primarily biological articles, but that others concerned themselves specifically with the environment. In 1983, for instance, Mark A. Stewart and C. Susan de Blois from the United States argued that sons learnt antisocial and aggressive behavior from their fathers. More common, however, was an orientation towards some form of biopsychosocial, model, where genetics, environment and psychology were all taken to be salient in the development of pathology (see, for example, Coid Reference Coid1999; McGuffin and Thapar Reference McGuffin and Thapar1992; Rutter Reference Rutter1987; Tyrer et al. Reference Tyrer, Casey and Ferguson1991). Yet, as prominent personality disorder commentator Jeremy Coid noted, much work remained to be done in order to more precisely illuminate the aetiology of these complex conditions: “Aetiology may be multifactorial, with personality disorder the outcome of multiple, constitutional and/or environmental factors, which are additive until they overcome a certain threshold, require a sufficient level of severity or most occur at a critical developmental phase during the lifespan” (Coid Reference Coid1999, 530). An uncertain aetiology might be clarifed though the analysis of developmental risk factors; entities which influenced the likelihood of developing personality disorder, and which were amenable to delineation and examination by investigators (e.g. Coid Reference Coid1999; Reitsma-Street et al. Reference Reitsma-Street, Offord and Finch1985). A family history of personality disorder was, for instance concluded by Coid (Reference Coid1999) to be a key risk factor. Nonetheless, how these risk factors mapped onto aetiology was not straightforward.

In the late twentieth century, British psychiatry was, it appears, moving further towards a biomedical orientation; as public spending on medical/biological research increased (Micale Reference Micale, Cooter and Pickstone2000), a techno-somatic ethic became ever more evident. Of course, it remains an open question as to whether mental health was actually becoming more “biological” in terms of its research and therapeutic emphases, or whether biological discourse was simply more visible and research in this area more expensive than in previous decades. However, as with the U.S., an ontological move had been gradually effected such that scientists, health professionals and publics increasingly seemed to view health and illness through a molecular lens (Kerr Reference Kerr2004; Novas and Rose Reference Novas and Rose2000), engendering an epistemological logic within psychiatry which dictated that psychopathology was best understood through genetic and neuroscientific technologies. Yet, even as biomedical ways of knowing and acting upon patients increased in prestige and prominence, other styles of thought and action that emphasized the role of psyche and society in the development and expression of psychopathology were visible within clinical discourse and practice. Thus, British psychiatry's tradition of theoretical and practical eclecticism (Baruch and Treacher Reference Baruch and Treacher1978) endured.

For personality disorder this ontological diversity was not without its limitations, for if aetiology was broad it was also unclear. Developmental mechanisms remained “extremely difficult to determine” (Tyrer et al. Reference Tyrer, Casey and Ferguson1991, 469), and the literature on the subject was regarded by many as “confusing and inconsistent” (Dolan Reference Dolan1994, 157). As the century closed, much work remained to be done to elucidate the precise pathways through which disorders of sociality developed (Coid Reference Coid1999), and a consensus around the aetiologies of ASPD and psychopathy remained lacking – as indeed was an immutable ontological imaginary in British psychiatry around which agreement might form.

Something Old, Something New

In the new millennium, the same old questions concerning the ambiguity of antisociality continued to plague psychiatrists. Such issues came to be regarded as especially problematic in light of the increased attention of the psychiatric profession to the management of the “risk” of the individuals and populations they treated and governed – itself a function of new attempts to revise the 1983 Mental Health Act of England and Wales (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill, Flear, Farrell, Hervey and Murphy2013a, Reference Pickersgill2013b). Risk thinking and actuarial ways of knowing had “become central” to psychiatry (Rose Reference Rose1998, 177), and indeed within biomedicine and the health professions more broadly. Such discourses and debates link closely with a larger public and policy emphasis on the regulation of antisocial behaviour (Millie Reference Millie2007), and a broader shift across various sectors of society towards a concern with “pre-crime” and the management of menacing potentialities (Zedner Reference Zedner2007).

Within psychiatry, talk of “dangerousness” did not revolve solely around adults; discussions of the children “at risk” of developing ASPD were also up for discussion (e.g. Hill Reference Hill2003), and the idiom of “callous unemotional traits” began to feature within frameworks that emphasized the potential childhood antecedents of psychopathy (and which were linked to particular genetic profiles; see, for instance, Viding et al. Reference Viding, Frick and Plomin2007). However, the methodological limitations that dominant standards built into research were once more raised; the DSM, for instance, was deemed to “lack specificity” (Hill Reference Hill2003, s13) – and hence limited the precision of research. In particular, critique was leveled at the DSM-IV-TR (the revised version of the fourth edition of the DSM) and its expansion of the category of ASPD further into the territory “traditionally” occupied by psychopathy (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2012).

The fresh emphasis on risk within psychiatry, and in society more broadly, was supported and furthered by controversial health and criminal justice policy developments which aimed at “Managing Dangerous People with Severe Personality Disorder” (Home Office and the Department of Health 1999).Footnote 25 These proposals – contested by mental health professionals and patients alike – sought to innovate new services aimed at both detaining and treating risky individuals who met the criteria for personality disorder. As Mairead Dolan and Michael Doyle put it in a review article for the BJP, “Risk prediction is once again high on the public, political and clinical agenda” (Dolan and Doyle Reference Dolan and Doyle2000, 303). Indeed, the management of risk itself became a key target for therapeutic work (e.g. Wong et al. Reference Wong, Gordon and Gu2007). Noting the different tools available to practitioners to predict risk – unaided clinical risk assessment, actuarial methods, and structured clinical judgment – Dolan and Doyle pointed to the increased use of the PCL-R, which was widely regarded as showing “reasonable predictive validity in determining future violence” (Dolan and Doyle Reference Dolan and Doyle2000, 304). This diagnostic standard was thus gradually transformed into a tool for the management of risk.

In so doing, the ontology of the disorder it sought to capture was further complicated, since psychopathy could now be considered not just a form of pathological antisociality per se, but also a criterion for estimating an individual's propensity to violence more broadly.Footnote 26 In effect, psychopathy no longer simply contained antisociality but was also contained within it in a complex synecdochical relationship that did nothing to dispel ontological uncertainty. Indeed, the ambiguity of psychopathy is today rendered even more acute, as researchers question not only the cross-cultural generalizability of the PCL-R (Cooke et al. Reference Cooke, Mitchie, Hart and Clark2005) but even whether antisociality (as measured by criminal behavior) truly comprises a core-feature of the disorder at all (Cooke et al. Reference Cooke, Mitchie and Skeem2007).

This lack of certainty is maintained alongside a further transformation in the political and social dimensions of antisociality. Today, individuals are not just “at risk” of developing conditions like ASPD, or deemed “risky” to others as a consequence; those living under the label of personality disorder are increasingly viewed as at risk of further complications following the advent of their mental ill-health: “excess mortality” and “high levels of psychiatric morbidity” have been recorded (Davies et al. Reference Davies, Clark, Hollin and Duggan2007). Such findings further power the normative impulse within psychiatry to attend to pathological antisociality, and broaden the remit of researchers to clarify the concept and shed light on its causal mechanism. Risks, and ontologies, multiply.

The “neurochemical” motif that Nikolas Rose (Reference Rose2007) has argued to be a major vocabulary through which to articulate and regulate subjectivity in the twenty-first century threaded through certain psychiatric debates on antisociality; the function of serotonin – perhaps the quintessential locus of neurochemical selfhood – was examined, for instance, by Dolan et al. (Reference Dolan, Anderson and Deakin2001) as a potential biomarker for impulsivity and aggression. Continuing longstanding debates within psychiatry (and elsewhere; Roberts Reference Roberts2006) about the role of hormones in personality, health, and illness, Dolan and colleagues also investigated (and confirmed) the correlation between plasma testosterone levels and aggressive acts. Yet, what is perhaps most strongly evident in recent psychiatric research into the biological aspects of antisociality is the recourse to what Elizabeth Fein (Reference Fein, Pickersgill and Keulen2011) might refer to as a “neurostructural” model of selfhood. Many of the investigations into psychopathy in particular that were published within the BJP – and which resulted in clinical, policy, and public traction – were studies into neurologic structures, rather than the chemicals that flow through them. For instance, in a BJP editorial, prominent psychologist James Blair opened his commentary with the following programmatic statement: “To understand a psychiatric disorder we need to know why the pathology causes the behavioural disturbance, the neural structures implicated in the pathology and the cause of the dysfunction in the neural structures” (Blair Reference Blair and James2003, 5).

For Blair, a key neural system implicated in psychopathy is the amygdala – understood to play a key role in the processing of emotion. For IoP psychiatrist Quinton Deeley and colleagues (Reference Deeley, Daly, Surguladze, Tunstall, Mezey, Beer, Ambikapathy, Robertson, Giampietro, Brammer, Clarke, Dowsett, Fahy, Phillips and Murphy2006), this hypothesized centrality of the amygdala to psychopathic personality disorder might partly explain their neuroscientific findings that showed a “reduced rather than increased visual cortical response to fearful faces in psychopathy” (Deeley et al. Reference Deeley, Daly, Surguladze, Tunstall, Mezey, Beer, Ambikapathy, Robertson, Giampietro, Brammer, Clarke, Dowsett, Fahy, Phillips and Murphy2006, 538). This is in spite of the fact that these investigators “did not find significant between-group differences in amygdala function” (ibid.). This, it was speculated, could have been due to a “small sample size” (ibid.). Regardless, such comments are indicative of the idea taking shape and gaining credence today in many corners of mental health praxis: namely, that psychopathy is a disorder of empathy and hence related to the amygdala.

But let us not take this growing understanding that psychopathy is related to the amygdala to be a universal consensus, nor that it has rendered concrete the fuzzy nature of the condition; the precise biomarkers articulated by researchers have not lead to a fully biologically-characterized topology of antisociality. In Blair's case, he notes that the “basic causes of the pathology remain unclear” and although the “noradrenergic system could be implicated,” exactly “why this system should be dysfunctional is, again, unknown” (Blair Reference Blair and James2003, 6).

Furthermore, while neurologic reflections and research on antisociality continue to be apparent within journals such as the BJP and in prominent meetings and conferences, more commonly, a broadly biopsychosocial narrative is evident in articles, editorials, and correspondence within journals, and, indeed, in practice (e.g., Coid Reference Coid2003; Duggan et al. Reference Duggan, Milton, Egan, McCarthy, Palmer and Lee2003; J. Hill Reference Hill2003; Hodgins Reference Hodgins2007; Rutter Reference Rutter2005). Discourse on ASPD and psychopathy can thus be seen as an instantiation of a broader holistic ethic within U.K. psychiatry: whilst neuroscientific and biological perspectives attract significant funding and prestige, advocates of more “patient-centered” approaches also occupy prominent positions (e.g. Bracken and Thomas Reference Bracken and Thomas2001). Often, psychiatrists attempt to integrate these perspectives, as Fonagy's (Reference Fonagy2003) “developmental understanding of violence” epitomizes. Indeed, even influential biologically inclined clinician-scientists such as Sir Michael Rutter and Peter McGuffin at the IoP have advocated models of mental disorder that account for the dynamic inter-relations of biology, psychology and the social environment (Rutter and McGuffin Reference Rutter and McGuffin2004). Acting as a discursive resource through which developmental understandings of psychopathology can be fashioned, it is this particular kind of biopsychosocial holism that seems to capture the aetiological understandings of perhaps the majority of researchers and clinicians. With it, an ethic of complexity disunifies psychiatry; research, theory, and therapeutic modalities pluralize; and as a consequence, understandings of the nature of antisociality multiply and thus render the ontology of disorders like psychopathy and ASPD yet more uncertain.

Discussion

In this paper, I have analyzed the history of British psychiatric discourse on personality disorders associated with antisocial behavior; specifically, psychopathy and ASPD. These are categories that have been relatively neglected by historians and sociologists, but which can have important implications for individuals living under such diagnostic labels. I have attended closely to the diversity of models of these disorders, and noted the enduring uncertainties associated with them – not least of which has been a lack of stable consensus regarding how psychopathy and ASPD develop. In so doing, I have brought into sharp relief the various kinds of biomarkers posited to correlate with or even explain these disorders (e.g. an XYY karyotype, amygdala dysfunction), and situated these within a broader account of the ontological terrain of psychiatry.

Psychopathy has long been a problematic category. First, the causes of the disorder were, and are, uncertain (by which I mean a firmly established and historically consistent aetiological model is lacking): accounts of the condition range widely between biological, psychological, and environmental “factors.” Second, there remains a lack of stable consensus regarding what the term psychopathy properly refers to: exactly who should and should not be considered a psychopath is not always clear. The 1980s saw considerable changes to understandings of antisociality, with two important tools for standardizing pathology developed: the DSM-III and the Hare Psychopathy Checklist. Providing a concrete definition around which personality disorder discourse might revolve, these standards contributed significantly to research and clinical debate. The DSM and PCL were not perfect standards, and debate continued regarding, for instance, their conceptual rigor. However, these concerns were largely set to one side; in processes of “practical uncertainty work” (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2011c; Moreira et al. Reference Moreira, May and Bond2009) researchers and clinicians engaged in therapeutic and investigative practices, whilst at the same time recognizing and lamenting the lack of clarity regarding the pathological image of personality disorder.

The DSM and PCL thus reframed ASPD and psychopathy as legitimate clinical categories and “do-able” research problems (Fujimura Reference Fujimura1987); they became the “right tools for the job” (Clarke and Fujimura Reference Clarke and Fujimura1992) of producing rigorous, “objective” knowledge. This was particularly important in light of the epistemological and ontological emphases evident within biomedicine in the late twentieth century. However, the profusion of research investigating antisociality in the 1980s and 1990s appeared to do little to generate a consensus around the ontology of ASPD and psychopathy; developmental accounts continued to be wide-ranging. Such diversity reflected the broad focus of British psychiatry more generally, even as so-called “biological” psychiatry predominated.

Today, the ontologies of ASPD and psychopathy remain unresolved. Some assert that (neuro)scientific research into brain-based biomarkers holds promise in clarifying some of the ambiguities associated with these disorders. However, at present, neuroscientific knowledge appears to be making little difference to practice and neurologic claims may be ignored if they counter clinical goals and assumptions (Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2012). As I have sought to show, psychiatry exists in a state of what I refer to as “ontological anarchy” (cf. Feyerabend Reference Feyerabend2010, Reference Feyerabend2011), characterized by an ethic of complexity and constituted through a plurality of perspectives on antisociality as a consequence of the wide range of biological, psychological, and sociological “markers” posited to relate in some way to this “condition.”

At the same time, individual psychiatrists have long been what we might call “ontological bricoleurs,” assembling frameworks for understanding antisociality by piecing together different knowledges concerning psyche, soma, and society. Rather than create a consensus about the aetiology of psychopathy and ASPD, biomarker research might, therefore, render these disorders yet more problematic, increasing the intellectual resources from which clinicians might draw to articulate their nature. By consistently failing to fully specify psychopathy, psychiatry comes to be further disunified as new knowledge is produced and circulated – hence widening the scope of the research that may contribute to a more concrete delineation of the category. Whilst collective uncertainty is an important driver of biomedical innovation (Moreira et al. Reference Moreira, May and Bond2009), we can also see how biomarker research into antisociality might be regarded as exemplifying the limits of technoscience in resolving clinical challenges and concerns; it makes “the pathological image more complex rather than clarifying it” (Löwy and Gaudillière Reference Löwy and Gaudillière2008, 318).

This analysis, then, also offers up possibilities for thinking about the role of biomarkers more broadly. In particular, it suggests how proposed somatic and psychosocial indicators (such as high levels of theta wave activity, or low serotonergic function) come to play a role in constituting the very pathologies with which they are associated. New research programs, clinical routines, policy guidelines, and ethical regimes evolve together and are energized through discourse and debate on conditions like psychopathy and ASPD and their markers. In this sense, the practices of mental health professionals are, in part, governed through the material-semiotic entity that the (proposed) biomarker “is.” This further complicates attempts to clarify the ontology of psychiatric disorder. Uncertainty endures.

Acknowledgments

This work was generously sponsored by the ESRC and the Wellcome Trust. It has benefited greatly from the insights of the referees, members of the Edinburgh Sociology of Health and Illness Study Group, Alison Kraft, Paul Martin, and Kevin Howells. Earlier versions were presented at the Brocher Foundation and the University of Vienna.

Footnotes

1 In this paper, I use the term “antisociality” to capture diverse forms of behavior and temperament that can be considered to be in some way antisocial. Later in the analysis I will refer to “pathological antisociality”; by this, I mean forms of antisociality that are deemed by mental health professionals to be representative of some kind of personality (or other psychiatric) disorder, such as psychopathy.

2 In this paper I do not discuss “dissocial personality disorder,” the World Health Organization framing of pathological antisociality, since this term has had minimal purchase within the literature surveyed. A search of the online archives of the British Journal of Psychiatry reveals the following instances of the different diagnostic categories: Psychopathy, 654; antisocial personality disorder, 320; dissocial personality disorder, 9.

3 Bynum, Lock, and Porter (Reference Bynum, Lock and Porter1992) describe the role of key professional journals in the production of medical knowledge. In this paper, I focus primarily on one journal – the BJP – in order to define limits to my analysis. An advantage of the BJP as opposed to, for instance, Psychological Medicine, is its strongly British focus – especially evident in the editorials and correspondence pages which provide such rich material for the analyst to engage with (however, key articles from Psychological Medicine are cited herein, where they have had obvious impacts on UK debate and dialogue). My starting date of 1950 is a convenient locus for shifts in postwar psychiatry, and in particular enables the capturing of debates around antisociality that contributed to the writing of the 1959 Mental Health Act of England and Wales.

4 By “uncertainty,” I mean a lack of stable, working consensus about the ontology of psychopathy/ASPD that can be regarded as widely agreed upon by mental health professionals and researchers.

5 A range of scholars have discussed this point; for example, Baruch and Treacher (Reference Baruch and Treacher1978); Bennett (Reference Bennett, Berrios and Freeman1991); Busfield (Reference Busfield, Gijtraswijt-Hofstra and Porter1998); Ewins (Reference Ewins1976); Healy (Reference Healy1997); K. Jones (Reference Jones, Berrios and Freeman1991); Moncrieff and Crawford (Reference Moncrieff and Crawford2001); Porter (Reference Porter2002); Tansey (Reference Tansey, Gijtraswijt-Hofstra and Porter1998). However, the degree to which there is a direct, causal relationship between the new drugs and decreased asylum care is unclear (Rogers and Pilgrim Reference Rogers and Pilgrim2001).

6 More generally, postwar British psychiatry largely tended to eschew the insights of psychoanalytic theory (Gelder Reference Gelder, Berrios and Freeman1991; E. Jones Reference Jones2004; Moncrieff and Crawford Reference Moncrieff and Crawford2001; Porter Reference Porter2002). However, as Thomson (Reference Thomson2006) has shown, Freudian thought nevertheless had a broad valence within popular culture.

7 The U.S. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (the MMPI), which focussed explicitly on the characterization of abnormal personality, was also popular.

8 As Hayward (Reference Hayward2001, 625) has pointed out, Grey Walters – a pioneer of EEG – conducted electroencephalogic studies in the 1940s of criminals considered psychopathic.

9 Social psychiatry can thus be thought of as belonging to the tradition of holism within biomedicine that Rosenberg (Reference Rosenberg2007) has characterized as present in various forms throughout the twentieth century.

10 For more on the interactive models of influential figures like Lewis, see Gelder (Reference Gelder, Berrios and Freeman1991); Hilton (Reference Hilton2005); McGuffin and Plomin (Reference McGuffin and Plomin2004); Pilgrim (Reference Pilgrim2002); Rutter and McGuffin (Reference Rutter and McGuffin2004); Shorter (Reference Shorter2005).

11 As noted by Gaylin (Reference Gaylin1980) and Rainer et al. (Reference Rainer, Abdullah and Jarvik1972). For examples, see Bartholemew (Reference Bartholemew1972); Cowen and Mullen (Reference Cowen and Mullen1979); Eysenck and Eysenck (Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1972); Griffiths (Reference Griffiths1971); Griffiths et al. (Reference Griffiths, Marks, Fry, Morley and Lewis1972); Nielson and Tsuboi (Reference Nielson and Tsuboi1969).

12 As Temkin (Reference Temkin1994) remarks, beliefs in a link between violence and epilepsy are very old.

13 For work on holism in biomedicine more broadly, see Rosenberg (Reference Rosenberg2007).

14 This was not a problem solely restricted to psychopathy (Copeland et al. Reference Copeland, Cooper, Kendell and Gourlay1971).

15 Conversely, the authority of the DSM-III was less recognized in countries like Argentina and France where the recourse to biology in psychiatry was less evident (Lakoff Reference Lakoff2005; Lloyd Reference Lloyd2006).

16 For more detailed accounts of the emergence of ASPD, see Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2012; Gurley Reference Gurley2009.

17 However, from the research conducted for this analysis, the extent to which the SCID-II was actually employed within the clinic is unclear. This is a complex empirical question that merits further attention.

18 The term dissocial personality disorder is, anecdotally, regularly used in practice in Scotland, although much public discourse employs the DSM category ASPD (and especially, as noted earlier, the clinical research literature). This raises historical questions about what we mean by, and how we apprehend, the ‘use’ of a diagnostic construct.

19 Journalist Jon Ronson (Reference Ronson2011) has also examined the PCL-R.

20 Stone also highlighted the cultural relativity of antisociality, another common complaint about the personality disorders. For a related historical critique of these classifications’ cultural insensitivities, see Ernst (Reference Ernst, Berrios and Porter1995).

21 This critique remains current; see, for instance, Pilgrim (Reference Pilgrim2001).

22 This kind of research attracted attention from both lawyers (Andrews Reference Andrews, Carson and Rothstein1999; Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Mehlman and Rothstein2006) and sociologists (Dingwall et al. Reference Dingwall, Nerlich and Hillyard2003; Rose Reference Rose2007).

23 There had, however, been debate about the utility of taking routine EEG readings for antisocial individuals detained in hospitals since at least the 1970s; see, for example, Driver et al. (Reference Driver, West and Faulk1974).

24 The lecture, titled “Mindlessness and Brainlessness in Psychiatry” was reprinted in the BJP. This theme was revisited over a decade later in Eisenberg (Reference Eisenberg2000).

25 For sociological commentary, see Manning Reference Manning2006; Pickersgill Reference Pickersgill2013b; Pilgrim Reference Pilgrim2001, Reference Pilgrim2007; and Rose Reference Rose2010.

26 For wider discussion on risk in British psychiatry, see Rose (Reference Rose1998, Reference Rose2010).

References

Abou-Saleh, Mohammad T. 1990. “Editorial Preface.” British Journal of Psychiatry 157 (suppl. 9):79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, Lori B. 1999. “Predicting and Punishing Antisocial Acts: How the Criminal Justice System Might Use Behavioural Genetics.” In Behavioral Genetics: The Clash of Culture and Biology, edited by Carson, Ronald A. and Rothstein, Mark A., 116156. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Andrews, Lori B., Mehlman, Maxwell J., and Rothstein, Mark A., eds. 2006. Genetics: Ethics, Law and Policy. St. Paul: Thomson West.Google Scholar
Armstrong, David, Lilford, Richard, Ogden, Jane, and Wesseley, Simon. 2007. “Health-Related Quality of Life and the Transformation of Symptoms.” Sociology of Health & Illness 29 (4):570583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bannister, D. 1972. “Do Mental Events Exist?British Journal of Psychiatry 120 (557):474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartholemew, Allen A. 1972. “Prisoners of XYY Constitution.” British Journal of Psychiatry 120 (559):695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baruch, Geoff, and Treacher, Andrew. 1978. Psychiatry Observed. London, Henley and Boston: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Bean, Philip. 1979. “The Mental Health Act 1959: Rethinking an Old Problem.” British Journal of Law and Society 6 (1):99108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaulieu, Anne. 2001. “Voxels in the Brain: Neuroscience, Informatics and Changing Notions of Objectivity.” Social Studies of Science 31 (5):435480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaulieu, Anne. 2002. “Images are Not the (only) Truth: Iconoclasm, Visual Knowledge and Experimentation in Brain Mapping.” Science, Technology & Human Values 21 (1):5386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaulieu, Anne. 2003. “Brains, Maps, and the New Territory of Psychology.” Theory and Psychology 13 (4):561568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, Douglas. 1991. “The Drive Towards the Community.” In 150 Years of British Psychiatry, 1841–1991, edited by Berrios, German E. and Freeman, Hugh, 321332. London: Gaskell.Google Scholar
Benning, T. B. 2003. “Neuroimaging Psychopathy: Lessons from Lombroso.” British Journal of Psychiatry 183 (6):563564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, Richard P. 2003. Madness Explained: Psychosis and Human Nature. Bury St. Edmunds: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Berrios, German E. 1996. The History of Mental Symptoms: Descriptive Psychopathology since the Nineteenth Century. Cambridge, New York and Melbourne: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackburn, R. 1975. “An Empirical Classification of Psychopathic Personality.” British Journal of Psychiatry 127 (5):456560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackburn, R. 1988. “On Moral Judgements and Personality Disorders: The Myth of Psychopathic Personality Revisited.” British Journal of Psychiatry 153 (4):505512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R., James, R. 2003. “Neurobiological Basis of Psychopathy.” British Journal of Psychiatry 182 (1):57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borck, Cornelius. 2008. “Recording the Brain at Work: The Visible, the Invisible, and the Readable in Electroencephalography.” Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 17 (3):367379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borgaonkar, D. S., Unger, W. M., Moore, S. M., and Crofton, T. A.. 1972. “47, XYY Syndrome, Height and Institutionalization.” British Journal of Psychiatry 120 (558):549550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowker, Geoffrey C., and Star, Susan L.. 1999. Sorting Things Out: Classification and Its Consequences. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bracken, Patrick, and Thomas, Philip. 2001. “Postpsychiatry: A New Direction for Mental Health.” BMJ 322 (7288):724727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, Rebekah, Heim, Amy Kegley, and Westen, Drew. 2005. “Transference Patterns in the Psychotherapy of Personality Disorders: Empirical Investigation.” British Journal of Psychiatry 186 (4):342349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, Felix, and Epps, Phyllis. 1966. “Childhood Bereavement and Subsequent Crime.” British Journal of Psychiatry 112 (491):10431048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, Nik. 2003. “Hope against Hype – Accountability in Biopasts, Presents and Futures.” Science Studies 16 (2):321.Google Scholar
Brown, Nik, and Kraft, Alison. 2006. “Blood Ties: Banking the Stem Cell Promise.” Technology Analysis and Strategic Management 18 (3/4):313327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, Roderick D. 2010. Playing with Fire: The Controversial Career of Hans J. Eysenck. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bullmore, Ed, and Fletcher, Paul. 2003. “The Eye's Mind: Brain Mapping and Psychiatry.” British Journal of Psychiatry 182 (5):381384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busfield, Joan. 1998. “Restructuring Mental Health Services in Twentieth-Century Britain.” In Cultures of Psychiatry and Mental Health Care in Postwar Britain and the Netherlands, edited by Gijtraswijt-Hofstra, Marijke and Porter, Roy, 928. Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bynum, William F., Lock, Stephen, and Porter, Roy. 1992. Medical Journals and Medical Knowledge: Historical Essays. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Calton, T. 2004. “Personality in Psychiatry: What Thin Partitions?British Journal of Psychiatry 184 (2):183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canguilhem, Georges. [1943] 1991. The Normal and the Pathological. New York: Zone Books.Google Scholar
Caspi, Avshalom, McClay, Joseph, Moffitt, Terrie E., Mill, Jonathan, Martin, Judy, Craig, Ian W., Taylor, Alan, and Poulton, Richie. 2002. “Role of Genotype in the Cycle of Violence of Maltreated Children.” Science 297 (5582):851854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, Raymond B. 1965. The Scientific Analysis of Personality. Baltimore: Penguin.Google Scholar
Caux, Chantal, Roy, David J., Gilbert, Louise, and Viau, Claude. 2007. “Anticipating Ethical Aspects of the Use of Biomarkers in the Workplace: A Tool for Stakeholders.” Social Science & Medicine 65 (2):344354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cawthra, Richard, and Gibb, Robert. 1998. “Severe Personality Disorder – Whose Responsibility?British Journal of Psychiatry 173 (1):810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, Adele, and Fujimura, Joan, eds. 1992. The Right Tools for the Job. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, Simon. 2008. “Petty Cash and the Neuroscientific Mapping of Pleasure.” BioSocieties 3 (2):151163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coid, Jeremy. 1999. “Aetiological Risk Factors for Personality Disorders.” British Journal of Psychiatry 174 (6):530538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coid, Jeremy. 2003. “Epidemiology, Public Health and the Problem of Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 182 (suppl. 44):s3–s10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, C. C. H. 1988. “Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 153 (5):700701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooke, David J., Mitchie, Christine, Hart, Stephen D., and Clark, Danny. 2005. “Assessing Psychopathy in the UK: Concerns about Cross-Cultural Generalizability.” British Journal of Psychiatry 186 (4):335341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooke, David J., Mitchie, Christine, and Skeem, Jennifer. 2007. “Understanding the Structure of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised.” British Journal of Psychiatry 190 (suppl. 49):s39–s50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copeland, J. R. M., Cooper, J. E., Kendell, R. E., and Gourlay, A. J.. 1971. “Difference in Usage of Diagnostic Labels amongst Psychiatrists in the British Isles.” British Journal of Psychiatry 118 (547):629640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, P. and Mullen, P. E.. 1979. “An XYY Man.” British Journal of Psychiatry 135 (1):7881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craft, Michael. 1968. “Psychopathic Disorder: A Second Trial of Treatment.” British Journal of Psychiatry 114 (512):813820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craft, Michael, Fabisch, Walter, Stephenson, Geoffrey, Burnard, Gordon, and Kerridge, David. 1962. “100 Admissions to a Psychopath Unit.” Journal of Mental Science 108 (456):564583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craft, Michael, Stephenson, Geoffrey, and Granger, Clive. 1964. “The Relationship between Severity of Personality Disorder and Certain Adverse Childhood Influences.” British Journal of Psychiatry 110 (466):392396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crown, S. 1983. “H. J. Eysenck: Dimensions of Personality.” British Journal of Psychiatry 142 (1):9597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cullen, Joe, and Cohn, Simon. 2006. “Making Sense of Mediated Information: Empowerment and Dependency.” In New Technologies in Health Care: Challenge, Change and Innovation, edited by Webster, Andrew, 112130. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curran, Desmond. 1952. “‘Psychiatry Ltd.’.” Journal of Mental Science 98 (412):373381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, Steffan, Clark, Martin, Hollin, Clive, and Duggan, Connor. 2007. “Long-term Outcomes after Discharge from Medium Secure Care: A Cause for Concern.” British Journal of Psychiatry 191 (1):7074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, William, and Feldman, Philip. 1981. “The Diagnosis of Psychopathy by Forensic Specialists.” British Journal of Psychiatry 138 (4):329331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deeley, Quinton, Daly, Ellen, Surguladze, Simon, Tunstall, Nigel, Mezey, Gill, Beer, Dominic, Ambikapathy, Anita, Robertson, Dene, Giampietro, Vincent, Brammer, Michael J., Clarke, Amory, Dowsett, John, Fahy, Tom, Phillips, Mary L., and Murphy, Declan G.. 2006. “Facial Emotion Processing in Criminal Psychopathy: Preliminary Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study.” British Journal of Psychiatry 189 (6):533539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dingwall, Robert, Nerlich, Brigitte, and Hillyard, Samantha. 2003. “Biological Determinism and Symbolic Interaction: Hereditary Streams and Cultural Roads.” Symbolic Interaction 26 (4):631644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, Mairead. 1994. “Psychopathy – a Neurobiological Perspective.” British Journal of Psychiatry 165 (2):151159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolan, Mairead, and Doyle, Michael. 2000. “Violence Risk Prediction: Clinical and Actuarial Measures and the Role of the Psychopathy Checklist.” British Journal of Psychiatry 177 (4):303311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolan, Mairead, Anderson, I. M., and Deakin, J. F. W.. 2001. “Relationship between 5-HT Function and Impulsivity and Aggression in Male Offenders.” British Journal of Psychiatry 178 (4):352359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Driver, M. V., West, L. R., and Faulk, M.. 1974. “Clinical and EEG Studies of Prisoners Charged with Murder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 125 (6):583587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duggan, Connor, Milton, John, Egan, Vince, McCarthy, Lucy, Palmer, Ben, and Lee, Alan. 2003. “Theories of General Personality and Mental Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 182 (suppl. 44):s19–s23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumit, Joseph. 2004. Picturing Personhood: Brain Scans and Biomedical Identity. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupré, John. 1983. “The Disunity of Science.” Mind 92 (367):321346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrenberg, Alain. 2010. The Weariness of the Self: Diagnosing the History of Depression in the Contemporary Age. Montreal and Kingston: McGill-Queen's University Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, Leon. 1986. “Mindlessness and Brainlessness in Psychiatry.” British Journal of Psychiatry 148 (5):497508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, Leon. 2000. “Is Psychiatry more Mindful or Brainier than It Was a Decade Ago?British Journal of Psychiatry 176 (1):15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, Waltraud. 1995. “Personality Disorders: Social Section.” In A History of Clinical Psychiatry: The Origin and History of Psychiatric Disorders, edited by Berrios, German E. and Porter, Roy, 645655. London and New Brunswick: Athlone Press.Google Scholar
Ewins, David. 1976. “The Butler Report.” British Journal of Law and Society 3 (1):101109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, Hans J. 1953. Uses and Abuses of Psychoanalysis. Aylesbury and Slough: Penguin.Google Scholar
Eysenck, Hans J., and Eysenck, S. B. G.. 1972. “Prisoners of XYY Constitution.” British Journal of Psychiatry 120 (554):124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, Hans J., and Prell, D. B.. 1951. “The Inheritance of Neuroticism: An Experimental Study.” Journal of Mental Science 97 (408):441465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fein, Elizabeth. 2011. “Innocent Machines: Asperger's Syndrome and the Neurostructural Self.” In Sociological Reflections on the Neurosciences, edited by Pickersgill, Martyn D. and Keulen, Ira van, 2750. Bingley: Emerald.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feyerabend, Paul. [1975] 2010. Against Method, new edition. London: Verso Books.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, Paul. 2011. The Tyranny of Science. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Fonagy, Peter. 2003. “Towards a Developmental Understanding of Violence.” British Journal of Psychiatry 183 (3):190192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, Richard. 1964. “Sin, Crime and the Psychopath.” Modern Law Review 27 (2):190198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, Mariam. 2003. “Material Theory: Duration and the Serotonin Hypothesis of Depression.” Theory, Culture and Society 20 (5):126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frommer, Eva A., Mendelson, Wallace B., and Reid, M. A.. 1972. “Differential Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disturbance in Pre-School Children.” British Journal of Psychiatry 121 (560):7174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujimura, Joan. 1987. “Constructing ‘Do-able’ Problems in Cancer Research: Articulating Alignment.” Social Studies of Science 17 (2):257293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabbard, Glen O. 2000. “A Neurobiologically Informed Perspective on Psychotherapy.” British Journal of Psychiatry 177 (2):117122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaylin, Willard. 1980. “The XYY Controversy: Researching Violence and Genetics.” Hastings Center Report 10 (4):12.Google Scholar
Gelder, Michael. 1991. “Adolf Meyer and His Influence on British Psychiatry.” In 150 Years of British Psychiatry, 1841–1991, edited by Berrios, German E. and Freeman, Hugh, 419435. London: Gaskell.Google Scholar
Gibbens, T. C. N. 1957. “The Sexual Behaviour of Young Criminals.” Journal of Mental Science 103 (432):527540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbens, T. C. N., Pond, D.A., and Stafford-Clark, D.. 1959. “A Follow-up Study of Criminal Psychopaths.” Journal of Mental Science 105 (438):108115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gittins, Diana. 1998. Madness in Its Place: Narratives of Severalls Hospital, 1913–1997. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gremillion, Helen. 2003. Feeding Anorexia: Gender and Power at a Treatment Center. Durham and London: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, A. W. 1971. “Prisoners of XYY Constitution: Psychological Aspects.” British Journal of Psychiatry 119 (549):193194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, A. W., Marks, V., Fry, D., Morley, G., and Lewis, Gillian. 1972. “Prisoners of XYY Constitution: Biochemical Studies.” British Journal of Psychiatry 121 (563):365368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grounds, Adrian T. 1987. “Detention of ‘Psychopathic Disorder’ Patients in Special Hospitals.” British Journal of Psychiatry 151 (4):474478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunn, John. 1974. “Social Factors and Epileptics in Prison.” British Journal of Psychiatry 124 (583):509517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunn, John, and Bonn, John. 1971. “Criminality and Violence in Epileptic Prisoners.” British Journal of Psychiatry 118 (544):337343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurley, Jessica R. 2009. “A History of Changes to the Criminal Personality in the DSM.” History of Psychology 12 (4):285304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, E. H. 1952. “The Ecology of Mental Disease: A Dissertation of the Influence of Environmental Factors in the Distribution, Development and Variation of Mental Disease.” Journal of Mental Science 98 (413):579594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, Robert. 1980. “A Research Scale for the Assessment of Psychopathy in Criminal Populations.” Personality and Individual Differences 1 (2):111119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayward, Rhodri. 2001. “The Tortoise and the Love-Machine: Grey Walter and the Politics of Electroencephalography.” Science in Context 14 (4):615641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, David. 1997. The Antidepressant Era. Cambridge MA and London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Healy, David. 2002. The Creation of Psychopharmacology. Cambridge MA and London: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, David. 2004. “Shaping the Intimate: Influences on the Experience of Everyday Nerves.” Social Studies of Science 34 (2):219245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedgecoe, Adam. 2001. “Schizophrenia and the Narrative of Enlightened Geneticization.” Social Studies of Science 31 (6):875911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedgecoe, Adam, and Martin, Paul. 2003. “The Drugs Don't Work: Expectations and the Shaping of Pharmacogenetics.” Social Studies of Science 3 (3):327364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Denis. 1956. “Clinical Applications of EEG in Psychiatry.” Journal of Mental Science 102 (427):264271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, Jonathan. 2003. “Early Identification of Individuals at Risk for Antisocial Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 182 (suppl. 44):s11–s14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hills, N. F. 1971. “Criminality and Violence in Epileptic Prisoners.” British Journal of Psychiatry 119 (548):116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilton, Claire. 2005. “The Origins of Old Age Psychiatry in Britain in the 1940s.” History of Psychiatry 16 (3):267289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodgins, Sheilagh. 2007. “Persistent Violent Offending: What Do We Know?British Journal of Psychiatry 190 (suppl. 49):s12–s14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, Jeremy. 2001. “Ten Books.” British Journal of Psychiatry 179 (5):468471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Home Office and the Department of Health. 1999. Managing Dangerous People with Severe Personality Disorder. Proposals for Policy Development. London: Home Office. <http://www.dh.gov.uk/prod_consum_dh/groups/dh_digitalassets/documents/digitalasset/dh_120634.pdf> (last accessed 7 July 2012).+(last+accessed+7+July+2012).>Google Scholar
Howard, Richard C., Fenton, George W., and Fenwick, Peter C. C.. 1984. “The Contingent Negative Variation, Personality and Antisocial Behaviour.” British Journal of Psychiatry 144 (5):463474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingleby, David. 1998. “The View from the North Sea.” In Cultures of Psychiatry and Mental Health Care in Postwar Britain and the Netherlands, edited by Gijtraswijt-Hofstra, Marijke and Porter, Roy, 79102. Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi.Google Scholar
Jones, Edgar. 2004. “War and the Practice of Psychotherapy: The UK Experience 1939–1960.” Medical History 48 (4):493510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, Kathleen. 1991. “The Culture of the Mental Hospital.” In 150 Years of British Psychiatry, 1841–1991, edited by Berrios, German E. and Freeman, Hugh, 1728. London: Gaskell.Google Scholar
Joyce, Kelly A. 2008. Magnetic Appeal: MRI and the Myth of Transparency. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Keller, Evelyn F. 2000. The Century of the Gene. Cambridge MA and London: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendell, R. E. 1981. “The International Classification and the Diagnoses of English Psychiatrists 19681980.” British Journal of Psychiatry 139 (3):177180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, Alexander. 1954. “Psychopathic Personality and Social Responsibility.” Journal of Mental Science 100 (421):873881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, Anne. 2004. Genetics and Society: A Sociology of Disease. London and New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koller, K. M. 1971. “Parental Deprivation, Family Background and Female Delinquency.” British Journal of Psychiatry 118 (544):319327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraft, Alison. 2006. “Between Medicine and Industry: Medical Physics and the Rise of the Radioisotope 1945–65.” Contemporary British History 20 (1):135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, Stephen. 1972. “Do Mental Events Exist?British Journal of Psychiatry 120 (557):474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kristianson, Per. 1974. “The Personality in Psychomotor Epilepsy Compared with Explosive and Aggressive Personality.” British Journal of Psychiatry 125 (586):221229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lake, Brian. 1972. “Do Mental Events Exist?British Journal of Psychiatry 121 (560):119120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, Andrew. 2005. Pharmaceutical Reason: Knowledge and Value in Global Psychiatry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, Sing. 1999. “Diagnosis Postponed: Shenjing Shuairuo and the Transformation of Psychiatry in post-Mao China.” Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 23 (3):349380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leibing, Annette. 2008. “Entangled Matters – Alzheimer's, Interiority, and the ‘Unflattening’ of the World.” Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 32 (2):177193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, Aubrey. 1953. “Health as a Social Concept.” British Journal of Sociology 4 (2):109124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, Aubrey. 1974. “Psychopathic Personality: A Most Elusive Category.” Psychological Medicine 4 (2):133140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, Glynn, and Appleby, Louis. 1988. “Personality Disorder: The Patients Psychiatrists Dislike.” British Journal of Psychiatry 153 (1):4449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, Stephanie. 2006. “The Clinical Clash over Social Phobia: The Americanization of French Experiences?BioSocieties 1 (2):229250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lock, Margaret, Freeman, Julia, Sharples, Rosemary, and Lloyd, Stephanie. 2006. “When It Runs in the Family: Putting Susceptibility Genes in Perspective.” Public Understanding of Science 15 (3):277300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löwy, Ilana. 2007. “The Social History of Medicine: Beyond the Local.” Social History of Medicine 20 (3):465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löwy, Ilana, and Gaudillière, Jean-Paul. 2008. “Localizing the Global: Testing for Hereditary Risks of Breast Cancer.” Science, Technology, & Human Values 33 (3):299325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macaskill, N., Geddes, J., , J. and Macaskill, A.. 1991. “DSM-III in the Training of British Psychiatrists: A National Survey.” International Journal of Social Psychiatry 37 (3):182186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manning, Nick. 2006. “Dangerous and Severe Personality Disorder - An Essay.” Social Science & Medicine 63 (7):19601971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, Paul, Brown, Nik, and Kraft, Alison. 2008. “From Bedside to Bench? Communities of Promise, Translational Research and the Making of Blood Stem Cells.” Science as Culture 17 (1):2941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCallum, David. 2001. Personality and Dangerousness: Genealogies of Antisocial Personality Disorder. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuffin, Peter, and Plomin, Robert. 2004. “A Decade of the Social, Developmental and Genetic Psychiatry Centre at the Institute of Psychiatry.” British Journal of Psychiatry 185 (4):280282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuffin, Peter, and Thapar, Anita. 1992. “The Genetics of Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 160 (1):1223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzl, Jonathan. 2010. The Protest Psychosis: How Schizophrenia Became a Black Disease. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Metzler, Ingrid. 2010. “Biomarkers and Their Consequences for the Biomedical Profession: A Social Science Perspective.” Personalized Medicine 7 (4):407420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micale, Mark. 2000. “The Psychiatric Body.” In Medicine in the Twentieth Century, edited by Cooter, Roger and Pickstone, John V., 323346. Singapore: Harwood Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Millie, Andrew. 2007. “Looking for Anti-social Behaviour.” Policy and Politics 35 (4):611627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moncrieff, Joanna, and Crawford, Michael J.. 2001. “British Psychiatry in the 20th Century – Observations from a Psychiatric Journal.” Social Science & Medicine 53 (3):349356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, Paul, Leese, Morven, Lee, Tennyson, Walters, Paul, Thornicroft, Graham, and Mann, Anthony. 2003. “Standardised Assessment of Personality – Abbreviated Scale (SAPAS): Preliminary Validation of a Brief Screen for Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 183 (3):228232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreira, Tiago, May, Carl, and Bond, John. 2009. “Regulatory Objectivity in Action: Mild Cognitive Impairment and the Collective Production of Uncertainty.” Social Studies of Science 39 (5):665690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowbray, R. M. 1960. “The Concept of the Psychopath.” Journal of Mental Science 106 (443):537542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neve, Michael. 2004. “A Commentary on the History of Social Psychiatry and Psychotherapy in Twentieth-Century Germany, Holland and Great Britain.” Medical History 48 (4):407412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ni Bhriain, S., Clarke, A. W., and Lawlor, B. A.. 2005. “Neuroimaging: A Training Issue for Psychiatry?Psychiatric Bulletin 29 (5):189192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielson, Johannes. 1971. “Prevalence and a 2 ½ Years Incidence of Chromosome Abnormalities among All Males in a Forensic Psychiatric Clinic.” British Journal of Psychiatry 119 (552):503512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielson, Johannes, and Tsuboi, Takayuki. 1969. “Intelligence, EEG, Personality Deviation and Criminality in Patients with the XYY Syndrome.” British Journal of Psychiatry 115 (525):965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielson, Johannes, and Tsuboi, Takayuki. 1970. “Correlation between Stature, Character Disorder and Criminality.” British Journal of Psychiatry 116 (531):145150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novas, Carlos, and Rose, Nikolas. 2000. “Genetic Risk and the Birth of the Somatic Individual.” Economy and Society 29 (4):485513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, Jackie. 2006. Panic Diaries: A Genealogy of Panic Disorder. Durham and London: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Parker, Ian, Georgaca, Eugenie, Harper, David, McLaughlin, Terence, and Stowell-Smith, Mark. 1995. Deconstructing Psychopathology. London: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persaud, Raj. 2002. “Ten Books.” British Journal of Psychiatry 181 (3):258261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2009. “Between Soma and Society: Neuroscience and the Ontology of Psychopathy.” BioSocieties 4 (1):4560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2010. “From Psyche to Soma? Changing Accounts of Antisocial Personality Disorders in the American Journal Psychiatry.” History of Psychiatry 21 (3):294311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2011a. “Connecting Neuroscience and Law: Anticipatory Discourse and the Role of Sociotechnical Imaginaries.” New Genetics and Society 30 (1):2740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2011b. “‘Promising’ Therapies: Neuroscience, Clinical Practice, and the Treatment of Psychopathy.” Sociology of Health & Illness 33 (3):448464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2011c. “Ordering Disorder: Knowledge Production and Uncertainty in Neuroscience Research.” Science as Culture 20 (1):7187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2012. “Standardising Antisocial Personality Disorder: The Social Shaping of a Psychiatric Technology.” Sociology of Health & Illness 24 (4):544559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2013a. “Sociotechnical Innovation in Mental Health: Articulating Complexity.” In European Law and New Health Technologies, edited by Flear, Mark L., Farrell, Annemarie, Hervey, Tamara, and Murphy, Thérèse, 323342. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickersgill, Martyn. 2013b. “How Personality Became Treatable: The Mutual Constitution of Clinical Knowledge and Mental Health Law.” Social Studies of Science 43 (1):3053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilgrim, David. 2001. “Disordered Personalities and Disordered Concepts.” Journal of Mental Health 10 (3):253265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilgrim, David. 2007. “New ‘Mental Health’ Legislation for England and Wales: Some Aspects of Consensus and Conflict.” Journal of Social Policy 36 (1):7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilgrim, David. 2002. “The Biopsychosocial Model in Anglo-American Psychiatry: Past, Present and Future?Journal of Mental Health 11 (6):585594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, Robert. 1986. Development, Genetics, and Psychology. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Plomin, Robert. 1994. Genetics and Experience: The Interplay between Nature and Nurture. Newbury Park CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, Robert, DeFries, John C., and McClearn, Gerald E.. 1980. Behavioral Genetics: A Primer. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Plomin, Robert, DeFries, John C., and Fulker, David W.. 1988. Nature and Nurture in Infancy and Early Childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, Robert, and McClearn, Gerald E., eds. 1993. Nature, Nurture, and Psychology. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, Roy. 2002. Madness: A Brief History. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pritchard, Michael, and Graham, Philip. 1966. “An Investigation of a Group of Patients who Have Attended both the Child and Adult Departments of the Same Psychiatric Hospital.” British Journal of Psychiatry 112 (487):603612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rafter, Nicole H. 2006. “H. J. Eysenck in Fagin's Kitchen: The Return to Biological Theory in 20th-Century Criminology.” History of the Human Sciences 19 (4):3756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rafter, Nicole H. 2007. “Somatotyping, Antimodernism, and the Production of Criminological Knowledge.” Criminology 45 (4):805833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rainer, John D., Abdullah, Syed, and Jarvik, Lissy F.. 1972. “XYY Karyotype in a Pair of Monozygotic Twins: A 17-Year Life-history Study.” British Journal of Psychiatry 120 (558):543548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rees, Linford. 1950a. “Body Size, Personality and Neurosis.” Journal of Mental Science 96 (402):168180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rees, Linford. 1950b. “Body Build, Personality and Neurosis in Women.” Journal of Mental Science 96 (403):426–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, Linford.1950c. “A Factorial Study of Physical Constitution in Women.” Journal of Mental Science 96 (404):619632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitsma-Street, Marge, Offord, David R., and Finch, Terri. 1985. “Pairs of Same-Sexed Siblings Discordant for Antisocial Behaviour.” British Journal of Psychiatry 146 (4):415423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, Celia. 2006. “‘What Can I Do to Help Myself?’” Somatic Individuality and Contemporary Hormonal Bodies.” Science Studies 19 (2):5476.Google Scholar
Rogers, Anne and Pilgrim, David. 2001. Mental Heath Policy in Britain, 2nd ed. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronson, Jon. 2011. The Psychopath Test: A Journey Through the Madness Industry. London: Picador.Google Scholar
Rose, Nikolas. 1998. “Governing Risky Individuals: The Role of Psychiatry in New Regimes of Control.” Psychiatry, Psychology and Law 5 (2):177195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Nikolas. 2007. The Politics of Life Itself: Biomedicine, Power, and Subjectivity in the Twenty-First Century. Princeton and Woodstock: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Nikolas. 2010. “‘Screen and Intervene’: Governing Risky Brains.” History of the Human Sciences 23 (1):19105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, Charles. 2007. Our Present Complaint: American Medicine, Then and Now. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, Michael. 1987. “Temperament, Personality and Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 150 (4):443458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, Michael. 2005. “How the Environment Affects Mental Health.” British Journal of Psychiatry 186 (1):46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, Michael, and McGuffin, Peter. 2004. “The Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre: Its Origins, Conception and Initial Accomplishments.” Psychological Medicine 34 (5):933947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samson, Colin. 1995. “The Fracturing of Medical Dominance in British Psychiatry?Sociology of Health & Illness 17 (2):245268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sands, Dalton E. 1953. “A Special Hospital Unit for the Treatment of Psychosis and Neurosis in Juveniles.” Journal of Mental Science 99 (414):123129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadowsky, Jonathan. 2005. “Beyond the Metaphor of the Pendulum: Electroconvulsive Therapy, Psychoanalysis and the Styles of American Psychiatry.” Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences 61 (1):125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saß, Henning, and Felthous, Alan R.. 2008. “History and Conceptual Development of Psychopathic Disorders.” In The International Handbook of Psychopathic Disorders and the Law, edited by Felthous, Alan R. and Saß, Henning, 930. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Saß, Henning, and Herpertz, Sabine. 1995. “Personality Disorders: Clinical Section.” In A History of Clinical Psychiatry: The Origin and History of Psychiatric Disorders, edited by Berrios, German E. and Porter, Roy, 632644. London and New Brunswick: Athlone Press.Google Scholar
Sclare, A. Balfour. 1953. “The Psychiatric Patient in America.” Journal of Mental Science 99 (416):572579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, P. D. 1977. “Assessing Dangerousness in Criminals.” British Journal of Psychiatry 131 (2):127–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shagrass, Charles, Roemer, Richard A., Straumanis, John J., and Josiassen, Richard C.. 1984. “Psychiatric Diagnostic Discrimination with Combinations of Quantitative EEG Variables.” British Journal of Psychiatry 144 (6):581592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shorter, Edward. 2005. A Historical Dictionary of Psychiatry. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shostak, Sara. 2003. “Locating Gene-Environment Interaction: At the Intersections of Genetics and Public Health.” Social Science & Medicine 56 (11):23272342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, Chole. 2012. Understanding Autism: Parents, Doctors, and the History of a Disorder. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Singh, Ilina, and Rose, Nikolas. 2009. “Biomarkers in Psychiatry.” Nature 460 (7252):202207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S. 2001. “Personality Disorder: Agency and Responsibility.” British Journal of Psychiatry 179 (6):558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Star, Susan L. 1985. Scientific Work and Uncertainty. Social Studies of Science 15 (3):391427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Star, Susan L. 1986. “Triangulating Clinical and Basic Research: British Localizationists, 18701906.” History of Science 24 (1):2948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, Mark A., and de Blois, C. Susan. 1983. “Father-Son Resemblances in Aggressive and Antisocial Behaviour.” British Journal of Psychiatry 142 (1):7884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, Michael H. 1993. Long-term Outcome in Personality Disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry 162 (3):299313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tansey, E. M. 1998. “‘They Used to Call It Psychiatry’: Aspects of the Development and Impact of Psychopharmacology.” In Cultures of Psychiatry and Mental Health Care in Postwar Britain and the Netherlands, edited by Gijtraswijt-Hofstra, Marijke and Porter, Roy, 79102. Amsterdam: Editions Rodopi.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temkin, Owsei. 1994. The Falling Sickness: A History of Epilepsy from the Greeks to the Beginnings of Modern Neurology, 3rd ed. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Tennent, T. G., McQuaid, A., Loughnane, T., and Hands, A. J.. 1971. “Female Arsonists.” British Journal of Psychiatry 119 (552):497502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, Matthew. 2006. Psychological Subjects: Identity, Culture, and Health in Twentieth Century Britain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, F. T., and Hardman, James. 1952. “The Lower Quadrant Leucotomy.” Journal of Mental Science 98 (412):389400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmermans, Stefan, and Haas, Steven. 2008. “Towards a Sociology of Disease.” Sociology of Health & Illness 30 (5):659696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turney, Jon, and Turner, Jill. 2000. “Predictive Medicine, Genetics and Schizophrenia.” New Genetics and Society 19 (1):522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turton, E. C. 1958. “The EEG as a Diagnostic and Prognostic Aid in the Differentiation of Organic Disorders in Patients over 60.” Journal of Mental Science 104 (435):461465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrer, Peter, and Alexander, John. 1979. “Classification of Personality Disorders.” British Journal of Psychiatry 135 (2):163167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyrer, Peter, Casey, Patricia, and Ferguson, Brian. 1991. Personality Disorder in Perspective. British Journal of Psychiatry 159 (4):463471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyrer, Peter. 2001. “Personality Disorder.” British Journal of Psychiatry 179 (1):8184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, Essi, Frick, Paul J., and Plomin, Robert. 2007. “Aetiology of the Relationship between Callous-Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in Children.” British Journal of Psychiatry 190 (suppl. 49):s33–s38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Virkkunen, Maui. 1983. “Insulin Secretion during the Glucose Tolerance Test in Antisocial Personality.” British Journal of Psychiatry 142 (6):598604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, Nora D. and Tancredi, Lawrence. 1987. “Neural Substrates of Violent Behaviour: A Preliminary Study with Positron Emission Tomography.” British Journal of Psychiatry 151 (5):668673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldby, Catherine. 2009. “Biobanking in Singapore: Post-developmental State, Experimental Population.” New Genetics and Society 28 (3):253265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walton, H. J. 1966. “Differences between Physically-minded and Psychologically-minded Medical Practitioners.” British Journal of Psychiatry 122 (492):10971102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werlinder, Henry. 1978. Psychopathy: A History of the Concepts. Analysis of the Origin and Development of a Family of Concepts in Psychopathology. Stockholm: Almqvist and Wiksell International.Google Scholar
Wolkind, S. N. 1974. “Sex Differences in the Aetiology of Antisocial Disorders in Children in Long Term Residential Care.” British Journal of Psychiatry 125 (585):125130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, Stephen C. P., Gordon, Audrey, and Gu, Deqiang. 2007. “Assessment and Treatment of Violence-Prone Forensic Clients: An Integrated Approach.” British Journal of Psychiatry 190 (suppl. 49):s66–s74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wootton, Barbara. 1959. Social Science and Social Pathology. Woking and London: George Allen and Unwin Ltd.Google Scholar
Wootton, Barbara. 1980. “Psychiatry, Ethics, and the Criminal Law.” British Journal of Psychiatry 136 (6):525532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zedner, Lucia. 2007. “Pre-crime and Post-criminology?Theoretical Criminology 11 (2):261281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar