Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-16T06:58:54.312Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of a front-of-package disclosure on accuracy in assessing children’s drink ingredients: two randomised controlled experiments with US caregivers of young children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 November 2023

Frances Fleming-Milici*
Affiliation:
Rudd Center for Food Policy and Health, University of Connecticut, Hartford, CT 06103, USA
Haley Gershman
Affiliation:
Rudd Center for Food Policy and Health, University of Connecticut, Hartford, CT 06103, USA
Jennifer Pomeranz
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health Policy and Management, School of Global Public Health, New York University, New York, USA
Jennifer L Harris
Affiliation:
Rudd Center for Food Policy and Health, University of Connecticut, Hartford, CT 06103, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Test effects of a standardised front-of-package (FOP) disclosure statement (indicating added sugar, non-nutritive sweetener (NNS) and juice content) on accuracy in assessing ingredients and perceived healthfulness of children’s drinks.

Design:

In two randomised controlled experiments, the same participants viewed drink packages and indicated if products contained added sugar or NNS and percent juice and rated drink healthfulness. Experiment 1 (E1) included novel (non-US) children’s drinks with a) product claims only (control), b) claims and disclosure, or c) disclosure only. Experiment 2 (E2) included existing children’s drinks (with claims) with a) no disclosure (control) or b) disclosure. Both experiments evaluated sweetened (fruit drink and flavoured water) and unsweetened (100 % juice and juice/water blend) drinks. Potential individual differences (education level and race/ethnicity) in effects were explored.

Setting:

Online survey

Participants:

Six hundred and forty-eight US caregivers of young children (1–5 years)

Results:

FOP disclosures significantly increased accuracy for most ingredients and drink types, including identifying presence or absence of NNS in sweetened drinks, no added sugar in juice/water blends, and actual percent juice in fruit drinks and juice/water blends in both experiments. Disclosures also increased recognition that the novel 100 % juice and juice/water blend did not contain NNS or added sugar (E1) and existing sweetened drinks contained added sugar (E2). Disclosures reduced perceived healthfulness of sweetened drinks but did not increase unsweetened drink healthfulness ratings. Some differences by participant socio-demographic characteristics require additional research.

Conclusions:

FOP disclosures on children’s drink packages can increase caregivers’ understanding of product ingredients and aid in selecting healthier children’s drinks.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Ages 1 to 5 years is a critical time for developing taste preferences for nutritious foods and healthy eating habits that support children’s optimal development and lifelong health outcomes. Therefore, experts recommend against providing any type of drink that contains added sugar and/or non-nutritive sweeteners (NNS)(Reference Vos, Kaar and Welsh1Reference Lott, Callahan and Welker4). Providing sweetened drinks can reinforce a child’s natural taste preference for sweet and reduce acceptance of plain milk and water(Reference Perez-Escamilla, Segura-Perez and Lott5), the only drinks recommended for children under the age of 6 years(Reference Lott, Callahan and Welker4). Despite these recommendations, 45 % of 2- to 4-year-olds in the USA consume sugary drinks on a given day(Reference Welker, Jacquier and Catellier6), primarily in the form of fruit drinks(Reference Kay, Welker and Jacquier7), and 13 % of young children (2–5 years) consume drinks with NNS(Reference Sylvetsky, Jin and Clark8). Further, children in households with less-educated (v. higher-educated) parents are more likely to consume sugary drinks(Reference Han and Powell9), and as compared with non-Hispanic White and Mexican-American children, Black children have higher rates of fruit drink consumption(Reference Mendez, Miles and Poti10).

Common sweetened children’s fruit-flavoured drinks in the US market, including fruit drinks and flavoured waters (labelled as ‘water’ beverages), contain added sugar and/or NNS with little or no juice(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11). Common unsweetened children’s drinks include 100 % juice and juice/water blends, consisting of juice diluted with water and no added sweeteners(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11). In 2018, US sales of sweetened children’s drinks totalled $1·4 billion, compared with $838 million for unsweetened children’s drinks(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11). Also, although companies spent more in total advertising for unsweetened v. sweetened drink products ($34·4 v. $20·7 million)(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11), they devoted a higher proportion of advertising spending in children’s media to sweetened drinks (66 % v. 54 %).

Parents purchase sweetened fruit-flavoured children’s drinks for many reasons, including marketing appeals, convenience, price, child requests, misperceptions about healthfulness and confusion with 100 % juice(Reference Choi, Jensen and Fleming-Milici12Reference Fleming-Milici, Phaneuf and Harris15). Research shows that common product packaging features contribute to this confusion(Reference Fleming-Milici, Phaneuf and Harris15,Reference Harris and Pomeranz16) . For example, sweetened fruit-flavoured drink packages often include words such as ‘juice’, ‘nectar’ or fruit names(Reference Duffy, Hall and Dillman Carpentier17) and pictures of fruit, even when they contain little or no juice(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11,Reference Pomeranz and Harris18) . They also often contain nutrition-related marketing claims such as ‘100 % Vitamin C’ and ‘all natural’ ingredients, including ‘natural’ sweeteners (e.g. stevia, cane sugar)(Reference Jensen, Choi and Fleming-Milici13). Parents report that these package features lead them to believe that sweetened fruit-flavoured drinks are healthy choices for their children(Reference Fleming-Milici, Phaneuf and Harris15) and may not consider them to be ‘sugary drinks’ that are not recommended for children(Reference Choi, Jensen and Fleming-Milici12,Reference Jensen, Choi and Fleming-Milici13,Reference Munsell, Harris and Sarda19,Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20) .

Research has also shown that parents have difficulty distinguishing between sweetened and unsweetened options and identifying ingredients in drinks they serve their children(Reference Fleming-Milici, Phaneuf and Harris15). Children’s drink brands often offer both sweetened and unsweetened drinks with similar branding and packaging features(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11), and parents have reported mistakenly purchasing sweetened drinks, believing they were 100 % juice(Reference Fleming-Milici, Phaneuf and Harris15). In one study(Reference Harris and Pomeranz16), a majority of caregivers could not identify children’s drinks with NNS, and many incorrectly believed that unsweetened drinks contained added sugar, sweetened flavoured waters had no added sugar, and 100 % juice contained less than 100 % juice, even when looking at nutrition and ingredient information. Although parents express concerns about artificial sweeteners(Reference Munsell, Harris and Sarda19,Reference Sylvetsky, Greenberg and Zhao21,Reference Smith, Wells and Scarbecz22) , most could not identify NNS in the drinks they serve their children(Reference Jensen, Choi and Fleming-Milici13). Further, individuals with a high school education or less are less likely to understand current ingredient information provided on pre-packaged food (i.e. nutrition facts panel)(Reference Campos, Doxey and Hammond23); therefore, confusion about children’s drink ingredients may be greater among less-educated caregivers. Increasing parents’ knowledge of NNS, added sugar and juice content in children’s drinks may reduce misperceptions about the healthfulness of sweetened drinks(Reference Jensen, Choi and Fleming-Milici13,Reference Musicus, Roberto and Moran24) , which is associated with frequency of serving these drinks to their children(Reference Choi, Jensen and Fleming-Milici12,Reference Munsell, Harris and Sarda19) .

Therefore, efforts to increase caregivers’ understanding of ingredients in the drinks they serve their children may help reduce sugary drink consumption among young children. Previous research has demonstrated that adding warning labels and/or removing claims and fruit images from drink packages may also be effective(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20,Reference Musicus, Roberto and Moran24Reference Hall, Lazard and Grummon26) . However, government-mandated sugary beverage warnings (i.e. on billboards) have been successfully challenged in court and restrictions on truthful claims and images may not be legally feasible in the USA due to US constitutional protections for commercial speech(Reference Pomeranz and Harris18,Reference Pomeranz27) . Thus, experts have proposed front-of-package (FOP) disclosures that provide the same factual, uncontroversial and evidence-based information about all products in a standardised format to help consumers identify ingredients in both sweetened and unsweetened drinks and to reduce perceived healthfulness of sweetened children’s drinks(Reference Harris and Pomeranz16,Reference Pomeranz, Wilde and Mozaffarian28) . However, to-date research has not examined the effectiveness of FOP disclosures that contain factual ingredient-specific information without warning labels or other interpretive information, nor the effects of NNS disclosures on product packages, and further whether disclosures are effective among caregivers of diverse race/ethnicity and education level.

This study tests the effects of a proposed standardised factual FOP disclosure on accuracy in identifying added sugar, NNS and percentage of juice in common types of children’s drinks and perceived product healthfulness(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20). We hypothesised that: H1) the disclosure would improve accuracy on both novel and existing sweetened and unsweetened children’s drink products; H2) accuracy would be greater if common claims were also removed; and H3) the disclosure would reduce perceived healthfulness of sweetened drinks. Exploratory analyses assessed whether the FOP disclosure also affected caregivers’ intent to purchase the products and whether effects of FOP disclosures were consistent across participant education level and race/ethnicity to ensure effectiveness for priority populations(Reference Greenthal and Sorscher29).

Methods

We conducted one online study with two randomised controlled experiments using the same participants in May 2022. To test H1, Experiment 1 (E1) examined the effects of FOP disclosures on accuracy of assessing added sugar, NNS and percent juice in novel (i.e. non-US) children’s drink products; and Experiment 2 (E2) examined effects on existing packages of a popular US children’s drink brand. In both experiments, participants viewed four package images: two sweetened drinks (fruit drink and flavoured water) and two unsweetened drinks (100 % juice and juice/water blend), as these are the four common types of children’s drinks available in the USA.(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11) In E1, participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: a) control (children’s drink packages with product claims), b) disclosure with claims (added to control packages) or c) disclosure only (with claims removed). In E2, they were assigned to one of two conditions: a) control (existing product package with claims) or b) disclosures (added to existing package). To test H2, we examined differences between the two disclosure conditions in E1 (disclosure with claims (b) and disclosure only (c)). To test H3, we examined effects of disclosures on perceived healthfulness of sweetened and unsweetened drinks in both experiments. Study procedures, hypotheses and analysis plan were registered prior to data collection on AsPredicted.org (https://aspredicted.org/Y9X_FMK). Data were analysed from October to December 2022.

Participants

A national online survey panel company (InnovateMR) invited eligible panel members (US caregivers with children 1–5 years) to participate via email and sent a link to the study survey to interested participants. The survey was administered through Qualtrics software. Panel members received points for participating, which can be exchanged for online gift cards(Reference Innovate30). A target sample size of 600 was pre-determined based on power analysis assuming a medium effect size(Reference Cohen31). Quota sampling ensured at least 150 Black, 150 Hispanic and 150 participants with a high school education or less to evaluate differences by race/ethnicity and education. Prior to starting the survey, participants read an information sheet and indicated agreement to participate. Participants were then screened for eligibility (having a child 1–5 years, some responsibility for what that child eats and drinks, and no children with a disease/condition requiring a special diet). The University’s Institutional Review Board approved the study and determined it to be exempt as only survey data with no identifiable information were collected.

Stimuli

The packages to be used as stimuli were chosen following an online pre-test with caregivers of young children (1–5 years) (n 150). The pre-test assessed four alternative FOP disclosure designs and two alternative novel products not available in the USA (see online Supplementary Information for details). A graphic designer created the images for both experiments, which included the package front, nutrition facts panel (with percent juice at the top) and ingredient list (see Fig. 1 for one example and online Supplementary Information for all package images).

Fig. 1 Novel fruit drink image with claims and FOP disclosure*. *Packages in all conditions contained the same nutrition facts and ingredient list for each product type. FOP, front-of-package

In both experiments, the ‘control’ condition (a) images incorporated information currently included on children’s drink packages. In the novel drink experiment (E1), we added two product claims that are commonly found on US drink packages (‘natural flavors’, ‘good source of Vitamin C’)(Reference Harris, Romo Palafox and Choi11) and used the information panel (i.e. the nutrition facts panel, ingredient list and percent juice) from popular US children’s drinks in each drink category. Consistent with US regulations, the designation of a drink as 100 % juice was shown on both the package front and information panel. For the existing drink experiment (E2), the control condition utilised images of actual (Capri Sun) packages for each drink type, including claims and other product information on the existing package.

For both experiments, the ‘disclosure with claims’ condition (b) utilised the same packages as the control condition with the FOP disclosure box added. The FOP disclosure was configured to be less than 10 % of the entire front panel. The term ‘diet sweeteners’ was used to indicate NNS content on the FOP disclosure as previous research had ascertained that the term was widely understood and encompassed all types of NNS (including both ‘artificial’ and ‘natural’ high-intensity sweeteners)(Reference Fleming-Milici, Phaneuf and Harris15,Reference Harris, Phaneuf and Fleming-Milici32) . In E1, the ‘disclosure only’ condition (c) included the FOP disclosure on the condition (b) package, with the claims removed. Table 1 provides ingredient information and disclosures for all products in both experiments.

Table 1 Product ingredients, disclosures and claims

NNS, non-nutritive sweetener; FOP, front-of-package.

Measures and survey design

After providing consent and answering eligibility questions, eligible participants were first randomised to one of three conditions in E1. They were informed that they would view some packages of children’s drinks that are not currently available in the USA but might be sold in the future and asked to look at the package and then answer questions about the drink. After viewing and assessing the four novel drink products (one for each drink type), they were then randomly assigned to one of the two E2 conditions. They were informed that they would see packages of children’s drink products available in the USA and asked to answer the same questions about these drinks. For both experiments, packages of the four drink types were presented in random order.

To assess participants’ accuracy in recognising drink ingredients, while viewing each package they were asked to indicate whether the drink contained added sugar (yes or no), diet sweeteners (yes or no) and percent juice in the product (0–100 % sliding scale). After rating all products in both experiments, they were shown the same eight novel and existing products previously viewed (presented in random order) and asked to indicate how likely they would be to buy each (for novel products, ‘if it was available in the US’) for their child(ren) who are 1–5 years old (sliding scale: 1 (extremely unlikely) to 10 (extremely likely)). They were also asked to rate how healthy they think the product is for their child (sliding scale: 1 (extremely unhealthy) to 10 (extremely healthy)).

After answering all questions in both experiments, participants were then asked about their provision of fruit drinks, flavoured waters, 100 % juice and juice/water blends to their child(ren) who are 1–5 years old (seven-point scales: ‘never’ to ‘3 or more times per day’) and if they had provided each type of Capri Sun in the past month (yes or no). Next, participants indicated their provision of other types of drinks to their child(ren) (toddler milk, plain milk, flavoured milk, plain water and other sugar-sweetened drinks), how often they themselves drink sugar-sweetened drinks, and knowledge and agreement with healthy beverage recommendations (data not reported). Finally, they provided demographic information.

Statistical analysis

We coded accuracy according to whether participants correctly identified the presence or absence of added sugar and diet sweeteners. For percent juice, we conservatively coded responses as accurate if participants selected a percentage within a specified range, as in previous research(Reference Harris and Pomeranz16)(see Table 1). We assessed significant differences between the control and disclosure conditions in the percent of participants who accurately identified added sugar, NNS and percent juice for each drink type and experiment. We used Kruskal–Wallis analyses for E1 (three conditions) and chi-square analyses for E2 (two conditions). Post hoc chi-square analyses examined the effects of removing claims on packages in E1 by comparing accuracy for all ingredients and drink types between the two disclosure conditions (with and without claims). In analyses to test effects of adding the disclosure on perceived healthfulness, we calculated average healthfulness ratings for sweetened drinks (fruit drink and flavoured water) and unsweetened drinks (100 % juice and juice/water blend) as the dependent variables and experimental condition as the independent variable, using multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) for E1 and t tests for E2. Significance of all primary analyses was assessed using Bonferroni–Holm corrections to adjust for multiple comparisons within each experiment (P = 0·05, adjusted for fourteen comparisons).

Exploratory secondary analyses examined effects of disclosures on purchase intentions using MANOVA (E1) and t tests (E2), with purchase intention as the dependent variable and condition as the independent variable. To explore potential effects of disclosures on participants of differing race/ethnicity and education level, differences in accuracy by condition were also assessed separately for each individual characteristic using Kruskal–Wallis (E1) and chi-square (E2) analyses. Additional MANOVA assessed main effects of condition controlling for individual characteristics (education and race/ethnicity) on perceived healthfulness of sweetened and unsweetened drinks. For all exploratory analyses, we report significant differences at P < 0·05.

Results

Of 1075 panel members who responded to the survey invitation, 17 declined participation, 304 did not meet eligibility criteria, and 106 did not complete the survey, finished the survey too quickly (< 5 min) or failed the data integrity check. See online Supplementary Information for CONSORT participant flow diagram. The final sample (n 648) was 73 % female and socio-demographically diverse, including 72 % with less than a 4-year college degree, 27 % Hispanic, 25 % non-Hispanic Black, and more than one-third with annual household incomes below $35 000 (see Table 2). The majority reported serving their child each type of children’s drink at least once in the past week, ranging from 66 % for flavoured water to 91 % for 100 % juice. More than one-half reported serving Capri Sun 100 % juice and/or fruit drink. Participants were equally divided into conditions for both experiments. Comparisons of demographic characteristics by condition demonstrated successful random assignment for both experiments (all p’s ≥ 0·07), as well as distribution of E1 condition by condition in E2 (P = 0·63).

Table 2 Sample characteristics

HH, Household; SNAP, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program; WIC, Women, Infants, and Children Program.

Effects of front-of-package disclosures on accuracy in identifying drink ingredients

In the first experiment with novel drinks (E1), the majority of participants in the control condition (packages with claims and no disclosures) recognised that the fruit drink and flavoured water contained added sugar and the 100 % juice did not, but approximately two-thirds mistakenly believed the juice/water blend contained added sugar (see Table 3). The majority also accurately perceived that the 100 % juice and juice/water blend did not contain NNS, but fewer than one-half recognised that the fruit drink and flavoured water did contain NNS. Approximately one-half of participants understood that the fruit drink and flavoured water contained less than 20 % juice. On average, participants in the control condition believed that the fruit drink and flavoured water contained 30 % or more juice, although the products contained 5 % and 0 % juice, respectively. Just 42 % accurately identified the percent juice in the juice/water blend, and only 62 % correctly indicated that the 100 % juice contained 100 % juice.

Table 3 Effects of FOP disclosures on accuracy of assessing drink ingredients and product attitudes

FOP, front-of-package; NNS, non-nutritive sweetener.

* Significant after Bonferroni–Holm correction (P = 0·05 adjusted for fourteen comparisons).

Accuracy assessed as follows: proportion of participants who selected the accurate response. Significant differences between conditions assessed using Kruskal–Wallis (E1) and chi-square (E2).

Differences between conditions in product attitudes assessed using multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) (E1) and t tests (E2).

The effects of FOP disclosures on accuracy in identifying ingredients in novel drinks (E1) varied by ingredient and drink type. There were significant differences in the proportion of participants who recognised that the 100 % juice and juice/water blend product did not contain added sugar; the fruit drink and flavoured water contained NNS and the 100 % juice and juice/water blend did not; and accurate assessment of juice in the fruit drink, flavoured water and juice/water blend. In all cases, accuracy was higher in the two disclosure conditions than the control, but there were no significant differences between the two disclosure conditions (all P’s ≥ 0·04) (data not reported).

In E2 with existing products, two-thirds or fewer of participants who viewed the actual product package without the disclosure (control condition) recognised that the juice/water blend did not contain added sugar, the fruit drink did not contain NNS, and the accurate percent juice in the fruit drink and flavoured water. Accuracy was lowest for knowing that the flavoured water contained NNS (45 %). Most participants (≥ 70 %) understood that the fruit drink and flavoured water contained added sugar, the 100 % juice did not contain added sugar or NNS, and the percent juice in 100 % juice. Adding FOP disclosures to existing drinks significantly improved accuracy for all drinks and ingredients with one exception: it did not increase understanding that the 100 % juice contained 100 % juice.

The disclosures reduced perceived healthfulness of sweetened drinks in both experiments, with small to medium effect sizes, and perceived healthfulness did not differ significantly between the two FOP disclosure conditions in E1. However, disclosures did not affect perceived healthfulness of unsweetened drinks in either experiment. In addition, disclosures reduced intent to purchase the sweetened fruit drink and flavoured water in both experiments but did not affect purchase intent for unsweetened drinks.

Exploring differences by education level and race/ethnicity

Results of the exploratory analyses to examine whether FOP disclosures affected participants of differing education level and race/ethnicity were inconsistent (see online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2). In four cases, disclosures increased accuracy across all education levels and race/ethnicities: understanding that the novel juice/water blend did not contain added sugar, and identifying NNS in the three drinks that contained them (novel fruit drink and both flavoured waters). In most other analyses, we found significant differences for some but not all individual characteristics. However, disclosures did not consistently affect one group v. the others. In many cases, disclosures increased accuracy for individuals with a high school education or less but not for those with a 4-year college degree or higher, and for non-Hispanic Black or Hispanic individuals and not those in other racial/ethnic groups. In contrast, the main effects of disclosures on perceived healthfulness of sweetened drinks remained significant and effects on perceived healthfulness of unsweetened drinks remained non-significant in the models with race/ethnicity and education level as independent variables (see online Supplementary Table 3).

Discussion

These results largely support our first hypothesis (H1) that FOP disclosures improve caregivers’ ability to assess added sugar, NNS and percent juice in both novel and existing children’s drink products. In both experiments, disclosures increased accuracy for twenty of twenty-four ingredients disclosed (i.e. eight different drinks with three ingredients disclosed on each), including identifying absence of added sugar in juice and juice/water blends (in both experiments) and presence of added sugar in Capri Sun fruit drinks and flavoured water; presence or absence of NNS in sweetened drinks and absence of NNS in 100 % juice and juice/water blends; and actual percent juice in fruit drinks, flavoured water and juice/water blends. When effects of disclosures were non-significant, accuracy was relatively high (i.e. > 60 %) in the control condition with no disclosures, including recognising added sugar in novel sweetened drinks and percent juice in 100 % juice (both experiments).

Therefore, the disclosures provide an opportunity to increase caregivers’ understanding and awareness of ingredients in children’s drinks that raise major public health concerns(Reference Vos, Kaar and Welsh1,Reference Lott, Callahan and Welker4) . Although awareness of sugar in novel fruit drinks and flavoured waters was relatively high without disclosures, the disclosures increased awareness that Capri Sun fruit drink and flavoured water contain added sugar, products that most participants reported giving to their child. Disclosures also increased understanding that some children’s fruit drinks and flavoured waters contain NNS, an ingredient most parents do not believe is safe for their children’s consumption(Reference Sylvetsky and Dietz33) but have difficulty recognising when disclosed only in ingredient lists on nutrition facts panels(Reference Harris and Pomeranz16). Disclosures were also effective at increasing understanding that healthier juice/water blends do not contain added sugar and contain moderate amounts of juice.

These findings are similar to studies that have examined effects of warning labels. FOP warnings, including symbols to highlight ‘high-in’ ingredients to avoid or statements about health harms related to consumption, have been found to correct misperceptions about the healthfulness of fruit drinks and reduce selection for their children(Reference Musicus, Roberto and Moran24Reference Hall, Lazard and Grummon26). The results assessing caregivers’ base-level understanding of ingredients in children’s sweetened and unsweetened drinks when viewing packages without disclosures (i.e. control conditions in both experiments) are also in line with previous research showing limited knowledge that fruit drinks and flavoured waters often contain NNS and misunderstandings about added sugar content in flavoured waters and unsweetened juices(Reference Harris and Pomeranz16).

Our results did not support H2. Contrary to predictions, accuracy did not improve when common claims were removed from novel drink packages that contained disclosures. Previous research has shown that removing claims on fruit drinks can increase adults’ accurate understanding of ingredients(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20), decrease perceived healthfulness(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20,Reference Hall, Lazard and Grummon26) , and reduce intent to purchase for their children(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20)and consume themselves(Reference Hall, Lazard and Grummon26). However, research in this area is somewhat inconsistent. One study found that removing claims did not reduce parents’ selection of sweetened fruit drinks for their child if a fruit image remained on the package(Reference Musicus, Roberto and Moran24). The same study showed that either adding warnings or removing both claims and fruit imagery on product packages reduced parents’ selection of sugar-sweetened fruit drinks for their child but did not test a condition with warnings added and claims removed(Reference Musicus, Roberto and Moran24). Research is needed to understand the relative contribution of adding warnings or disclosures and/or removing claims and imagery in reducing parents’ purchases of sweetened drinks for their children.

Moreover, the present research shows that even when claims and fruit imagery were present on product packages, the disclosure corrected misperceptions about ingredients and healthfulness of sweetened drinks. Similar to our findings, previous research shows that warning labels were effective in reducing parents’ sugary drink selection for their children when added to packages that also contained fruit imagery and marketing claims(Reference Musicus, Roberto and Moran24). Therefore, FOP disclosures alone may provide enough information to counteract common claims, such as ‘good source of Vitamin C’, ‘all natural ingredients’ and ‘no high fructose corn syrup’, that may lead caregivers to believe that sweetened children’s drinks are healthful choices for children(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20).

Our results supported H3: FOP disclosures reduced perceived healthfulness of sweetened drinks for both novel and existing products. However, they did not increase perceived healthfulness of unsweetened drinks. Similarly, in the exploratory analyses, disclosures reduced caregivers’ intentions to purchase both types of sweetened drinks (fruit drinks and flavoured waters) for their child but did not increase intent to purchase unsweetened drinks. The disclosure also reduced perceived healthfulness and purchase intentions for sweetened drinks regardless of the presence of claims on product packages. As perceived healthfulness is strongly associated with parents’ purchases of sweetened drinks for their children(Reference Choi, Jensen and Fleming-Milici12,Reference Munsell, Harris and Sarda19) , this finding indicates that disclosures could be an effective tool to reduce purchases of sweetened children’s drinks in addition to increasing understanding of drink ingredients. However, further research is needed to test this hypothesis. Additional research is also needed to determine whether disclosures lead caregivers to substitute 100 % juice or juice/water blends for sweetened children’s drinks and/or affect provision of plain milk or water, as recommended by public health experts(Reference Lott, Callahan and Welker4,Reference Heyman and Abrams34) .

Exploratory analyses found that disclosures did not consistently affect all socio-demographic groups when results were stratified by education level and race/ethnicity. However, we found no evidence that the disclosures were consistently less effective with less-educated (v. higher-educated) caregivers or, Black or Hispanic (v. non-Hispanic White) participants. In many cases, differences were significant for these groups and not for more highly educated and/or non-Hispanic White participants. In addition, the disclosures continued to reduce perceived healthfulness of sweetened drinks when race/ethnicity and education level were included in the models. These findings help to address concerns raised by advocates that FOP changes must be appropriate for all consumers(Reference Greenthal and Sorscher29), but future research is needed to examine effects of disclosures with larger samples of individuals in populations of concern (i.e. lower education level, Black and Hispanic).

Strengths and limitations

This study has a number of strengths. To our knowledge, it is the first to assess disclosures of NNS on children’s drink packages, and the first to examine and demonstrate effects of a single standardised disclosure that indicates the presence or absence of NNS and added sugar and the percent juice. The randomised controlled design demonstrates causal impact of adding the FOP disclosure, including on packages of novel products and existing drinks that were widely purchased by our participants, even on sweetened drink packages with nutrition-related claims.

This study also has limitations. Use of quota sampling ensured a diverse sample, but findings are not representative of the US population. The randomised controlled design demonstrates causal impact of adding FOP disclosures in an online setting when participants were asked to examine the packages. However, these findings may differ in a real-world situation such as the supermarket where purchase decisions occur under time constraints and with competing brands and drink categories with various package claims and stocked on shelves together. Further, asking participants to identify product ingredients before rating product healthfulness likely affected absolute healthfulness ratings. However, participants answered the same questions in the same order in all conditions, allowing us to conclude that any differences in perceived healthfulness between conditions were due to the different stimuli they viewed (i.e. presence or absence of FOP disclosure). It is also possible that whether or not participants saw disclosures in E1 affected their accuracy in E2. However, E1 conditions were randomly distributed by condition in E2, and the effects of the disclosures remained significant in all models after controlling for E1 condition (analyses not reported). In addition, although we found no evidence that FOP disclosures were consistently more or less effective with individuals of differing education level or race/ethnicity, future research with larger sample sizes is needed to determine potential differential effects among individuals of diverse socio-demographic characteristics. Finally, our findings suggest potential effects on purchase intentions. Behavioural intentions (i.e. purchase intent) have been shown to predict dietary behaviours(Reference McDermott, Oliver and Simnadis35), and previous research shows that perceived healthfulness is associated with parents serving sweetened children’s drinks(Reference Choi, Jensen and Fleming-Milici12,Reference Munsell, Harris and Sarda19) , but future research is needed to determine the effects of the FOP disclosure on actual purchasing behaviour.

Policy implications

These findings can help inform efforts to develop FOP information to help consumers identify healthy foods when grocery shopping, as outlined in the US White House Strategy for Hunger, Nutrition and Health(36). The federal government should act to address inadequacies of current labelling, by requiring the disclosures tested in this study on the FOP of children’s sweetened and unsweetened drinks. In the USA, the FDA regulates beverage labels and has the authority to revise certain labelling requirements on beverages that contain or purport to contain fruit juice (i.e. fruit-flavoured sweetened drinks and unsweetened juices) to address misleading labelling practices; however, the FDA frequently acts only when Congress expressly requires it to do so. Further, some labelling changes (e.g. requiring the percent juice on the FOP) require an act of Congress(Reference Pomeranz and Harris18). The First Amendment of the US Constitution protects commercial speech, which includes product labelling, from government interference. However, the government routinely requires factual disclosures on products to provide information and prevent deception of consumers. The tested disclosures meet constitutional requirements for government-mandated disclosures on product labelling; they are purely factual and uncontroversial, evidence-based (justified), about the products at issue, reasonably related to the government’s interest in preventing deception of consumers, and not unduly burdensome as they occupied less than 10 % of the FOP(Reference Pomeranz27,Reference Pomeranz, Wilde and Mozaffarian28) .

The proposed factual disclosures may be more legally and politically feasible than warnings at this time. Warnings are a common feature of the consumer protection landscape, and a meta-analysis showed them to be an effective way to reduce sugary drink purchases(Reference Grummon and Hall25). A properly worded and formatted warning on products themselves should similarly survive First Amendment scrutiny(Reference Pomeranz, Mozaffarian and Micha37). However, the Court of Appeals for the Ninth Circuit found San Francisco’s billboard warning requirement for sugary beverages to be unconstitutional(38,39) . In striking down this warning, several judges made sweeping statements doubting the link between added sugar and health harms and inconceivably arguing that only warnings dating back to 1791 are constitutional(Reference Pomeranz, Mozaffarian and Micha37).

Certain claims may be deceptive or misleading, and these would be also ripe for regulation (e.g. ‘no artificial sweeteners’ when the product contains stevia extract)(Reference Pomeranz and Harris18); conversely, prohibiting factually accurate claims (e.g. ‘no high fructose corn syrup’) on product packaging is not a legally feasible policy option(Reference Pomeranz40). Another method to address misleading claims would be for the FDA to reconsider its fortification policy to disallow the fortification of drinks with added sugars. Currently, fruit drink labels tout high levels of Vitamin C due to fortification, which has been found to increase consumers’ perception that the drinks are healthy(Reference Hall, Lazard and Higgins20). Prohibiting the fortification of fruit drinks would be a method to minimise potentially misleading vitamin claims on fruit drinks. Nonetheless, this study confirms that standardised FOP ingredient disclosures were effective at increasing consumer understanding even when common claims remained on the package. Therefore, requiring the proposed standardised FOP disclosure may be an effective path to reducing purchases of products not recommended for children’s consumption, including drinks with NNS and/or added sugar.

Conclusion

Misperceptions about the ingredients and healthfulness of sweetened children’s drinks contribute to high levels of sugary drink consumption among young children. This research demonstrates that a clear standardised disclosure of added sugar and NNS and the percent of juice content on children’s drink packages provides enough information to correct these misperceptions and would aid parents in selecting healthier drinks for their children. Given other proposed FOP changes to reduce sugary drink consumption may not pass judicial scrutiny, the proposed disclosure may be both an effective strategy to reduce children’s sweetened drink consumption and a potentially viable policy change.

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge the contribution of the Rudd Center staff in designing the stimuli.

Financial support

This research was supported by Healthy Eating Research, a national programme of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. Heathy Eating Research had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Authorship

F.F-M. and J.H. formulated the research questions and designed the study. F.F-M. acquired funding for the research. F.F-M., J.L.H. and H.G. designed the survey. H.G. supervised the creation of the stimuli and data collection. J.H. analysed the data and interpreted results. F.F-M. and J.L.H. and wrote the manuscript. J.P. contributed to writing and editing. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Ethics of human subject participation

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the University of Connecticut’s Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects/patients.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980023001969

References

Vos, MB, Kaar, JL, Welsh, JA et al. (2017) Added sugars and cardiovascular disease risk in children: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation 135, e1017e1034.Google Scholar
Muth, ND, Dietz, WH, Magge, SN et al. (2019) Public policies to reduce sugary drink consumption in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 143, e20190282. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-0282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Department of Agriculture & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2020) Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2020–2025. https://www.dietaryguidelines.gov/sites/default/files/2021-03/Dietary_Guidelines_for_Americans-2020-2025.pdf (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Lott, M, Callahan, E, Welker, E et al. (2019) Consensus Statement. Healthy Beverage Consumption in Early Childhood: Recommendations from Key National Health and Nutrition Organizations. https://healthyeatingresearch.org/research/consensus-statement-healthy-beverage-consumption-in-early-childhood-recommendations-from-key-national-health-and-nutrition-organizations/ (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Perez-Escamilla, R, Segura-Perez, S & Lott, M (2017) Feeding Guidelines for Infants and Young Toddlers: A Responsive Parenting Approach. https://healthyeatingresearch.org/research/feeding-guidelines-for-infants-and-young-toddlers-a-responsive-parenting-approach/ (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Welker, EB, Jacquier, EF, Catellier, DJ et al. (2018) Room for improvement remains in food consumption patterns of young children aged 2–4 years. J Nutr 148, 1536S1546S.Google Scholar
Kay, MC, Welker, EB, Jacquier, EF et al. (2016) Beverage consumption patterns among infants and young children (0(-)47·9 months): data from the feeding infants and toddlers study. Nutrients 10, 825. doi: 10.3390/nu10070825.Google Scholar
Sylvetsky, AC, Jin, Y, Clark, EJ et al. (2017) Consumption of Low-Calorie Sweeteners among Children and Adults in the United States. J Acad Nutr Diet 117, 441448.e2.Google Scholar
Han, E & Powell, LM (2013) Consumption patterns of sugar-sweetened beverages in the United States. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 4353.Google Scholar
Mendez, MA, Miles, DR, Poti, JM et al. (2019) Persistent disparities over time in the distribution of sugar-sweetened beverage intake among children in the United States. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 7989.Google Scholar
Harris, JL, Romo Palafox, MJ, Choi, YY et al. (2019) Children’s Drink FACTS 2019. Sales, Nutrition, and Marketing of Children’s Drinks. http://uconnruddcenter.org/files/Pdfs/FACTS2019.pdf (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Choi, YY, Jensen, ML, Fleming-Milici, F et al. (2022) Caregivers’ provision of sweetened fruit-flavoured drinks to young children: importance of perceived product attributes and differences by socio-demographic and behavioural characteristics. Public Health Nutr 25, 23082316.Google Scholar
Jensen, ML, Choi, YY, Fleming-Milici, F et al. (2021) Caregivers’ understanding of ingredients in drinks served to young children: opportunities for nutrition education and improved labeling. Curr Dev Nutr 6, nzab151.Google Scholar
Choi, YY, Andreyeva, T, Fleming-Milici, F et al. (2022) U.S. Households’ Children’s Drink Purchases: 2006–2017 trends and associations with marketing. Am J Prev Med 62, 917.Google Scholar
Fleming-Milici, F, Phaneuf, L & Harris, JL (2022) Marketing of sugar-sweetened children’s drinks and parents’ misperceptions about benefits for young children. Matern Child Nutr 18, e13338-n/a.Google Scholar
Harris, JL & Pomeranz, JL (2021) Misperceptions about added sugar, non-nutritive sweeteners and juice in popular children’s drinks: experimental and cross-sectional study with U.S. parents of young children (1–5 years). Pediatr Obes 16, e12791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, EW, Hall, MG, Dillman Carpentier, FR et al. (2021) Nutrition claims on fruit drinks are inconsistent indicators of nutritional profile: a content analysis of fruit drinks purchased by households with young children. J Acad Nutr Diet 121, 3646.e4.Google Scholar
Pomeranz, JL & Harris, JL (2020) Children’s fruit ‘Juice’ drinks and FDA regulations: opportunities to increase transparency and support public health. Am J Public Health 110, 871880.Google Scholar
Munsell, CR, Harris, JL, Sarda, V et al. (2016) Parents’ beliefs about the healthfulness of sugary drink options: opportunities to address misperceptions. Public Health Nutr 19, 4654.Google Scholar
Hall, MG, Lazard, AJ, Higgins, ICA et al. (2022) Nutrition-related claims lead parents to choose less healthy drinks for young children: a randomized trial in a virtual convenience store. Am J Clin Nutr 115, 11441154.Google Scholar
Sylvetsky, AC, Greenberg, M, Zhao, X et al. (2014) What parents think about giving nonnutritive sweeteners to their children: a pilot study. Int J Pediatr 2014, 819872.Google Scholar
Smith, MA, Wells, MH, Scarbecz, M et al. (2019) Parents’ preferences and perceptions of their children’s consumption of sugar and non-nutritive sugar substitutes. Pediatr Dent 41, 119128.Google Scholar
Campos, S, Doxey, J & Hammond, D (2011) Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 14, 14961506.Google Scholar
Musicus, AA, Roberto, CA, Moran, AJ et al. (2022) Effect of front-of-package information, fruit imagery, and high-added sugar warning labels on parent beverage choices for children: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open 5, e2236384.Google Scholar
Grummon, AH & Hall, MG (2020) Sugary drink warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med 17, e1003120.Google Scholar
Hall, MG, Lazard, AJ, Grummon, AH et al. (2020) The impact of front-of-package claims, fruit images, and health warnings on consumers’ perceptions of sugar-sweetened fruit drinks: three randomized experiments. Prev Med 132, 105998.Google Scholar
Pomeranz, JL (2022) United States: protecting commercial speech under the first amendment. J Law Med Ethics 50, 265275.Google Scholar
Pomeranz, JL, Wilde, P, Mozaffarian, D et al. (2019) Mandating front-of-package food labels in the U.S. – what are the first amendment obstacles? Food Policy 86, 101722. 10.1016/j.foodpol.2019.05.005.Google Scholar
Greenthal, E & Sorscher, S (2021) Can Food Labeling Policy Advance Health Equity? https://www.fdli.org/2021/09/can-food-labeling-policy-advance-health-equity/ (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Innovate, MR (2023) ESOMAR Guiding Questions to Help Buyers of Online Samples. https://app.hubspot.com/documents/1791513/view/438671377?accessId=92ee05 (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112, 155159.Google Scholar
Harris, JL, Phaneuf, L & Fleming-Milici, F (2022) Effects of sugary drink countermarketing videos on caregivers’ attitudes and intentions to serve fruit drinks and toddler milks to young children. Am J Public Health 112 (S8), S807S816.Google Scholar
Sylvetsky, AC & Dietz, WH (2014) Nutrient-content claims--guidance or cause for confusion? N Engl J Med 371, 195198.Google Scholar
Heyman, MB & Abrams, SA (2017) Section on gatroenterology H, and nutrition, committee on nutrition. Fruit juice in infants, children, and adolescents: current recommendations. Pediatrics 139, e20170967. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-0967.Google Scholar
McDermott, MS, Oliver, M, Simnadis, T et al. (2015) The theory of planned behaviour and dietary patterns: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Prev Med 81, 150156.Google Scholar
THE WHITE HOUSE (2022) Biden-Harris Administration National Strategy on Hunger, Nutrition, and Health. https://www.whitehouse.gov/wp-content/uploads/2022/09/White-House-National-Strategy-on-Hunger-Nutrition-and-Health-FINAL.pdf (accessed July 2023).Google Scholar
Pomeranz, JL, Mozaffarian, D & Micha, R (2020) Sugar-sweetened beverage warning policies in the broader legal context: health and safety warning laws and the first amendment. Am J Prev Med 58, 783788.Google Scholar
Am. Bev. Ass’n v. City & Cty. of San Francisco, 871 F.3d 884 (9th Cir. 2017). https://case-law.vlex.com/vid/am-beverage-ass-n-894492976 (accessed October 2023).Google Scholar
Am. Bev. Ass’n v. City & Cty. of San Francisco, 916 F.3d 749 (9th Cir. 2019). https://case-law.vlex.com/vid/am-beverage-ass-n-894492976 (accessed October 2023).Google Scholar
Pomeranz, JL (2019) Abortion disclosure laws and the first amendment: the broader public health implications of the supreme court’s becerra decision. Am J Public Health 109, 412418.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Novel fruit drink image with claims and FOP disclosure*. *Packages in all conditions contained the same nutrition facts and ingredient list for each product type. FOP, front-of-package

Figure 1

Table 1 Product ingredients, disclosures and claims

Figure 2

Table 2 Sample characteristics

Figure 3

Table 3 Effects of FOP disclosures on accuracy of assessing drink ingredients and product attitudes

Supplementary material: File

Fleming-Milici et al. supplementary material

Fleming-Milici et al. supplementary material

Download Fleming-Milici et al. supplementary material(File)
File 14.8 MB