Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T14:22:14.350Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Social Brain and the Myth of Empathy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2012

Allan Young*
Affiliation:
McGill University E-mail: [email protected]

Argument

Neuroscience research has created multiple versions of the human brain. The “social brain” is one version and it is the subject of this paper. Most image-based research in the field of social neuroscience is task-driven: the brain is asked to respond to a cognitive (perceptual) stimulus. The tasks are derived from theories, operational models, and back-stories now circulating in social neuroscience. The social brain comes with a distinctive back-story, an evolutionary history organized around three, interconnected themes: mind-reading, empathy, and the emergence of self-consciousness. This paper focuses on how empathy has been incorporated into the social brain and redefined via parallel research streams, employing a shared, imaging technology. The concluding section describes how these developments can be understood as signaling the emergence of a new version of human nature and the unconscious. My argument is not that empathy in the social brain is a myth, but rather that it is served by a myth consonant with the canons of science.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Addis, Donna Rose, Wong, Alana, and Schacter, Daniel L.. 2007. “Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction.”Neuropsychologia 45 (7):13631377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bargh, John A. and Morsella, Ezequiel. 2008. “The unconscious mind.” Perspectives in Psychological Science 3:7379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkow, Jerome H., Cosmides, Leda, and Tooby, John, eds. 1992. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, Simon, Knickmeyer, Rebecca C., and Belmonte, Matthew K.. 2005. “Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism.” Science 310:819823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, Simon and Wheelwright, Sally. 2004. “The Empathy Quotient: an investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:163175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barton, Robert A. and Dunbar, Robin I.M.. 1997. “Evolution of the social brain.” In Machiavellian Intelligence II, edited by Whiten, Andrew W. and Byrne, Richard W., 240263. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berntsen, Dorthe and Hall, Nicolene M.. 2004. “The episodic nature of involuntary autobiographical memories.” Memory and Cognition 32:789803.Google ScholarPubMed
Blakeslee, Sandra. 2006. “Cells that read minds.” New York Times, 10 January.Google Scholar
Boden, Margaret A. 2006. Mind as Machine: A History of Cognitive Science, vol. 2. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, Robert, Gintis, Herbert, Bowles, Samuel, and Richerson, Peter J.. 2003. “The evolution of altruistic punishment.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 100:35313535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyer, Pascal. 2010. “Evolutionary economics of mental time travel?Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12:219224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brass, Marcel, Schmitt, Ruth M., Spengler, Stephanie, and Gergely, György. 2007. “Investigating action understanding: inferential processes versus action simulation.” Current Biology 17:21172121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, David. 2011a. “Social animal: How the new sciences of human nature can help make sense of a life.” New Yorker, 17 January.Google Scholar
Brooks, David. 2011b. The Social Animal: The Hidden Sources of Love, Character, and Achievement. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Brothers, Leslie. 1989. “A biological perspective on empathy.” American Journal of Psychiatry 146:1019.Google ScholarPubMed
Busby, Janie and Suddendorf, Thomas. 2005. “Recalling yesterday and predicting tomorrow.” Cognitive Development 20:362372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, Richard and Whiten, Andrew, eds. 1988. Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carr, Laurie, Iacoboni, Marco, Dubeau, Maria-Charlotte, Mazziotta, John C., and Lenzi, Gian Luigi. 2003. “Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100:54975502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, Michael C. 2009. “Mental time travel and the shaping of language.” Experimental Brain Research 192:553560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Custers, Ruud and Aarts, Henk. 2010. “The unconscious will: how the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness.” Science 320:4750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, Jean and Grèze, Julie. 2006. “The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior.” Brain Research 1079:414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Quervain, Dominique, Fischbacher, Urs, Treyer, Valerie, Schellhammer, Melanie, Schnyder, Ulrich, Buck, Alfred, and Fehr, Ernst. 2004. “The neural basis of altruistic punishment.” Science 305:12541258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Vignemont, Frederique and Singer, Tania. 2006. “The empathic brain: how, when and why?Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:435441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vignemont, Frederique and Haggard, Patrick. 2008. Action observation and execution: What is shared? Social Neuroscience 3:421433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
di Pellegrino, Giussepe, Luciano, Fadiga, Leonardo, Fogassi, Vittorio, Gallese, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 1992. “Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study.” Experimental Brain Research 91:176180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinstein, Ilan. 2008. “Human cortex: reflections of mirror neurons.” Current Biology 18:R956959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinstein, Ilan, Uri Hasson, Nava Rubin, and Heeger, David J.. 2007. “Brain areas selective for both observed and executed movements.” Journal of Neurophysiology 98:14151427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinstein, Ilan, Thomas, Cibu, Behrmann, Marlene, and Heeger, David J.. 2008. “A Mirror up to Nature.” Current Biology 18:R1318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, Robin I. M. 2003. “The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective.” Annual Review of Anthropology 32:163181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fadiga, Luciano and Craighero, Laila. 2004. “Electrophysiology of action representation.” Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 21:57169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fadiga, Luciano, Pavesi, Giovanni, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 1996. “Motor facilitation during observation: a magnetic simulation study.” Journal of Neurophysiology 73:26082611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, Ernst and Camerer, Colin F.. 2007. “Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences.” Trends in Cognitive Science 11:419427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fliessbach, Klaus, Weber, Bernd, Trautner, P., Dohmen, T., Sunde, U., Elger, C.E., and Falk, A.. 2007. “Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum.”Science 318:13051308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, Jerry. 1983. Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry. 2000. The Mind Doesn't Work that Way: The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogassi, Leonardo, Gallese, Vittorio, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2002. “Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons.” Science 297:846848.Google Scholar
Fogassi, Leonardo, Ferrari, Piere F., Gesierich, Benno, Rozzi, Stefano, Chersi, Fabian, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2005. “Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding.” Science 308:662667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, Karl. 2002. Beyond phrenology: What can neuroimaging tell us about distributed circuitry? Annual Review of Neuroscience 25:2212250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittorio. 2001. “The “shared manifold” hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy.” Journal of Consciousness Studies 8:3350.Google Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio. 2003a. “The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity.” Psychopathology 36:171180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittorio. 2003b. “A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 358:12311240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittorio and Goldman, Alvin. 1998. “Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading.” Trends in Cognitive Science 2:493501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittoriao, Keysers, Christian, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2004. “A unifying view of the basis of social cognition.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:396403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittorio, Eagle, Morris, and Migone, Paolo. 2007. “Intentional attunement: mirror neurons and the neural underpinnings of interpersonal relations.” Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 55:151176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, Csibra, and Gergely, György. 2007. “‘Obsessed with goals’: functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans.” Acta Psychologica 124:6078.Google Scholar
Gergely, György, Bekkering, Harold, and Kiraly, Ildiko. 2002. “Rational imitation in preverbal infants.” Nature 415:755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadjikhani, Nouchine, Joseph, Robert M., Snyder, Josh, and Tager-Flusberg, Helen. 2006. “Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism.” Cerebral Cortex 16:12761282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, William D. 1964. “The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I.” Journal of Theoretical Biology 7:116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassin, Ran R., Uleman, James S., and Bargh, John A., eds. 2005. The New Unconscious. New York: Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Heyes, Cecilia. 2010. “Mesmerizing mirror neurons.” Neuroimage 51:789791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickcok, Gregory. 2009. “Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans.” Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences 21:12291243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, Sandra B. 2009. Mothers and Others: the Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, Marco. 2009. “Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons.” Annual Review of Psychology 60:653670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, Marco. 2008. Mirroring People: The New Science of How We Connect with Others. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, Marco and Dapretto, Mirella. 2006. “The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7:942951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, Marco, Molnar-Szakacs, Istvan, Gallese, Vittorio, Buccino, Giovanni, Mazziotta, John C., and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2005. “Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system.” PLoS (Public Library of Science) Biology 3:e79.Google ScholarPubMed
Jacob, Pierre. 2008. “What do mirror neurons contribute to human social cognition?Mind and Language 23:190223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, Pierre and Jeannerod, Marc. 2005. “The motor theory of social cognition: a critique.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:2125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, Philip L., Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Decety, Jean. 2005. “How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy.” NeuroImage 24:771779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, William. 1890. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Jeannerod, Marc. 1994. “The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17:187245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Edward G. 2008. “Cortical maps and modern phrenology.” Brain 131:22272233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keysers, Christian and Gazzola, Valeria. 2010. “Mirror neurons reported in Humans.” Current Biology 20:R353354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keysers, Christian, Kaas, Jon H., and Gazzola, Valeria. 2010. “Somatosensation in social perception.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11:417428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilner, James M. and Frith, Chris D.. 2008. “Action observation: inferring intentions without mirror neurons.” Current Biology 18:R3233. 2008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knoch, Daria, Pascual-Leone, Alvaro, Meyer, Kaspar, Treyer, Valerie, and Fehr, Ernst. 2006. “Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex.” Science 314:829832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohler, Evelyne, Keysers, Christian, Umlitá, M. Alessandra, Fogassi, Leonardo, Gallese, Vittorio, and Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2002. “Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons.” Science 297:846848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosik, Kenneth S. 2003. “Beyond phrenology, at last.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4:234239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, Stephen M. 2005. “Mental images and the brain.” Cognitive Neuropsychology 22:333347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanzetta, John T. and Englis, Basil G.. 1989. “Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses.” Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes 56:543554.Google Scholar
Lawson, John, Baron-Cohen, Simon, and Wheelwright, Sally. 2004. “Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperger Syndrome.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:301310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, Paul. 2003. Hysterical Men: War, Psychiatry, and the Politics of Trauma in Germany, 1890–1930. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Libet, Benjamin. 1986. “Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of the unconscious will in voluntary action.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:529566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, Benjamin, Gleason, Curtis A., Wright, Elwood W., and Pearl, Dennis K.. 1983. “Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act.” Brain 102:623642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindner, Isabel, Echterhoff, Gerald, Davidson, Patrick S.R., and Brand, Matthias. 2010. “Observation inflation: your actions become mine.” Psychological Science 21:12911299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maynard Smith, John. 1979. “Game theory and the evolution of behaviour.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 205:475488.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, John. 2001. “Reconciling Marx and Darwin.” Evolution 55:14961498.Google Scholar
Mukamel, Roy, Eckstrom, Arne D., Kaplan, Jonah, Iacoboni, Marco, and Fried, Iizhak. 2010. “Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions.” Current Biology 20:17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, Martin A. and Sigmund, Karl. 2005. “Evolution of indirect reciprocity.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 437:12911298.Google ScholarPubMed
Nummenmaa, Lauri, Hirvonen, Jussi, Parkkola, Ritta., and Hietanen, Jari K.. 2008. “Is emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for affective and cognitive empathy.” Neuroimage 43:571580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberman, Lindsay M., Hubbard, Edward M., McCleery, Joseph P., Altschuler, Eric L., Ramachandran, Vilayanur S., and Pineda, Jaime A.. 2005. “EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders.” Cognitive Brain Research 24:190198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okuda, Jiro, Fujii, Toshikatsu, Ohtake, Hiroya, Tsukiura, Takashi, Tanji, Katsuyo, Suzuki, Kyoko, Kawashima, Ryuta, Fukuda, Hiroshi, Itoh, Matsatoshi, and Yamadori, Atsushi. 2003. “Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes.” NeuroImage 19:13691380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacamo and Arbib, Michael A.. 1998. “Language within our grasp.” Trends in Neuroscience 21:188194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacamo and Corrado, Greg S.. 2010. “The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11:264274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacomo and Craighero, Laila. 2004. “The mirror-neuron system.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 27:169192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacamo and Fabbri-Destro, Maddlena. 2010. “Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism.” Experimental Brain Research 200:223237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosas, Alejandro. 2008. “The return of reciprocity: a psychological approach to the evolution of cooperation.” Biology and Philosophy 23:555566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryle, Gilbert. 1949. The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Schacter, Daniel, Addis, Donna Rose, and Buckner, Randy L.. 2007. “Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8:657661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, Simone G., Fischer, Meytal, Dvash, Jonathan, Harari, Hagai, Perach-Bloom, Nufar, and Levkovitz, Yechiel. 2009. “Intranasal administration of oxytocin increases envy and schadenfreude (gloating).” Biological Psychiatry 66:864870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, Daniel L. 1999. “Physical imagery: kinematic versus dynamic models.” Cognitive Psychology 38:433464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shmuelof, Lior and Zohary, Ehud. 2007. “Watching others’ actions: mirror representations in the parietal cortex.” Neuroscientist 13:667672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, Tania. 2006. “The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of literature and implications for future research.” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30:855863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, Tania, Seymour, Ben, O'Dougherty, John P., Kaube, Holger, Dolan, Raymond J., and Frith, Chris D.. 2004. “Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain.” Science 303:1157–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, Tania, Seymour, Ben, O'Dougherty, John P., Stephan, Klaas E., Dolan, Raymond J., and Frith, Chris D.. 2006. “Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others.” Nature 439:466469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D. F. 2010. “Cognitive brain mapping for better or worse.” Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 53:321329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, Sean A. 2004. “The deceptive brain.” Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 97:69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, Sean A., Hunter, Mike D., and Harpin, Gemma. 2002. “Neuroscience and the will.” Current Opinion in Psychiatry 15:519526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strawson, Galen. 2008. Real Materialism and Other Essays. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, Thomas, Rose Addis, Donna, and Corballis, Michael C.. 2009. “Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364:13171324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takahashi, Hidehiko, Kato, Motochiro, Matsuura, Masato, Mobbs, Dean, Suhara, Tetsuya, and Okubo, Yoshiro. 2009. “When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude.” Science 323: 937939. Supporting Online Material, at www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/323/5916/937/DC1 (accessed 17 April 2012).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tambiah, Stanley J. 1990. Magic, Science, Religion, and the Scope of Rationality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tettamanti, Marco, Buccino, Giovanni, Succaman, Maria C., Gallese, Vittorio, Danna, Massimo, Scifo, Paola, Fazio, Ferruccio, Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Cappa, Stefano E., and Perani, Daniela. 2005. “Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17:2732781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, Robert L. 1971. “The evolution of reciprocal altruism.” Quarterly Review of Biology 46:3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turella, Luca, Pierno, Andrea C., Tubaldi, Frederico, and Castiello, Umberto. 2009. “Mirror neurons in humans: consisting or confounding evidence?Brain and Language 108:1021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Gaag, Christiaan, Minderaa, Ruud B., and Keysers, Christian. 2007. “Facial expressions: What the mirror neuron system can and cannot tell us.” Social Neuroscience 2:179222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veyne, Paul. 1988. Did the Greeks Believe in their Myths? An Essay on the Constitutive Imagination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, Daniel M. 2002. The Illusion of Conscious Will. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., and Wakabayashi, A.. 2006. “Predicting Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) from the Systemizing Quotient-Revised (SQ-R) and Empathy Quotient (EQ).” Brain Research 1079:4756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wicker, Bruno, Keysers, Christian, Plailly, Jane, Royet, Jean-Pierre, Gallese, Vittorio, and Rizzolatti, Giacamo. 2003. “Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust.” Neuron 40:655664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, Justin H.G., Whitten, Andrew, Suddendorf, Thomas, and Perrett, David I.. 2001. “Imitation, mirror neurons and autism.” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review 25:287295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, Allan. 1995. The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Young, Allan. 2002. “The self-traumatized perpetrator as a ‘transient mental illness’.” Evolution Psychiatrique 67:2650.Google Scholar
Young, Allan. 2004. “When traumatic memory was a problem: On the antecedents of PTSD.” In Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Issues and Controversies, edited by Rosen, G., 127146. Chichester UK: John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, Allan. 2006. “Remembering the evolutionary Freud.” Science in Context 19:175189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed