Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T15:29:37.420Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is breakfast consumption related to mental distress and academic performance in adolescents?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2007

Lars Lien*
Affiliation:
Institute of General Practice and Community Medicine and Institute of Psychiatry, Frederik Holst Hus University of Oslo, Boks 1072 Blindern, No-0316 Oslo, Norway
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To examine the relationship between mental distress, academic performance and regular breakfast consumption across gender and immigration status.

Design

Cross-sectional population-based study. Two four-page questionnaires were filled in during two school sessions.

Setting

All junior high schools in Oslo, Norway using the classroom as the setting for the study.

Subjects

All 10th grade students 15–16 years olds in 2000 and 2001. Of 8316 eligible students, 7343 (88.3%) participated in the study.

Results

All immigrant groups, except the Western countries group, are skipping breakfast more often than Norwegian students, and girls more often than boys (27 versus 19%). After adjustment for possible confounding factors, the odds ratio (OR) for being mentally distressed when eating breakfast seldom/never compared with every day was 3.0 (2.0–4.5) for boys, 1.6 (1.2–2.1) for girls and 1.6 (1.5–2.6) for the immigrant group. The comparable OR for having low school grades was similar for boys and girls, 2.0 (1.3–3.0), and 1.6 (1.5–2.6) for the immigrant groups.

Conclusions

Skipping breakfast is a common feature among 10th grade students. The implications of skipping breakfast on mental distress and academic performance are stronger for boys than girls and stronger for Norwegians compared with immigrants.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2007

Numerous publications have focused on the association between nutrition and mental health problems, with sugar, micronutrients and omega-3 fatty acids at the centre of attentionReference Gesch, Hammond, Hampson, Eves and Crowder15. The effect of dieting, especially among girls, on various mental health problems has also been well documentedReference Lien, Dalgard, Heyerdahl, Thoresen and Bjertness6, Reference Littleton and Ollendick7.

The mental health effects of eating meals regularly have received less attention. Smith has published a few studies on the association between mental health problems and eating breakfast regularlyReference Smith8Reference Smith10, and others have examined the effect of daily healthy practices, where eating breakfast is one of them, on mental well-beingReference Arai, Anme, Katakura and Sato11Reference Morimoto, Takeshita, Inoue-Sakurai and Maruyama13. The conclusion from most of these studies is that eating breakfast regularly is associated with an improved mental health status.

Several explanations have been offered for the relationship between mental health and eating breakfast regularly. According to Smith, breakfast and especially the intake of cereals is beneficial due to its effect on brain glucose after a night-time fastReference Smith10. Regular consumption of breakfast cereals has, in experimental studies, been shown not only to lead to positive mood, but also to improve memory and driving performanceReference Smith10.

Regular breakfast consumption has also been linked to reduced stress and reduced infection rates, with a concurrent lowering of cortisol activityReference Smith14. Improved sleep quality and bowel functions have also been related to regular breakfast consumption, both important to mediate psychological well-beingReference Smith10, Reference Tanaka, Taira, Arakawa, Masuda, Yamamoto and Komoda15. The effect of breakfast in reducing fatigue is probably the most important factor contributing to mental healthReference Chen, Sekine, Hamanishi, Wang, Gaina and Yamagami16.

Most studies of breakfast and mental health have been on small study samples and with a few exceptions on adults. Chen et al. carried out a prospective study on factors contributing to quality of life in 12- to 13-year-old Japanese children. They found that in addition to physical activity and a normal bedtime, regular breakfast was the most important contributor to quality of lifeReference Chen, Sekine, Hamanishi, Wang, Gaina and Yamagami16. In a review of dietary habits in adolescents, Bull found that skipping breakfast was related to dieting among girls and that black people skipped breakfast more often than whites, and people from England and Wales had breakfast less often compared with people from Northern and Southern IrelandReference Bull17. A study among Norwegian adolescents from different ethnic groups found a wide variation in food habits and dietingReference Kumar, Holmboe-Ottesen, Lien and Wandel18.

Several factors might influence the association between mental distress, school grades and eating breakfast regularly. Parental education is a strong predictor for school performance, but less so for mental distressReference Costello, Compton, Keeler and Angold19, 20. A change in family structure might, however, influence both mental distress and school performanceReference Gilman, Kawachi, Fitzmaurice and Buka21. Socio-economic status is a strong predictor of both food choice and meal practicesReference Johansen, Rasmussen and Madsen22, Reference Bull23.

The aim of this study is to examine the association between regular breakfast consumption and mental distress (symptom load) and academic performance (functional impact) of 10th grade students in a multicultural city. Due to the large sample size, we are also able to study if the association differs with regard to gender and immigration status.

Methods

Participants and subjects

The study was based on data from the youth part of the Oslo Health Study, a cross-sectional survey conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health, the Municipality of Oslo and the University of Oslo. All pupils in the 10th grade of all schools in Oslo during 1999/2000 and 2000/2001 were included. The 10th grade is compulsory in Norway; therefore, the present study included all 15–16 year olds in two cohorts. Of 8316 eligible pupils, 7343 (88.3%) participated in the study and each pupil completed two four-page questionnaires. Information about sex was missing for 38 participants because of a data coupling error, and these participants were excluded from the study, leaving 7305 participants.

Dependent variables

Average grade

Participants were asked to fill in the most recent grade recorded in their school record book for mathematics, written Norwegian, English and social science. The grade scale is from 1 (lowest) to 6 (highest). An average grade score for each participant was calculated from the four grades. The variable was dichotomised between those with average grade ≤ 3 and >3.

Mental distress

Internalised problems, such as anxiety and depression, were measured by the 10-item version of Hopkins Symptoms Checklist (HSCL-10). The reliability was high (Cronbach α = 0.87) and the correlation with other instruments, including HSCL-90, has ranged between 0.87 and 0.97Reference Lipman, Covi and Shapiro24Reference Strand, Dalgard, Tambs and Rognerud26. Each item was rated on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 4 (extremely), with the time period being the past week. An average score for all 10 items of ≥ 1.85 is a valid predictor for mental distress among subjects aged 16–24 years of age, corresponding to the 1.75 cut-off of HSCL-25Reference Strand, Dalgard, Tambs and Rognerud26, Reference Tambs27.

Independent variable

Having breakfast

The question asked was: ‘How often do you eat breakfast in an ordinary week?’ The answering alternatives were seldom/never, 1–2 times per week, 3–4 times per week, 5–6 times per week and daily. In one of the analyses, we dichotomised between those eating seldom ( ≤ 2 times per week) and often ( ≥ 3 times).

Control variables

Educational level of parents

To obtain information on parental educational level, the questionnaire was linked to sociodemographic information collected by Statistics Norway for all participants. Their unique 11-digit personal identification (ID) number identifies the individuals, and this number was used for the linkage of data files. The data used for research purposes are anonymous because all names and personal ID numbers have been omitted. The following variables were added: Statistics Norway's register of highest parental education completed as at 1 October 2000 (more information at www.ssb.no/english/subjects/05/01/inntind)28.

The educational level was, for the purpose of the analysis, grouped into four major groups according to the highest educational level attained: higher university, lower university/college, secondary education and primary or less education.

Family structure

This variable was self-reported by the pupils. We divided the responses into three groups: living with both parents; living with one parent (comprising those living with only their mother, only their father, or about the same time with either mother or father); and living with foster parents.

Immigrant status

The majority of the adolescents with immigrant background were born in Norway and were second-generation immigrants. The labelling of participants as immigrant was therefore determined based on their parents' country of birth. Statistics Norway defines immigrants (or ethnic minorities) as those having both parents born in a country other than Norway29. This definition was applied in this study.

Smoking

The possible categories were as follows: no, never; yes, but stopped; yes, at times; and yes, daily.

Dieting

The question as asked was: ‘Have you ever dieted’ with these alternatives: no, never; yes, in the past; yes, now; and yes, all the time.

Soft drinks consumption

The following question was asked: ‘How much do you normally drink of cola/‘fizzy’ drinks with sugar’: seldom/never, 1–6 glasses per week, 1 glass per day, 2–3 glasses per day, 4 glasses or more per day. The variable was dichotomised between those drinking one glass per week or less and those drinking two glasses or more per week.

Data analysis

Cross tables were analysed with Pearson's χ2 test, and crude odd ratios (ORs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated. A logistic regression model, with mental distress and school grades as dependent variables, was used to calculate the ORs in the multivariate analysis in separate analyses for gender and immigration status. In addition, interaction analysis between sex, immigration status and the main independent variable was conducted. The level of significance was set to P ≤ 0.05, at a 95% CI.

Ethics

The study protocol was reviewed by the Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics and approved by the Norwegian Data Inspectorate. The study has been conducted in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

Eating breakfast is more of a Norwegian and Western countries phenomenon, as significantly more of the immigrants from other continents tend to skip breakfast (Table 1). Only 19% of boys and 27% of the girls eat breakfast ≤ 2 times or less during an ordinary week. There are also significant differences between boys and girls and Norwegians and immigrants in the level of mental distress and the number of those with ≤ 3 as their average school grade (Table 2).

Table 1 Prevalence (%) of those eating breakfast ≤2 times per week by selected sociodemographic variables, soft drinks consumption, smoking and dieting

* P-value for the difference between those eating breakfast seldom and often, Pearson's χ2 test.

Table 2 Prevalence (%) of eating breakfast, mental distress and school grade by gender and immigrant status

* Total number.

Pearson's χ2 test for differences between groups is < 0.001, except for mental distress between Norwegians and immigrants (P = 0.009).

Parental education and to a lesser degree family structure also seem to influence the regularity of breakfast consumption (Table 1). Only 10% of students whose parents have a higher university degree eat breakfast seldom, while 38% of those with only primary education are in the same category. Students who drink sugar-containing soft drinks, smoke and diet report less frequent breakfast consumption compared with students who drink fewer soft drinks and do not smoke or diet.

Regression analysis

Before we entered all the proposed variables into the model, we tested the change in OR of the independent variable going from eating breakfast every day as the reference category to seldom/never, on each of the covariates. For mental distress, the largest change was for the dieting variable (OR from 3.2 to 2.2). Smoking gave a change in OR from 3.2 to 2.7. For school grades, parental educational level made the greatest change in OR from 3.4 to 2.5. Smoking changed the OR to 2.8 and drinking soft drinks changed it to 3.0. For the other variables, the changes were small (results not presented in the table).

The strongest correlation between the variables was for average grade and parental educational level, with a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.22 (P = 0.001). The strongest correlation between the independent variables was for soft drinks consumption and smoking, with a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.18 (P = 0.001). The final evaluation of the model showed a Hosmer and Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test of P = 0.95.

Boys have higher crude and adjusted ORs for mental distress when skipping breakfast than girls, and Norwegians considerably higher than immigrants (Table 3). For immigrants, it is only when going from eating breakfast every day to seldom/never that remains statistically significant after adjustment. For boys, even going from every day to 3–4 times a week remains significant (OR = 2.1).

Table 3 Crude and adjusted* association between mental distress and eating breakfast across gender and immigrant status; odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals

* Adjustment made for parental educational level, family structure, dieting, smoking and drinking soft drinks.

With school grades as dependent variable, the gender differences are less prominent for both the crude and adjusted ORs, while the differences between Norwegians and immigrants are immense (Table 4). For the immigrant group none of the adjusted ORs are statistically significant.

Table 4 Crude and adjusted* association between school grades and eating breakfast across gender and immigrant status; odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals

* Adjustment made for parental educational level, family structure, dieting, smoking and drinking soft drinks.

Discussion

The main finding from this study is that eating breakfast regularly is associated with less mental distress and improved academic performance in a dose–response manner among adolescents. Girls and immigrants skip breakfast more often than boys and the Norwegian group, but the association between both mental distress and academic performance is stronger for boys than girls and stronger for the Norwegian compared with the immigrant group even after adjustment for possible confounding factors.

Several studies by Smith have confirmed that eating breakfast regularly improves mental well-being and physical health, and reduces stress in the form of a reduced cortisol level among adultsReference Smith8, Reference Smith9, Reference Smith14. Other studies have shown that breakfast is associated with quality of life in schoolchildrenReference Chen, Sekine, Hamanishi, Wang, Gaina and Yamagami16, natural killer cell and lymphokine activity in workersReference Morimoto, Takeshita, Inoue-Sakurai and Maruyama13 and sleep health in studentsReference Tanaka, Taira, Arakawa, Masuda, Yamamoto and Komoda15.

The Norwegian breakfast most often includes some kind of cereal and a drink such as coffee, tea, fruit juice or milk. The frequency of skipping breakfast increases as adolescents become olderReference Bull17. In a Norwegian study, 73% of 15–16 year olds were eating breakfast daily, while only 50% of 18–19 year olds maintained the same habitReference Lien, Lytle and Klepp30. Less is known about the breakfast content of the immigrant groups. Studies has revealed that immigrants from Asia and Africa often consume less fruits and vegetables and more chocolate, sweets and full-fat milk than immigrants from Western countriesReference Kumar, Holmboe-Ottesen, Lien and Wandel18.

There might be many beneficial effects of breakfast consumption. These include, as pointed out by Smith, the vital energy intake after a long night's fast and the improved bowel function arising from the consumption of cerealsReference Smith10. Glucose is essential in the formation of tryptophan, a precursor protein in the synthesis of serotonin, which regulates mood and memoryReference Wallin and Rissanen4, Reference Christensen and Redig31, Reference Young, Smith, Pihl and Ervin32. Other positive effects of breakfast might be the consumption of micronutrients and essential fatty acids that in some studies have been shown to be associated with mental health problems such as affective disorders, hyperactivity and behavioural problemsReference Gesch, Hammond, Hampson, Eves and Crowder1Reference Benton, Haller and Fordy25Reference Schoenthaler and Bier33Reference Hibbeln and Salem36.

A different theory on the beneficial effects of regular breakfast eating is that the effect of regularity in life is the most important factor. Adolescents eating breakfast and other meals regularly also tend to have more healthy food habits, drink less alcohol and abstain from smokingReference Wetzler and Ursano12, Reference Bull17, Reference Johansen, Rasmussen and Madsen22. Finally, the social gathering around a table might also be favourable for mood and of course be an indicator of the functioning of the family that has been shown to be a predictor of distress in adolescentsReference Gilman, Kawachi, Fitzmaurice and Buka37, Reference Rutter and Smith38.

The strength of this study is the high response rate (88.3%) from all 15- to 16-year-old adolescents in Oslo for two consecutive years, and the large number of adolescents with an immigrant background (25%), making selection bias a minor problem. A validated questionnaire was applied to assess mental distress and self-reported grade to assess academic performance. We were able to control for known possible confounders in the multivariate analysis. However, the breakfast questions have not been validated.

There are several limitations in this study. First, we are not able to decide on the direction of the association. It is possible that students who are mentally distressed tend to skip breakfast more often that those who are not, but it is more unlikely that students with lower grades should have a tendency to skip breakfast more often than those with higher grades. Secondly, there might be even more important confounders than those we have already detected, obscuring the association between mental distress, academic performance and regularly eating breakfast.

Thirdly, there might be information bias in the form of misclassifications. There might be responders (personality types) that systematically tend to report negative exposure and the most negative outcome, or positive exposure and the most positive outcome, which may lead to a false inflation or deflation of the associationReference Kristensen39. It is, however, unlikely that, for example, a pupil with a depressive personality should report more frequent skipping of breakfast and lower grades than a non-depressive personality type. One way to minimise the effect of non-differential, dependent misclassification is to obtain objective information about exposure and/or outcomeReference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff40. In our case, more extensive information about breakfast habits could have been obtained from parents.

The public health impact of our study is that parents should be made more aware of the possible benefit of eating breakfast regularly. With both parents working in busy full-time jobs, it might be difficult to set aside time for breakfast. One possible solution to the time constraint felt by parents and adolescents is to serve breakfast at school, thereby also securing a balanced diet with cereals, fruit and vegetables.

Acknowledgements

Sources of funding: The author was employed as a Research fellow by the University of Oslo and did not receive any further funding.

Conflict of interest declaration: The author declares that there are no competing interests.

Acknowledgements: Data collection was conducted as part of the Oslo Health Study 2000–2001 in collaboration with the National Health Screening Service of Norway, now the Norwegian Institute of Public Health.

References

1Gesch, CB, Hammond, SM, Hampson, E, Eves, A, Crowder, MJ. Influence of supplementary vitamins, minerals and essential fatty acids on the antisocial behaviour of young adult prisoners. British Journal of Psychiatry 2002; 181: 22–8.Google Scholar
2Benton, D, Haller, J, Fordy, J. Vitamin supplementation for 1 year improves mood. Neuropsychobiology 1995; 32: 98105.Google Scholar
3Schnoll, R, Burshteyn, D, Cea-Aravena, J. Nutrition in the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a neglected but important aspect. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 2003; 28: 6375.Google Scholar
4Wallin, MS, Rissanen, AM. Food and mood: relationship between food, serotonin and affective disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplementum 1994; 377: 3640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Lahey MRS. Diet and its possible role in developmental disorders [online], 2002. Available atwww.bamford-lahey.org.Google Scholar
6Lien, L, Dalgard, F, Heyerdahl, S, Thoresen, M, Bjertness, E. The relationship between age of menarche and mental distress in Norwegian adolescent girls and girls from different immigrant groups in Norway: results from an urban city cross-sectional survey. Social Science & Medicine 2006; 63: 285–95.Google Scholar
7Littleton, HL, Ollendick, T. Negative body image and disordered eating behavior in children and adolescents: what places youth at risk and how can these problems be prevented? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 2003; 6: 5166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Smith, AP. Breakfast and mental health. International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition 1998; 49: 397402.Google Scholar
9Smith, AP. Breakfast cereal consumption and subjective reports of health. International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition 1999; 50: 445–9.Google Scholar
10Smith, AP. The concept of well-being: relevance to nutrition research. British Journal of Nutrition 2005; 93(Suppl 1): 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Arai, H, Anme, T, Katakura, N, Sato, I. A study on the influence of daily life habits on people's health symptoms (in Japanese). Nippon Koshu Eisei Zasshi – Japanese Journal of Public Health 2003; 50: 435–45.Google Scholar
12Wetzler, HP, Ursano, RJ. A positive association between physical health practices and psychological well-being. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 1988; 176: 280–3.Google Scholar
13Morimoto, K, Takeshita, T, Inoue-Sakurai, C, Maruyama, S. Lifestyles and mental health status are associated with natural killer cell and lymphokine-activated killer cell activities. Science of the Total Environment 2001; 270: 311.Google Scholar
14Smith, AP. Stress, breakfast cereal consumption and cortisol. Nutritional Neuroscience 2002; 5: 141–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Tanaka, H, Taira, K, Arakawa, M, Masuda, A, Yamamoto, Y, Komoda, Y, et al. . An examination of sleep health, lifestyle and mental health in junior high school students. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 2002; 56: 235–6.Google Scholar
16Chen, X, Sekine, M, Hamanishi, S, Wang, H, Gaina, A, Yamagami, T, et al. . Lifestyles and health-related quality of life in Japanese school children: a cross-sectional study. Preventive Medicine 2005; 40: 668–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Bull, NL. Studies of the dietary habits, food consumption and nutrient intakes of adolescents and young adults. World Review of Nutrition and Dietetics 1988; 57: 2474.Google Scholar
18Kumar, BN, Holmboe-Ottesen, G, Lien, N, Wandel, M. Ethnic differences in body mass index and associated factors of adolescents from minorities in Oslo, Norway: a cross-sectional study. Public Health Nutrition 2004; 7: 9991008.Google Scholar
19Costello, EJ, Compton, SN, Keeler, G, Angold, A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. Journal of the American Medical Association 2003; 290: 2023–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Oppedal B. Adolescent mental health in multicultural context. Dissertation, National Institute of Public Health, Oslo, 2003.Google Scholar
21Gilman, SE, Kawachi, I, Fitzmaurice, GM, Buka, SL. Family disruption in childhood and risk of adult depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 2003; 160: 939–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Johansen, A, Rasmussen, S, Madsen, M. Health behaviour among adolescents in Denmark: influence of school class and individual risk factors. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 2006; 34: 3240.Google Scholar
23Bull, NL. Dietary habits, food consumption, and nutrient intake during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health 1992; 13: 384–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Lipman, RS, Covi, L, Shapiro, AK. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL) – factors derived from the HSCL-90. Journal of Affective Disorders 1979; 1: 924.Google Scholar
25Sandanger, I, Moum, T, Ingebrigtsen, G, Sorensen, T, Dalgard, OS, Bruusgaard, D. The meaning and significance of caseness: the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 and the Composite International Diagnostic Interview. II. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 1999; 34: 53–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Strand, BH, Dalgard, OS, Tambs, K, Rognerud, M. Measuring the mental health status of the Norwegian population: a comparison of the instruments SCL-25, SCL-10, SCL-5 and MHI-5 (SF-36). Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 2003; 57: 113–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Tambs, K. Moderate effects of hearing loss on mental health and subjective well-being: results from the Nord-Trondelag Hearing Loss Study. Psychosomatic Medicine 2004; 66: 776–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Statistics Norway. Den Individbaserte Utdanningsstatistikken [The Individual Based Educational Statistics]. Report No. NOS C645. Oslo: Statistics Norway, 2001.Google Scholar
29Lie B. Innvandring og innvandrere, 2002 [Immigration and Immigrants 2002]. Oslo: Statistics Norway, 2002.Google Scholar
30Lien, N, Lytle, LA, Klepp, KI. Stability in consumption of fruit, vegetables, and sugary foods in a cohort from age 14 to age 21. Preventive Medicine 2001; 33: 217–26.Google Scholar
31Christensen, L, Redig, C. Effect of meal composition on mood. Behavioural Neuroscience 1993; 107: 346–53.Google Scholar
32Young, SN, Smith, SE, Pihl, RO, Ervin, FR. Tryptophan depletion causes a rapid lowering of mood in normal males. Psychopharmacology 1985; 87: 173–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Schoenthaler, S, Bier, ID. The effect of vitamin–mineral supplementation on juvenile delinquency among American schoolchildren: a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 2000; 6: 717.Google Scholar
34Silvers, KM, Scott, KM. Fish consumption and self-reported physical and mental health status. Public Health Nutrition 2002; 5: 427–31.Google Scholar
35Burgess, JR, Stevens, L, Zhang, W, Peck, L. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2000; 71(Suppl): 327–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36Hibbeln, JR, Salem, N Jr. Dietary polyunsaturated fatty acids and depression: when cholesterol does not satisfy. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 1995; 62: 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37Gilman, SE, Kawachi, I, Fitzmaurice, GM, Buka, L. Socio-economic status, family disruption and residential stability in childhood: relation to onset, recurrence and remission of major depression. Psychological Medicine 2003; 33: 1341–55.Google Scholar
38Rutter, M, Smith, D. Psychological Disorders in Young People: Time Trends and Their Causes. New York: Wiley, 1995.Google Scholar
39Kristensen, P. Bias from dependent errors in observational studies (in Norwegian). Tidsskrift for Norsk Laegeforening 2005; 125: 173–5.Google Scholar
40Podsakoff, PM, MacKenzie, SB, Lee, JY, Podsakoff, NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology 2003; 88: 879903.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Prevalence (%) of those eating breakfast ≤2 times per week by selected sociodemographic variables, soft drinks consumption, smoking and dieting

Figure 1

Table 2 Prevalence (%) of eating breakfast, mental distress and school grade by gender and immigrant status

Figure 2

Table 3 Crude and adjusted* association between mental distress and eating breakfast across gender and immigrant status; odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals

Figure 3

Table 4 Crude and adjusted* association between school grades and eating breakfast across gender and immigrant status; odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals