Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T12:58:17.310Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of high doses of folic acid supplementation in early pregnancy on child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study in Crete, Greece

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2012

Leda Chatzi*
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Eleni Papadopoulou
Affiliation:
Centre for Research in Environmental Epidemiology (CREAL), Barcelona, Spain Municipal Institute of Medical Research (IMIM–Hospital del Mar), Barcelona, Spain Department of Experimental and Health Sciences, Pompeu Fabra University (UPF), Barcelona, Spain National School of Public Health, Athens, Greece
Katerina Koutra
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Theano Roumeliotaki
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Vaggelis Georgiou
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Nikolaos Stratakis
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Vassiliki Lebentakou
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Mariana Karachaliou
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Maria Vassilaki
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, PO Box 2208, 71003 Heraklion, Crete, Greece
Manolis Kogevinas
Affiliation:
Centre for Research in Environmental Epidemiology (CREAL), Barcelona, Spain Municipal Institute of Medical Research (IMIM–Hospital del Mar), Barcelona, Spain National School of Public Health, Athens, Greece
*
*Corresponding author. Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To investigate whether high doses of folic acid supplementation in early pregnancy are associated with child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age.

Design

The study uses data from the prospective mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study. Pregnant women completed an interviewer-administered questionnaire on folic acid supplementation at 14–18 weeks of gestation. Neurodevelopment at 18 months was assessed with the use of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (3rd edition). Red-blood-cell folate concentrations in cord blood were measured in a sub-sample of the study population (n 58).

Setting

Heraklion, Crete, Greece, 2007–2010.

Subjects

Five hundred and fifty-three mother–child pairs participating in the ‘Rhea’ cohort.

Results

Sixty-eight per cent of the study participants reported high doses of supplemental folic acid use (5 mg/d), while 24 % reported excessive doses of folic acid (>5 mg/d) in early pregnancy. Compared with non-users, daily intake of 5 mg supplemental folic acid was associated with a 5-unit increase on the scale of receptive communication and a 3·5-unit increase on the scale of expressive communication. Doses of folic acid supplementation higher than 5 mg/d were not associated with additional increase in the neurodevelopmental scales.

Conclusions

This is the first prospective study showing that high doses of supplementary folic acid in early pregnancy may be associated with enhanced vocabulary development, communicational skills and verbal comprehension at 18 months of age. Additional longitudinal studies and trials are needed to confirm these results.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Adverse environmental events during sensitive periods of organ development trigger plastic responses that result in long-lasting functional alterations and influence risk for disease later in life(Reference Gluckman, Hanson and Beedle1). Folic acid is an important micronutrient, essential for brain development and function in the antenatal and early postnatal periods(Reference Benton2, Reference Black3). Experimental data have shown that there is active placental transport of folic acid and that brain folic acid levels are elevated in the fetus during early development(Reference Greenblatt, Huffman and Reiss4). Folic acid intake is significantly correlated with circulating folic acid concentrations while during pregnancy large amounts of folic acid are required, thus there is a substantial risk of deficiency(Reference Scholl and Johnson5). A deficiency in folic acid during pregnancy is a well-known risk factor for neural tube defects(Reference Lumley, Watson and Watson6, Reference Lee, Lee and Lim7). Moreover, animal studies have shown that folic acid plays an important role in early brain development as it influences neuronal and glial growth and proliferation, and can affect the synthesis and release of neurotransmitters such as dopamine, adrenaline and serotonin(Reference Greenblatt, Huffman and Reiss4, Reference Craciunescu, Brown and Mar8).

There are few epidemiological studies demonstrating the impact of folic acid supplement use by healthy pregnant women on neurodevelopmental outcomes in their offspring and findings are not consistent. Four studies have shown a positive association of maternal folic acid status or folic acid supplement use on mental and cognitive development of the child(Reference Julvez, Fortuny and Mendez9Reference Wehby and Murray13). However, most of these studies have focused on the effect of folic acid supplement use on child neurodevelopment at school ages and only one study has evaluated the effect of folic acid supplement use in early childhood (18 months of life)(Reference Roza, van Batenburg-Eddes and Steegers10). Two other studies among healthy pregnant women did not find any association between folic acid supplement use in pregnancy and their children's neurodevelopment from 2 to 6 years of age(Reference Dobo and Czeizel14, Reference Tamura, Goldenberg and Chapman15). Thus, despite the fact that early pregnancy is the most important period for embryogenesis and fetal programming, relatively little is known about the implications of low, moderate and high doses of folic acid supplementation during this particular pregnancy period on child neurodevelopment.

We examined whether high daily doses of folic acid supplementation in early pregnancy affect children's mental and psychomotor development at 18 months in the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study in Crete, Greece.

Materials and methods

Study population

The ‘Rhea’ project is a mother–child study which examines prospectively a population-based cohort of pregnant women and their children at the prefecture of Heraklion in Crete, Greece(Reference Chatzi, Plana and Daraki16). Female residents (Greek and immigrants) who became pregnant during the 12-month period starting February 2007 were contacted at four maternity clinics in Heraklion and asked to participate in the study. The first contact was made at the time of the first major ultrasound examination. Women were then contacted at mid pregnancy, at third trimester, at birth, at 6 and 18 after delivery, and they are currently being contacted at 4 years after birth. Interview-administered and medical records were used to obtain information on sociodemographic, environmental and psychosocial factors during pregnancy and early childhood. The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the ethical committee of the University Hospital in Heraklion, Crete, Greece. Written informed consent was obtained from all women participating in the study.

During the study recruitment period 1765 eligible women were approached; 1610 (91 %) agreed to participate and 1388 (86 %) were followed up until delivery. Detailed characteristics of the study population have been described elsewhere(Reference Chatzi, Plana and Daraki16). A random sample of 828 mothers were contacted by telephone at the 18 months follow-up and 599 (72 %) agreed to participate in the neurodevelopmental assessment(Reference Koutra, Chatzi and Roumeliotaki17). Thirteen women (2 %) did not provide complete information on supplement use during pregnancy and therefore were excluded from the present analysis. We included only women with singleton pregnancies; thus multiple pregnancies were excluded (n 26). We also excluded seven infants due to incomplete examination (n 1), signs of Pervasive Developmental Disorders (n 1), plagiocephalus (n 1), brain tumour (n 1), microcephalus (n 2) and hydrocephalus (n 1). Hence, a cohort of 553 (92 % of the study population of children with neurodevelopment assessment) mother–child pairs was available for the present analysis (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1 Flow diagram defining the participants in the analysis

Folic acid supplement use

Pregnant women participating in the ‘Rhea’ study were asked, using a questionnaire administered by a trained research nurse between the 14th and 18th week of gestation, whether they had taken folic acid supplements since they became pregnant. Supplement users were asked to report the brand name, the dose and the frequency of intake. The answers were open and not pre-coded so as to ensure that all sources of supplementation were recorded and none was excluded. Supplement use (brand, dose, frequency) was converted into daily intake (mg/d) by using dosage information provided on the package of each product. The majority of women (n 492, 97 %) used only one product providing 5 mg of folic acid per daily dose. None of the women who reported folic acid supplement use during early pregnancy (n 507) used multivitamins containing folic acid as well, while fourteen (2·8 %) of them used multivitamin products without folic acid. A small proportion of women (n 72, 14 %) reported the use of both folic acid supplements and iron supplement products containing small amounts of folic acid; in these cases, the total quantity of folic acid was estimated by summing up all sources of daily intake. Folic acid supplement users were assigned to the following categories: (i) women with no folic acid intake from supplements (0 mg/d); (ii) those with daily intake of 5 mg of folic acid from supplements (5 mg/d); and (iii) women with daily intake of folic acid from supplements higher than 5 mg, ranging from 6 to 15 mg/d (>5 mg/d). There were no women with daily intake of supplemental folic acid between 0 and 4 mg.

Validation of supplemental folic acid use

Red-blood-cell folate concentrations in cord blood were measured by immunoassay with chemiluminescence (Roche kit, Elecsys 2010 analyser) in an effort to validate the self-reported folic acid supplement use. This analysis was available for a random sub-sample (n 58) of the total ‘Rhea’ study population. The collected cord blood samples were analysed in batches to reduce between-assay variability using an internal control (standard) for each batch. Mothers who reported supplemental folic acid use in early pregnancy (n 51; 88 %) gave birth to neonates with higher red-blood-cell folate concentrations in cord blood (mean 780 ng/ml; range 402–1670 ng/ml) compared with women (n 7; 12 %) who did not use folic acid supplements (mean 640 ng/ml; range 358–1182 ng/ml; P = 0·05 from Mann–Whitney test for non-parametric comparisons between groups).

Children's neurodevelopment

The children's mental and psychomotor development was assessed at 18 months (±6 weeks) using the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd edition (Bayley-III)(Reference Koutra, Chatzi and Roumeliotaki17). The Bayley-III is an individually administered instrument that assesses the developmental functioning of infants and young children between 1 month and 42 months of age. Its primary purposes are to identify children with developmental delay and to provide information for intervention planning(Reference Bayley18).

The Bayley-III assesses infant and toddler development across three domains: (i) the Cognitive Scale (COG) include items that assess sensorimotor development, exploration and manipulation, object relatedness, concept formation, memory and other aspects of cognitive processing; (ii) the Language Scale is composed of the Receptive Communication (RC) and the Expressive Communication (EC) subtests. The RC subtest includes items that assess preverbal behaviours, vocabulary development, vocabulary related to morphological development and understanding of morphological markers, children's social referencing and verbal comprehension, while the EC subtest includes items that assess preverbal communication, vocabulary development and morpho-syntactic development; (iii) the Motor Scale is divided into the Fine Motor (FM) and the Gross Motor (GM) subtests. In the FM subtest are included fine motor skills associated with apprehension, perceptual–motor integration, motor planning and motor speed, while the GM subtest primarily measures movement of the limbs and torso.

We also used the self-reported Social-Emotional Scale (SE) of Bayley-III. This scale assesses the acquisition of social and emotional milestones in infants and young children that should be achieved by certain ages. It includes items that assess the child's mastery of functional emotional skills, communication needs, the child's ability to engage others and establish relationships, use emotions in an interactive, purposeful manner and use emotional signals or gestures to solve problems.

Neurodevelopmental assessments were conducted by three trained psychologists who completed a formal training course in the use, administration and interpretation of Bayley-III. All testing was done at the Medical School of the University of Crete, two public hospitals in Heraklion, always in the presence of the mother. Total administration time was approximately 90 min.

The examiners also noted critical comments about the difficulties or special conditions of the neurodevelopmental assessment so as to evaluate the ‘quality of assessment’, such as no difficulties, infants participated in the reliability study, difficulties due to physical problems (e.g. physical illness, tiredness, asleep, etc.), or difficulties due to behaviour problems (e.g. nervous, shy, etc.).

For each scale, the child's score was determined by the number of items for which credit was received. We analysed raw scores instead of scaled scores and composite score equivalents because the US reference sample may not be appropriate for children outside the USA. Raw scores were standardised to a mean of 100 with a standard deviation of 15 to homogenise all scales.

Mothers were re-contacted by mail within a month of the assessment and given feedback on their child's performance at the test.

Validation of neurodevelopmental assessment

Twelve children participated in the reliability study of the neurodevelopmental assessment. Each of the psychologists conducted four interviews as examiner while the other two examiners were simultaneously scoring the Bayley-III tests for COG, RC, EC, FM and GM scales as observers, in order to achieve exchangeability. The intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) was used to measure the inter-rater reliability for absolute agreement between scores in a two-way random model. The inter-rater reliabilities were as follows: ICCCOG = 0·990 (P < 0·001), ICCRC = 0·996 (P < 0·001), ICCEC = 0·994 (P < 0·001), ICCFM = 0·924 (P < 0·001) and ICCGM = 0·972 (P < 0·001).

Potential confounders

Potential confounders included characteristics that have an established or potential association with child neurodevelopment and use of folic acid supplements in pregnancy, including: maternal and paternal age; maternal and paternal education (low level, ≤6 years of school; medium level, ≤12 years of school; high level, university or technical college degree); maternal working status in the first year of life (yes/no); maternal pre-pregnancy BMI (kg/m2); maternal and paternal origin (Greek/other); marital status (married/other); maternal smoking during pregnancy (yes/no); maternal alcohol intake during pregnancy (g/d); parity (primiparous; multiparous); type of delivery (Caesarean/vaginal); infant gender (male/female); birth weight (g); gestational age (weeks); preterm birth (yes/no); breast-feeding duration (months); day-care attendance (yes/no), parental smoking in the first year of life (yes/no); home-environment factors (number of people living in the house, number of bedrooms, cleaning practices, indoor domestic pets, presence of mould in the house).

Statistical analysis

Raw scores were standardised for psychologist and child's age at test administration using a parametric method for the estimation of age-specific reference intervals(Reference Royston and Wright19). The parameters of the distribution were modelled as fractional polynomial functions of age and estimated by maximum likelihood. Standardised residuals were then typified having a mean of 100 points with a standard deviation of 15 to homogenise the scales (parameters conventionally used in psychometrics for assessing intelligence quotient).

Univariate associations between the child neurodevelopmental assessment, both raw and standardised scores, and supplement use during early pregnancy were assessed using the χ 2 test for categorical variables, the independent t test for continuous normally distributed variables and the Mann–Whitney U test for continuous non-normally distributed variables. Multivariable linear regression models were implemented to examine the associations between standardised scores and supplement use during pregnancy, after adjusting for several confounders. Separate multivariable models were built having as an outcome each one of the six neurodevelopmental scales (COG, RC, EC, FM, GM and SE). Potential confounders related with the outcomes of interest in the bivariate models at P < 0·2 were included in the multivariable models. Beta coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals in multivariable linear regression models were computed to estimate the degree of association between the exposure and the outcome of interest. The ‘quality of assessment’ and child's gender were treated as a priori confounders and were included in all multivariable regression models. All hypothesis testing was conducted assuming a 0·05 significance level and a two-sided alternative hypothesis. All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software package PASW Statistics 18 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

The majority of women participating in the present study were taking folic acid supplements (n 507, 91·7 %) in early pregnancy. Sociodemographic, medical and lifestyle characteristics of the 553 mother–child pairs according to the use of folic acid supplements are presented in Table 1. Pregnant women who used folic acid supplements were more likely to be highly educated, married and working after birth. The children of women who used folic acid supplements in early pregnancy had longer duration of breast-feeding and were more likely to attend day care compared with children of mothers who did not use folic acid supplements.

Table 1 Sociodemographic, medical and lifestyle characteristics according to folic acid supplement use in early pregnancy, the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study, Crete, Greece (n 553)

P value from Pearson's χ 2 test for categorical variables and the Kruskal–Wallis test or ANOVA for continuous variables, for comparison between non-users and users of folic acid at a dose of 5 mg/d and >5 mg/d.

‡Values are presented as mean and standard deviation.

Sixty-eight per cent of women (n 373) reported daily intake of 5 mg folic acid from supplements (5 mg/d), while 24 % (n 134) reported higher doses of folic acid (>5 mg/d), and 8 % (n 46) reported no use of folic acid supplements in early pregnancy. Women with daily folic acid intakes of 5 mg were more likely to take one daily dose of supplemental folic acid, while women with higher intakes reported more than one daily dose.

Table 2 shows the unadjusted and adjusted associations between supplementary folic acid use in early pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age. In the univariate analysis, daily intake of 5 mg folic acid from supplements was associated with increases of 5·4 units in the scale of receptive communication, 4·2 units in the scale of expressive communication and 4·7 units in the scale of fine motor development. After adjustment for maternal age, education, parity, breast-feeding duration, day-care attendance, birth weight, infant gender and quality of assessment, significant associations remained only for receptive communication and expressive communication. Daily intakes of supplemental folic acid higher than 5 mg were not associated with additional increase in the mental and psychomotor scores.

Table 2 Association between folic acid intake from supplements in early pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age (n 553), the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study, Crete, Greece

Bayely-III, Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd edition.

*Coefficient was significant at P< 0·05.

β and 95 % CI are estimated using linear regression models adjusting for quality of assessment and child gender. Reference category is women who did not use folic acid supplements in pregnancy.

β and 95 % CI are estimated using linear regression models adjusting for maternal age, education, parity, breast-feeding duration, day-care attendance, birth weight, infant gender and quality of assessment. Reference category is women who did not use folic acid supplements in pregnancy.

To test the possibility of confounding by prematurity we performed additional analyses excluding all preterm births (n 56, 10 %), and results remained similar to the original analysis (Table 3).

Table 3 The association between folic acid intake from supplements in early pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age, after excluding preterm births (n 56), the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study, Crete, Greece

Bayely-III, Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd edition.

*Coefficient was significant at P< 0·05.

β and 95 % CI are estimated using linear regression models adjusting for quality of assessment and child gender. Reference category is women who did not use folic acid supplements in pregnancy.

β and 95 % CI are estimated using linear regression models adjusting for maternal age, education, parity, breast-feeding duration, day-care attendance, birth weight, infant gender and quality of assessment. Reference category is women who did not use folic acid supplements in pregnancy.

Discussion

In the prospective, population-based ‘Rhea’ mother–child cohort study we showed that daily intakes of 5 mg supplemental folic acid in early pregnancy were associated with improvement in offspring's receptive and expressive communication skills at 18 months of age. No additional improvement was observed with excessive doses of folic acid supplementation.

These results are in accordance with two other birth cohort studies in The Netherlands and Spain evaluating the effect of folic supplementation in the first trimester of pregnancy on children's neurodevelopment, which showed positive associations with behavioural development at 18 months of age(Reference Roza, van Batenburg-Eddes and Steegers10) and verbal and verbal-executive function abilities at 4 years of age(Reference Julvez, Fortuny and Mendez9), respectively. In contrast to these findings, another longitudinal study assessing folic acid status late in pregnancy found no significant associations with children's neurodevelopment at 5 years of age(Reference Tamura, Goldenberg and Chapman15). This discrepancy may be explained by the time of measuring exposure and the follow-up of a non-population-based sample of educationally and environmentally deprived women in the latter study.

Animal studies have linked increased cognitive performance with folic acid intake during pregnancy(Reference Joshi, Rao and Girigosavi20), while others have focused on the adverse neurochemical and behavioural effects of folate restriction during gestation(Reference Middaugh, Grover and Blackwell21Reference Troen, Chao and Crivello23). Findings in folate-deficient rodents include ultrastructural abnormalities in rat brain capillaries(Reference Kim, Lee and Chang24) and slowed neurogenesis in mouse brain(Reference Kruman, Mouton and Emokpae25).

Folic acid is critically important for fetal development as it is required for cell division because of its role in DNA synthesis(Reference Tamura and Picciano26). It acts also as a cofactor for many essential cellular reactions including the transfer of single-carbon units, and it is a substrate for a variety of reactions that affect the metabolism of several amino acids, including the transmethylation and transsulfuration pathways(Reference Bailey and Gregory27). Folic acid is important in neurogenesis, in cell growth and proliferation, and in myelination(Reference Greenblatt, Huffman and Reiss4). Craciunescu et al. have shown that dietary folic acid availability affects mouse brain development, especially the neocortex which is the part of the brain responsible for complex behaviours such as cognition, attention and social competence, long after neural tube closure by influencing cell mitosis and apoptosis in the brain(Reference Craciunescu, Brown and Mar8). Additionally, folic acid is involved in processes of the synthesis of dopamine and serotonin, neurotransmitters that affect behavioural and cognitive functions(Reference Fernstrom28).

A rising body of evidence suggests epigenetic modification as a plausible mechanism that leads to neuropsychiatric conditions(Reference Bassett, Chow and Weksberg29). It has been hypothesised that aberrant DNA methylation might be implicated within the aetiology of psychotic disorders through abnormal neurodevelopment(Reference Suzuki and Bird30, Reference Singh, Murphy and O'Reilly31). The fidelity of DNA methylation patterns crucially depends on the regulation of the folate and one-carbon cycle(Reference McNulty and Scott32) and folate is critically involved in processes of methylation and hydroxylation which appear to be of particular importance for neurotransmission. Thus, the pathways underlying the association between folic acid and brain development involve several genetic, biochemical and biological factors.

An important finding in the present analysis is that all women had daily intakes of folic acid from supplements higher than the tolerable upper intake level for adults (1 mg/d), while no additional protection was observed with doses of folic acid supplementation higher than 5 mg/d. This high dose is mainly due to the fact that the majority of women (n 1096, 85·7 %) used only one product providing 5 mg of folic acid per dose. This specific product is distributed in Greece and Malta and contains 5 mg of folic acid per tablet. Similarly, a previous study in Greece examining the total intake of micronutrients in pregnant women in Greece revealed that the total intake of folate was about 3000 μg/d(Reference Petrakos, Panagopoulos and Koutras33). These intakes are extremely high compared with the estimates from other European countries(Reference Timmermans, Jaddoe and Hofman34, Reference Pastor-Valero, Navarrete-Munoz and Rebagliato35). Although folic acid fortification has been successful in reducing the incidence of neural tube defects(Reference Williams, Rasmussen and Flores36, Reference Boulet, Yang and Mai37), the consequences of long-term high folate intake are not known. High doses of folic acid supplementation have been associated with the presence of plasma unmetabolised folic acid(Reference Sweeney, Staines and Daly38) and recent studies have linked high folate intake and increased plasma concentrations to a number of adverse effects(Reference Morris, Evans and Bienias39Reference Lawrance, Deng and Rozen43). Moreover, excessive folic acid intake (15 mg/d) has been associated with behavioural changes such as irritability, over-activity and altered sleep patterns in volunteer subjects(Reference Greenblatt, Huffman and Reiss4). Because of the potential epigenetic effects of folic acid supplementation on the genome of the offspring, further studies are needed to investigate the effects of prenatal high doses of supplemental folic acid in children's mental and psychomotor development.

Strengths of the present study include the population-based prospective design and the high participation rate (90 %). The Bayley-III that was used for the neurodevelopmental assessment of children is recognised internationally as one of the most comprehensive tools to assess children starting as young as 1 month old, identify infant and toddler strengths and competencies, as well as their weaknesses, and provide a valid and reliable measure of a child's mental and psychomotor abilities(Reference Hoyo, Murtha and Schildkraut44). The exclusion of women who gave birth to twins as well as the adjustment for several sociodemographic variables reduced the likelihood of confounding. The study population included women from the follow-up of a birth cohort, providing the opportunity to account for the effect of exposures during pregnancy prospectively within the cohort. We did not observe any substantial differences between the crude and the adjusted models. Thus, it is unlikely that over-adjustment affected our findings. Participants were unaware of the hypothesis being tested, so misclassification of folic acid supplement intake estimated by the questionnaire is likely to be random with respect to neurodevelopmental outcomes.

There are several limitations in the present study that deserve acknowledgement. We could not distinguish between mothers who took supplements preconceptionally from those who started once pregnancy was detected. The use of questionnaires to assess micronutrient supplementation may have led to the misclassification of exposure, despite the clear question concerning supplements. However, the validation study showed good level of agreement between the estimates from the questionnaire and red-blood-cell folate concentrations in cord blood samples. Unfortunately, we were not able to perform multivariable linear regression analysis with red-blood-cell folate levels and neurodevelopmental outcome parameters due to small numbers. We aimed to assess folic acid supplementation in early pregnancy to minimise possible recall bias, although we had no information on the duration of folic acid supplementation. The majority of women used folic acid supplements alone or in combination with iron, while a very small proportion of mothers reported the use of multivitamins in early pregnancy. Consequently, the possibility of confounding by intake of other macro- and micronutrients that could affect folate metabolism is minimised. Finally, use of supplements in pregnancy is related to socio-economic status, lifestyle factors (dietary habits) and adverse health behaviours (smoking). Although we incorporated extensive information on potential social and environmental factors that are associated with children's neurodevelopment, residual confounding cannot be excluded. For example, maternal intellectual ability, children's nutritional status at the time of testing, postnatal data on multivitamin use and growth curves of the children up to the postnatal study moment may well serve as additional confounding factors. Although the information on children's dietary intake as well as postnatal growth data were collected within the ‘Rhea’ cohort, the data are not available yet for analysis.

Conclusions

The present study suggests that high doses of folic acid supplementation in early pregnancy may be associated with enhanced vocabulary development, communicational skills and verbal comprehension at 18 months of age. Future longitudinal studies are needed to confirm these findings, better understand the complex underlying processes and examine the effect of high doses of folic acid supplements during pregnancy on neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism in later childhood.

Acknowledgements

This work was partly supported by the EU Integrated Projects ENVIROGENOMARKERS (7th Framework Programme, Contract no. FP7-ENV-2008-226756) and CHICOS (7th Framework Programme, Contract no. Health-F2-2009-241604). The authors declare no conflict of interest. All authors made a substantial contribution to the study and they have all personally reviewed and approved the submitted manuscript. Specifically: L.C. conceived the study, conducted the data analysis and prepared the manuscript; E.P. contributed to the design of the study and the drafting of the manuscript; K.K. implemented the measures of neurodevelopment used in the study, reviewed and provided key input to the manuscript; T.R. and V.G. carried out the statistical analysis and helped with the data interpretation and manuscript preparation; N.S., V.L., M. Karachaliou and M.V. supervised the data collection and helped with the analysis of supplements and manuscript preparation; M. Kogevinas supervised the data collection, provided critical review of the manuscript and helped with data interpretation and manuscript preparation. The authors would particularly like to thank all of the cohort participants for their generous collaboration.

References

1.Gluckman, PD, Hanson, MA & Beedle, AS (2007) Early life events and their consequences for later disease: a life history and evolutionary perspective. Am J Hum Biol 19, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Benton, D (2008) Micronutrient status, cognition and behavioral problems in childhood. Eur J Nutr 47, Suppl. 3, 3850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Black, MM (2008) Effects of vitamin B12 and folate deficiency on brain development in children. Food Nutr Bull 29, 2 Suppl., S126S131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Greenblatt, JM, Huffman, LC & Reiss, AL (1994) Folic acid in neurodevelopment and child psychiatry. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 18, 647660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Scholl, TO & Johnson, WG (2000) Folic acid: influence on the outcome of pregnancy. Am J Clin Nutr 71, 5 Suppl., 1295S1303S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Lumley, J, Watson, L, Watson, M et al. (2001) Periconceptional supplementation with folate and/or multivitamins for preventing neural tube defects. Cochrane Database Syst Rev issue 3, CD001056.Google Scholar
7.Lee, JI, Lee, JA & Lim, HS (2005) Effect of time of initiation and dose of prenatal iron and folic acid supplementation on iron and folate nutriture of Korean women during pregnancy. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 843849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Craciunescu, CN, Brown, EC, Mar, MH et al. (2004) Folic acid deficiency during late gestation decreases progenitor cell proliferation and increases apoptosis in fetal mouse brain. J Nutr 134, 162166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Julvez, J, Fortuny, J, Mendez, M et al. (2009) Maternal use of folic acid supplements during pregnancy and four-year-old neurodevelopment in a population-based birth cohort. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 23, 199206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10.Roza, SJ, van Batenburg-Eddes, T, Steegers, EAP et al. (2010) Maternal folic acid supplement use in early pregnancy and child behavioural problems: The Generation R Study. Br J Nutr 103, 445452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Schlotz, W, Jones, A, Phillips, DIW et al. (2010) Lower maternal folate status in early pregnancy is associated with childhood hyperactivity and peer problems in offspring. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51, 594602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Veena, SR, Krishnaveni, GV, Srinivasan, K et al. (2010) Higher maternal plasma folate but not vitamin B-12 concentrations during pregnancy are associated with better cognitive function scores in 9- to 10-year-old children in South India. J Nutr 140, 10141022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Wehby, GL & Murray, JC (2008) The effects of prenatal use of folic acid and other dietary supplements on early child development. Matern Child Health J 12, 180187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Dobo, M & Czeizel, AE (1998) Long-term somatic and mental development of children after periconceptional multivitamin supplementation. Eur J Pediatr 157, 719723.Google ScholarPubMed
15.Tamura, T, Goldenberg, RL, Chapman, VR et al. (2005) Folate status of mothers during pregnancy and mental and psychomotor development of their children at five years of age. Pediatrics 116, 703708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Chatzi, L, Plana, E, Daraki, V et al. (2009) Metabolic syndrome in early pregnancy and risk of preterm birth. Am J Epidemiol 170, 829836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Koutra, K, Chatzi, L, Roumeliotaki, T et al. (2012) Socio-demographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother–Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev 35, 4859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Bayley, N (2006) Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd ed. [PsychCorp, editor]. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc.Google Scholar
19.Royston, P & Wright, EM (1998) How to construct ‘normal ranges’ for fetal variables. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 11, 3038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Joshi, S, Rao, S, Girigosavi, S et al. (2004) Differential effects of fish oil and folic acid supplementation during pregnancy in rats on cognitive performance and serum glucose in their offspring. Nutrition 20, 465472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Middaugh, LD, Grover, TA, Blackwell, LA et al. (1976) Neurochemical and behavioral effects of diet related perinatal folic acid restriction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 5, 129134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Ferguson, SA, Berry, KJ, Hansen, DK et al. (2005) Behavioral effects of prenatal folate deficiency in mice. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol 73, 249252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Troen, AM, Chao, WH, Crivello, NA et al. (2008) Cognitive impairment in folate-deficient rats corresponds to depleted brain phosphatidylcholine and is prevented by dietary methionine without lowering plasma homocysteine. J Nutr 138, 25022509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Kim, JM, Lee, H & Chang, N (2002) Hyperhomocysteinemia due to short-term folate deprivation is related to electron microscopic changes in the rat brain. J Nutr 132, 34183421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Kruman, II, Mouton, PR, Emokpae, R Jr et al. (2005) Folate deficiency inhibits proliferation of adult hippocampal progenitors. Neuroreport 16, 10551059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Tamura, T & Picciano, MF (2006) Folate and human reproduction. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 9931016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Bailey, LB & Gregory, JF 3rd (1999) Folate metabolism and requirements. J Nutr 129, 779782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Fernstrom, JD (2000) Can nutrient supplements modify brain function? Am J Clin Nutr 71, 6 Suppl., S1669S1675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Bassett, AS, Chow, EW, Weksberg, R et al. (2002) Schizophrenia and genetics: new insights. Curr Psychiatry Rep 4, 307314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Suzuki, MM & Bird, A (2008) DNA methylation landscapes: provocative insights from epigenomics. Nat Rev Genet 9, 465476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Singh, SM, Murphy, B & O'Reilly, RL (2003) Involvement of gene–diet/drug interaction in DNA methylation and its contribution to complex diseases: from cancer to schizophrenia. Clin Genet 64, 451460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.McNulty, H & Scott, JM (2008) Intake and status of folate and related B-vitamins: considerations and challenges in achieving optimal status. Br J Nutr 99, Suppl. 3, S48S54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.Petrakos, G, Panagopoulos, P, Koutras, I et al. (2006) A comparison of the dietary and total intake of micronutrients in a group of pregnant Greek women with the Dietary Reference Intakes. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol 127, 166171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34.Timmermans, S, Jaddoe, VWV, Hofman, A et al. (2009) Periconception folic acid supplementation, fetal growth and the risks of low birth weight and preterm birth: the Generation R Study. Br J Nutr 102, 777785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Pastor-Valero, M, Navarrete-Munoz, EM, Rebagliato, M et al. (2011) Periconceptional folic acid supplementation and anthropometric measures at birth in a cohort of pregnant women in Valencia, Spain. Br J Nutr 105, 13521360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36.Williams, LJ, Rasmussen, SA, Flores, A et al. (2005) Decline in the prevalence of spina bifida and anencephaly by race/ethnicity: 1995–2002. Pediatrics 116, 580586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37.Boulet, SL, Yang, Q, Mai, C et al. (2008) Trends in the postfortification prevalence of spina bifida and anencephaly in the United States. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol 82, 527532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38.Sweeney, MR, Staines, A, Daly, L et al. (2009) Persistent circulating unmetabolised folic acid in a setting of liberal voluntary folic acid fortification. Implications for further mandatory fortification? BMC Public Health 9, 295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39.Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Bienias, JL et al. (2005) Dietary folate and vitamin B12 intake and cognitive decline among community-dwelling older persons. Arch Neurol 62, 641645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Troen, AM, Mitchell, B, Sorensen, B et al. (2006) Unmetabolized folic acid in plasma is associated with reduced natural killer cell cytotoxicity among postmenopausal women. J Nutr 136, 189194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Cole, BF, Baron, JA, Sandler, RS et al. (2007) Folic acid for the prevention of colorectal adenomas: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA 297, 23512359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Yajnik, CS, Deshpande, SS, Jackson, AA et al. (2008) Vitamin B12 and folate concentrations during pregnancy and insulin resistance in the offspring: the Pune Maternal Nutrition Study. Diabetologia 51, 2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Lawrance, AK, Deng, L & Rozen, R (2009) Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase deficiency and low dietary folate reduce tumorigenesis in Apc min/+ mice. Gut 58, 805811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Hoyo, C, Murtha, AP, Schildkraut, JM et al. (2011) Folic acid supplementation before and during pregnancy in the Newborn Epigenetics STudy (NEST). BMC Public Health 11, 46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow diagram defining the participants in the analysis

Figure 1

Table 1 Sociodemographic, medical and lifestyle characteristics according to folic acid supplement use in early pregnancy, the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study, Crete, Greece (n 553)

Figure 2

Table 2 Association between folic acid intake from supplements in early pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age (n 553), the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study, Crete, Greece

Figure 3

Table 3 The association between folic acid intake from supplements in early pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at 18 months of age, after excluding preterm births (n 56), the mother–child cohort ‘Rhea’ study, Crete, Greece