Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T19:46:27.409Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Diet quality and cognitive function in an urban sample: findings from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2016

Regina S Wright*
Affiliation:
School of Nursing, University of Delaware, 25 N. College Avenue, Newark, DE 19716, USA
Shari R Waldstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD, USA Geriatric Research Education & Clinical Center, Baltimore Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Baltimore, MD, USA
Marie Fanelli Kuczmarski
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Health and Nutrition, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
Ryan T Pohlig
Affiliation:
Biostatistics Core, College of Health Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
Constance S Gerassimakis
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Health and Nutrition, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
Beatrice Gaynor
Affiliation:
School of Nursing, University of Delaware, 25 N. College Avenue, Newark, DE 19716, USA
Michele K Evans
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
Alan B Zonderman
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Epidemiology and Population Sciences, National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Poor diet quality contributes to morbidity, including poor brain health outcomes such as cognitive decline and dementia. African Americans and individuals living in poverty may be at greater risk for cognitive decrements from poor diet quality.

Design

Cross-sectional.

Setting

Baltimore, MD, USA.

Subjects

Participants were 2090 African Americans and Whites (57 % female, mean age=47·9 years) who completed two 24 h dietary recalls. We examined cognitive performance and potential interactions of diet quality with race and poverty status using baseline data from the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study. Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) scores were calculated and interpreted using federal guidelines. A neurocognitive test battery was administered to evaluate cognitive function over several domains.

Results

Linear regression analyses showed that lower HEI-2010 scores were associated with poorer verbal learning and memory (P<0·05) after adjustment for covariates. Diet quality within the sample was poor. Significant interactions of HEI-2010 and poverty status (all P<0·05) indicated that higher diet quality was associated with higher performance on tests of attention and cognitive flexibility, visuospatial ability and perceptual speed among those below the poverty line. No significant race interactions emerged. Higher diet quality was associated with better performance on two measures of verbal learning and memory, irrespective of race and poverty status.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that diet quality and cognitive function are likely related at the population level. Future research is needed to determine whether the association is clinically significant.

Type
Research Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2016 

Optimal cognitive function is an important indicator of quality of life and necessary for independent living across the lifespan( Reference Royall, Palmer and Chiodo 1 ). Multiple indices of poor health have been associated with decrements in cognitive function( Reference Waldstein and Wendell 2 ). Health effects on interindividual variability in cognitive function have been documented across the lifespan and health-related decrements in cognitive function( Reference Waldstein 3 ), as detected by sensitive neuropsychological tests, are apparent in some middle-aged or younger adults( Reference Salthouse 4 , Reference Singh-Manoux, Kivimaki and Glymour 5 ). These early decrements to cognitive function may negatively impact later-life cognitive outcomes( Reference Waldstein 3 ). Poor health behaviours, such as unhealthy eating, are also associated with worse cognitive outcomes and promote the onset and progression of cognitive decline and dementia( Reference Gillette-Guyonnet, Van Kan and Andrieu 6 ). Among Americans, diet quality is strikingly poor, with only 3 to 4 % of US adults meeting federal dietary guidelines( Reference King 7 ). Poor eating among the majority of US adults suggests that the implications of a diet and cognitive function link may be widespread.

Growing evidence suggests poor diet quality is associated with reduced cognitive function and cognitive decline. For example, high intakes of saturated and trans-unsaturated fats are associated with cognitive decline and risk of dementia( Reference Gillette-Guyonnet, Van Kan and Andrieu 6 ). Other evidence suggests high energy intake and habitual sugar intake may be deleterious to cognitive function( Reference Creavin, Gallacher and Pickering 8 , Reference Ye, Gao and Scott 9 ). In contrast, high diet quality has been shown to protect or enhance cognitive function. In that regard, a high-quality diet such as the Mediterranean diet, characterized by high intakes of vegetables, fruit, legumes, nuts, cereals and monounsaturated fat (e.g. olive oil), and moderate to low intakes of dairy, meat, poultry, fish and alcohol, is protective against cognitive decline, cognitive impairment and dementia( Reference Lourida, Soni and Thompson-Coon 10 ). Other evidence suggests specific benefits of the Mediterranean diet for global cognition( Reference Kuczmarski, Allegro and Stave 11 ), verbal memory, verbal fluency( Reference Martínez-Lapiscina, Clavero and Toledo 12 ) and visual memory performance( Reference Berr, Portet and Carriere 13 ). Similarly, intake of vitamin E, an antioxidant, has been associated with enhanced verbal memory and verbal fluency performance and psychomotor speed among women( Reference Beydoun, Fanelli-Kuczmarski and Kitner-Triolo 14 ).

Prior research suggests that diet quality, measured by either nutrient- or food-based indices, varies as a function of race( Reference Hiza, Casavale and Guenther 15 ). For example, in several studies, African Americans report poorer diet quality than other racial/ethnic groups( Reference Sijtsma, Meyer and Steffen 16 ). Poorer diet quality among African Americans as compared with other races is associated with lower levels of education and literacy( Reference Raffensperger, Kuczmarski and Hotchkiss 17 ), reduced access to and unavailability of healthful foods( Reference Larson, Story and Nelson 18 ), and cultural differences in food preferences, including a propensity for consuming high-energy, fatty foods( Reference Moser, Green and Weber 19 ). African Americans may also be more likely to engage in maladaptive eating behaviours to cope with psychosocial stress and perceived racism that are unique to their social experiences( Reference Mwendwa, Gholson and Sims 20 ).

Individuals living in poverty tend to consume poorer diets due to socio-economic and environmental barriers to healthful food consumption( Reference Darmon and Drewnowski 21 ). Socio-economic disadvantage is consistently associated with lower consumption of fruits and vegetables and higher consumption of energy-dense foods( Reference Darmon and Drewnowski 21 ). Inversely, researchers have found that as income increases, fruit and vegetable consumption tends to increase, improving the healthfulness of dietary patterns( Reference Kirkpatrick, Dodd and Reedy 22 ). There are several explanations for the inverse association between diet quality and socio-economic status (SES). For example, the economic stratification of neighbourhoods tends to segregate lower-SES and higher-SES individuals, creating very different environments for consumption( Reference Gordon-Larsen, Nelson and Page 23 ). Individuals in economically disadvantaged neighbourhoods often have limited access to retail outlets that offer healthful foods coupled with greater exposure to advertisements that promote unhealthful food options( Reference Larson, Story and Nelson 18 ). In addition, home food environments, including the structure of meals and food availability constrained by lesser resources, play a role in poor diet quality among individuals of low SES( Reference Ranjit, Wilkinson and Lytle 24 ).

Given the documented associations between diet quality and cognitive function, identifying cognitive domains that may be affected, as well as the profiles of individuals most at risk for cognitive decrements related to poor diet quality, is imperative. Since diet quality tends to be poorer among African Americans and individuals living in poverty as compared with other groups, it is possible that diet confers a greater influence on cognitive function in these groups, making them particularly vulnerable to cognitive decline and dementia. The primary aim of the current study was to examine cross-sectional relationships of diet quality, measured with the Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010), with several domains of cognitive function. A secondary aim was to examine whether relationships of diet quality with cognitive function vary by race and poverty status. To date, we are unaware of any studies that have examined these associations in a racially and socio-economically diverse sample of adults.

Methods

Data source

The Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity Across the Life Span (HANDLS) study is a 20-year prospective longitudinal study examining health disparities among African American and White adults in thirteen urban neighbourhoods of Baltimore City, MD, USA. Participants include a fixed cohort of 3720 community-dwelling White and African American men and women, initially 30–64 years of age, who are above or below 125 % of the 2004 Department of Health and Human Services poverty guideline. Participants were sampled from the thirteen neighbourhoods using predetermined contiguous census tracts of an area probability sample. Stratified random sampling was intended to include a range of socio-economic levels in a four-way factorial cross for age (seven 5-year age groups), both sexes, race (White, African American) and SES. The HANDLS study is currently collecting wave 4 data; data collection began in 2004. Only wave 1 data, collected between 2004 and 2009, were analysed for the current cross-sectional study. Wave 1 occurred in two phases. In phase 1, residential households were identified within selected neighbourhoods. Field interviewers identified eligible candidates at doorstep interviews and invited participation in HANDLS. After signing informed consents, participants completed a household questionnaire and the first of two 24 h dietary recall surveys. Trained professionals working in mobile research vehicles collected the comprehensive data for phase 2. Measurements included the second dietary recall, a battery of cognitive and neuropsychological tests, and extensive medical history and physical examinations. The mobile research vehicles were located in selected neighbourhoods where participants resided.

Overall inclusion criteria for HANDLS were ages 30–64 years, ability to give informed consent, ability to perform at least five study measures and possession of a valid picture ID. Exclusion criteria included: (i) pregnancy; (ii) having received chemotherapy, radiation or biological cancer treatment within the past 6 months; (iii) self-reported AIDS; and (iv) presence of uncontrolled blood pressure (>160/100 mmHg). For more detailed procedures see Evans et al.( Reference Evans, Lepkowski and Powe 25 ).

Participants

A total of 2801 participants completed both phases 1 and 2. Among these participants, eighty were excluded due to HIV infection, sixty were excluded due to a history of stroke, sixty-nine were excluded due to transient ischaemic attacks, thirty were excluded due to low vitamin B12 (<200 pg/ml) and folate levels (<3·0 mg/ml), 124 showed probable cognitive impairment as indicated by a Mini-Mental Status Examination score <20( Reference Wood, Giuliano and Bignell 26 ), and 348 additional participants were excluded due to at least one missing dietary recall between the two visits. These exclusions resulted in a final sample of 2090 participants for analysis.

Diet quality measures

Participants were asked to recall all the foods and beverages consumed during the previous 24 h by trained interviewers on two occasions: during phase 1 (in-home interview) and phase 2 (mobile research vehicle visit). Interviewers recorded dietary recalls using the validated US Department of Agriculture Automated Multiple Pass Method( Reference Conway, Ingwersen and Moshfegh 27 , Reference Conway, Ingwersen and Vinyard 28 ). Measurement aids including the Food Model Booklet, spoons, cups and a ruler were used to assist the participant in estimating accurate quantities consumed( Reference McBride 29 ). Foods and beverages reported in the dietary recalls were coded with their own unique food codes by trained nutritional professionals using the Food and Nutrient Database for Dietary Studies (FNDDS) version 3.0( 30 ).

The HEI-2010, a recognized measure of diet quality based on compliance with the US 2010 Dietary Guidelines for Americans (DGA), was used in the present study( Reference Guenther, Casavale and Reedy 31 , 32 ). Compliance was measured according to recommendations for types and amounts of food to consume from two perspectives: adequacy and moderation. Adequacy components included Total Fruit, Whole Fruit, Total Vegetables, Greens and Beans, Whole Grains, Dairy, Total Protein Foods, Seafood and Plant Proteins, and Fatty Acids. Moderation components included Refined Grains, Sodium, and Empty Calories. Scoring used a density-based approach, based on g/4184 kJ (1000 kcal) or percentage of energy, with ratios for fatty acids. Standards for maximum scores employed the least-restrictive recommendations, among those that vary by sex, age or energy intake. Adequacy and moderation component scores are summed to create the HEI-2010 total score. A detailed description of the calculation of the HEI-2010 scores for participants in the HANDLS study can be found on the HANDLS website( 33 ). The maximum possible HEI-2010 total score is 100. A higher HEI-2010 score corresponds to higher diet quality and better compliance to the DGA 2010( Reference Guenther, Casavale and Reedy 31 ). Thus, adequacy components receive the maximum number of points when intakes are at the recommended level or higher. Inversely, moderation components receive the maximum number of points when intake levels are at the standard or lower( Reference Guenther, Casavale and Reedy 31 ). To date there are no specific criteria for score interpretation by component. Overall, scores that are closer to 100 are optimal.

Cognitive measures

Verbal learning and memory

The California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) assesses verbal learning and memory( Reference Delis, Kramer and Kaplan 34 ). The standard format assesses immediate and delayed recall and recognition of two sixteen-word lists. Recall of list A consists of five trials. Recall of list B takes place after list A and allows one trial. Following list B, short-delay free recall and cued recall of list A are tested. After a 20 to 30 min delay, long-delay free recall, long-delay cued recall and yes/no-recognition attempts of list A conclude the test. Total recall of list A, short-delay free recall, long-delay free recall and learning slope performance were assessed.

Non-verbal memory

The Benton Visual Retention Test assesses visual memory, visual perception and visuoconstructional abilities as participants view ten designs and reproduce each design, as precisely as possible, from memory( Reference Benton 35 ). The test is untimed and scored by the examiner. Test–retest reliability ranges from 0·74 to 0·84( Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen 36 ).

Working memory

The Digit Span subscale of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition consists of a forward and backward test of attention, working memory and concentration( Reference Wechsler 37 ). In both tests, seven pairs of random number sequences, of increasing length, are presented verbally at a rate of one per second. In Digits Forward, the subject repeats the same number sequence after the examiner. In Digits Backward the participant is asked to repeat the number sequence in reverse order. The test is discontinued when the subject fails both the forward and backward trial of any given sequence string. Digits Forward and Backward are measured by the number of correct trials.

Attention and cognitive flexibility

The Trail Making Test (TMT) measures attention, scanning and visuomotor tracking, and cognitive flexibility( Reference Reitan 38 ). The TMT is administered in two parts that are timed, lasting 5 to 10 min. TMT A is administered first. Subjects draw lines to connect, in ascending order, numbered circles randomly arranged on a page( Reference Reitan 38 ). In TMT B, subjects connect numbered and lettered circles, alternating between number and letter in ascending order by drawing connecting lines. Each test requires that the correct sequence be followed in order to correctly complete the test. Cognitive task demands in TMT B are greater than in TMT A; slower performance in TMT A appears to underlie deficits in TMT B( Reference Reitan 38 ). Scores reflect time taken to complete the tasks; thus, higher scores reflect poorer performance. The test–retest coefficient for TMT A and TMT B is 0·79 and 0·89, respectively.

Visuospatial ability

The Card Rotations Test assesses visuospatial ability by testing competence in visualizing differences in card shapes( Reference Ekstrom, French and Harman 39 ). Ten rows of eight cards each are compared with a sample card shape to determine if each card is rotated or flipped over, compared with the sample. Two sets of ten card rows must be completed in 3 min each. The score is the number marked correctly minus those marked incorrectly( Reference Ekstrom, French and Harman 39 ).

Perceptual speed

The Identical Pictures Test, also from the ETS Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests, assesses three components of visual perceptual speed: perceptual fluency, decision speed and immediate perceptual memory. In this timed test, sample objects are matched with an identical picture in the adjacent row of test objects( Reference Ekstrom, French and Harman 39 ). The score is the number of correct answers, minus a fraction of the number of incorrect answers.

Semantic fluency

The Semantic Fluency Test is used to assess spontaneous generation of words that correspond to a specific category in a set amount of time( Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen 36 ). In this case, participants are told to generate as many animals as possible within 60 s. Afterwards, the total number of unique animals named is summed to generate a categorical animal fluency score. Test–retest reliability for the Semantic Fluency Test is sound, with coefficients greater than 0·70( Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen 36 ).

Covariates

Covariates included sociodemographic variables, depressive symptoms, health behaviours/lifestyle factors, cardiovascular and anthropometric measures, and haemodynamic measures. Covariates were selected based on significant correlations with diet quality and/or cognitive function and typical inclusion in the relevant literature. Variables that were tested as potential covariates, but were unrelated to diet quality or cognitive performance, included past alcohol use, past and current illicit drug use, and total serum cholesterol.

Sociodemographic variables

Education was assessed as the total number of years of formal education. Race was dichotomized by self-identification as African American or White. Poverty status was dichotomized using the US Census Bureau poverty thresholds for 2004 based on income, size of family and related children under age 18 years. Poverty status was determined at the doorstep by asking the number of people in the household and then inquiring whether the household income was greater or less than the threshold for that household size. Poverty status is hereafter referred to as ‘below the poverty threshold’ (<125 % of the poverty threshold) or ‘above the poverty threshold’ (≥125 % of the poverty threshold)( 40 ).

Depressive symptoms

Depressive symptomatology was assessed with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), a depressive symptomatology scale designed for non-clinical populations( Reference Radloff 41 ).

Health behaviours/lifestyle factors

The smoking status variable was dichotomized as 0=not current user (i.e. never tried, never used regularly or former users) and 1=current user. Alcohol status was coded as 0=not current user (i.e. never tried, never used regularly or used >6 months ago) or 1=used within the past 6 months. Lastly, hard drug use, defined as the use of marijuana, cocaine or opiates, was coded as 0=not current user or 1=used in the past 6 months.

Cardiovascular and anthropometric measures

A diagnosis of diabetes was documented by a fasting glucose value ≥126 mg/dl, evidence of prescribed diabetic medication or a self-reported history of diabetes. Height and weight were obtained using calibrated equipment, and BMI was computed as the ratio of weight to height-squared (kg/m2).

Haemodynamic measures

The standard brachial artery auscultation method, following a 5 min rest in a seated position, was implemented to assess systolic and diastolic blood pressure measurements (SBP and DBP, respectively). Measurements were obtained on each arm, with the arm at a 90° angle from the body with the palm facing upward, with an appropriate cuff size. Two SBP readings were averaged and two DBP readings were averaged to yield an estimate of participants’ resting SBP and DBP. Since SBP and DBP are highly correlated, systolic hypertension is more common among older adults than diastolic hypertension, and our sample was more likely to have elevated SBP, not elevated DBP, only SBP was included in the regression analyses to avoid multicollinearity.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were computed using the statistical software package IBM SPSS Statistics Version 22.0. All variables were assessed for outliers, homoscedasticity and assumptions of normality. To determine relationships between diet quality and neurocognitive performance, sequential linear regressions were run for each of the neurocognitive outcome variables with HEI-2010 score as the main predictor. All outcomes were analysed as continuous variables. Interaction terms were created to assess race and poverty status as effect modifiers. Variables were entered into the models in the following order: block 1=age, race, sex, education, poverty status, CES-D score, current alcohol use (yes/no), current cigarette smoker (yes/no), BMI, mean SBP and diabetes status; block 2=HEI-2010 score; and block 3=HEI-2010 score×race interaction term and HEI-2010 score×poverty interaction term.

Results

Sample characteristics

Descriptive statistics for all demographic characteristics, covariates and cognitive measures are found in Table 1. The mean age of participants (n 2090) was 47·85 (sd 9·22) years. The sample was comprised of 51·2 % African Americans and 42·8 % males. Mean educational attainment was 12·16 (sd 2·89) years. Forty-two per cent of the sample fell below the poverty line. Mean BMI of 30·26 (sd 8·03) kg/m2 was in the obese range. Mean SBP of 120·80 (sd 17·79) mmHg was at the lower limit of pre-hypertension, and 16·5 % had a diagnosis of diabetes. Self-reports of current illicit drug use, alcohol use and cigarette use were 5·9, 52·5 and 49·1 %, respectively. HEI-2010 scores were approximately 43 out of 100, reflecting poor compliance with the DGA 2010 and thus poor diet quality. There were no significant differences in HEI-2010 scores by race or poverty status.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for sample characteristics and cognitive test scores for the total sample and poverty status groups: White and African American men and women (n 2090) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

SBP, systolic blood pressure; HEI-2010, Healthy Eating Index-2010; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test-II; BVRT errors, Benton Visual Retention Test total errors; TMT, Trail Making Test.

Mean value or percentage is significantly higher: *P<0·05, **P<0·01, ***P<0·001.

Sequential linear regression findings

Unadjusted associations between HEI-2010 scores and cognitive test scores are provided in Table 2. Prior to adjustment, HEI-2010 scores were significantly and positively correlated with CVLT-II list A recall and CVLT-II learning slope. Significant relationships of covariates (block 1), significant and non-significant main effects (block 2) and significant and non-significant interactions (block 3) are reported with respect to each cognitive outcome in Table 3. No significant race interactions emerged in any of the models.

Table 2 Unadjusted associations between Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) scores and cognitive test scores among White and African American men and women (n 2090; 57 % female, mean age 47·9 years) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test-II; CVLT-II list A, CVLT-II list A total recall; CVLT-II short, CVLT-II short-delay free recall; CVLT-II, long, CVLT-II long-delay free recall; CVLT-II slope, CVLT-II learning slope; BVRT errors, Benton Visual Retention Test total errors; TMT, Trail Making Test.

Significant association: *P<0·05.

Table 3 Associations of Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) scores and race and poverty interaction terms with cognitive outcomes in sequential linear regression models among White and African American men and women (n 2090; 57 % female, mean age 47·9 years) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test-II; BVRT errors, Benton Visual Retention Test total errors; TMT, Trail Making Test.

Significant association: *P<0·05, **P<0·01.

Covariates included in these models were age, race, sex, education, poverty status, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score, current alcohol use (yes/no), current cigarette smoker (yes/no), BMI, mean systolic blood pressure and diabetes status.

Verbal learning and memory

After adjustment for all covariates that included age, race, sex, education, poverty status, CES-D score, current alcohol use (yes/no), current cigarette smoker (yes/no), BMI, mean SBP and diabetes status, HEI-2010 scores were significantly associated with CVLT-II list A recall (B=0·05, P<0·01) and CVLT-II learning slope (B=0·01, P<0·05), suggesting that higher diet quality was associated with better verbal learning and memory.

Non-verbal memory

After adjustment for covariates, there were no significant relationships of HEI-2010 scores with performance on the Benton Visual Retention Test. No significant interactions emerged.

Working memory

After adjustment for covariates, there was no significant association between HEI-2010 scores and Digit Span Forward and Backward performance. No significant interactions emerged.

Attention and cognitive flexibility

There were no significant main effects of HEI-2010 scores on TMT A or TMT B performance (block 2); however, there was a significant HEI-2010 by poverty status interaction in block 3 (B=−1·37, P<0·05). A plot of the interaction showed that the negative linear association between HEI-2010 scores and time to complete the TMT B task was steeper when participants were below the poverty threshold (see Fig. 1(a)).

Fig. 1 Interaction between Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) score and poverty status (, above the poverty line; , below the poverty line) among White and African American men and women (n 2090; 57 % female, mean age 47·9 years) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study. HEI-2010 score by poverty status interaction predicting performance on: (a) the Trail Making Test B (illustrating the relationship of HEI-2010 scores with time to complete the test in both poverty groups); (b) the Card Rotations Test (illustrating the relationship of HEI-2010 scores with number correct on the test in both poverty groups); and (c) the Identical Pictures Test (illustrating the relationship of HEI-2010 scores with number correct on the test in both poverty groups)

Visuospatial ability

There were no significant main effects of HEI-2010 scores on Card Rotations Test performance (block 1); however, there was a significant HEI-2010 by poverty status interaction in block 3 (B=0·21, P<0·01). A plot of the interaction showed that the positive linear association between HEI-2010 scores and Card Rotations Test scores was more pronounced among participants below the poverty threshold. There was no association among participants above the poverty threshold (see Fig. 1(b)). Thus, higher diet quality resulted in better visuospatial performance for those with less income, but did not have an effect for those with higher income.

Perceptual speed

There were no significant relationships of HEI-2010 scores with Identical Pictures Test performance. However, there was a significant HEI-2010 by poverty status interaction (B=0·05, P<0·05). A plot of the interaction showed a steep positive linear association between HEI-2010 scores and Identical Pictures Test total correct among participants below the poverty threshold, while the slope for those above the poverty threshold was relatively flat (see Fig. 1(c)). Similar to the aforementioned findings, better diet quality was related to better perceptual speed performance among those with less income.

Semantic fluency

After adjustment for covariates, there were no significant relationships of HEI-2010 scores with performance on the Semantic Fluency Test. No significant interactions emerged.

Discussion

The present investigation is, to our knowledge, the first to examine potential interactive relationships of diet quality (as defined by HEI-2010 scores), race and poverty status with multiple measures of cognitive function. Results revealed significant interactive relationships of diet quality and poverty status with performance on several measures of cognitive function among a diverse sample of urban adults. Specifically, when diet quality was higher, performance in the domains of attention and cognitive flexibility, visuospatial ability and perceptual speed was better among those with lower SES. In addition, higher diet quality was associated with better performance on two measures of verbal learning and memory, irrespective of race and poverty status.

The current findings suggest that better diet quality may play a protective role for cognitive function (across multiple domains) among the poorest. These findings are quite surprising given that the overall quality of the diet for the total sample and both poverty status groups was quite low. Cognitive function in our sample was lower, in general, among those in the low poverty status group. This finding is consistent with substantial evidence that shows cognitive function is lower among adults with lower SES( Reference Karlamangla, Miller-Martinez and Aneshensel 42 ). It is plausible that even a minimally healthful dietary pattern is beneficial in the sample. Participants who make more healthful food choices despite their limited economic means may be making choices that are ultimately helping to preserve their cognitive health.

Preliminary evidence suggests that poor-quality diets reflecting at least some adherence to dietary guidelines may show some benefits for health among the poor. For example in a previous HANDLS study analysis of chronic kidney disease, among individuals living in poverty, those who were somehow able to follow a healthful diet derived more of a renal health benefit than those with fewer economic disadvantages( Reference Crews, Kuczmarski and Miller 43 ). In that case, closer adherence to the DASH (Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension) diet carried greater benefit for low-SES individuals than for those with higher SES. Potential public health implications may therefore include a greater emphasis on improving the diets of socio-economically disadvantaged populations to improve or preserve cognitive function.

The finding that higher diet quality was associated with better verbal learning and memory performance in all participants is consistent with similar findings for other well-known measures of diet quality and dietary pattern. Among older adults enrolled in a Mediterranean diet intervention supplemented with extra-virgin olive oil, adherence to the intervention was associated with significantly better verbal learning and memory performance and less mild cognitive impairment over time after adjustment for age, sex, smoking, alcohol, diabetes, dyslipidaemia, hypertension, apo E genotype, physical activity, education, family history of cognitive impairment/dementia, BMI and total energy intake( Reference Martínez-Lapiscina, Clavero and Toledo 12 ). A study that examined the long-term impact of overall dietary patterns on cognition showed that a dietary pattern defined as healthful was associated with better verbal learning and memory performance over 13 years as compared with less healthful patterns( Reference Kesse-Guyot, Andreeva and Jeandel 44 ). Within the HANDLS study, a prior analysis of the association between antioxidants and cognitive function yielded cross-sectional evidence that greater vitamin E intake was associated with better verbal learning and memory performance( Reference Beydoun, Fanelli-Kuczmarski and Kitner-Triolo 14 ).

There is at least one plausible explanation for the link between diet quality and verbal learning and memory, specifically. Verbal learning and memory performance is particularly sensitive to cardiovascular and metabolic health indices that are linked to poor diet quality( Reference Sims, Madhere and Clark 45 , Reference Sims, Levy and Katzel 46 ). Inasmuch as individuals with poor diet quality are at risk for developing conditions that negatively influence verbal memory, they may also show preclinical decrements in verbal learning and memory that are detectable by sensitive cognitive measures.

Our significant findings were particularly striking given that the HEI-2010 may not be an optimal assessment of dietary intake. The DGA has provided federal nutritional guidance for Americans for nearly four decades; however, there is some evidence that these guidelines may not adequately apply to the socio-economic, cultural and ethnically diverse population of the USA( Reference Hite and Schoenfeld 47 ). For example, some evidence suggests that African American adults have gained more weight, and adults with low income have experienced higher rates of chronic disease, when following DGA guidelines( Reference Zamora, Gordon-Larsen and Jacobs 48 ). Another major criticism of the DGA and similar guidelines is that they do not take into account ‘real world’ factors that affect compliance to guidelines and that input is not derived from diverse groups or stakeholders when creating the guidelines( Reference King 7 ). It follows, therefore, that an assessment designed to measure compliance to these federal guidelines may underestimate the quality of the diet in a heterogeneous population such as in the HANDLS study. These limitations may help to partially explain the low HEI-2010 scores for the sample. Importantly, despite having utilized a less than ideal measure of diet quality, we documented several significant associations, suggesting that the benefit of a healthy diet on cognitive function is likely underestimated in the present investigation.

Several studies have suggested that diet quality tends to vary as a function of race( Reference Hiza, Casavale and Guenther 15 ), with African Americans having poorer dietary patterns( Reference Sijtsma, Meyer and Steffen 16 ); however, in our study, there were no significant differences in HEI-2010 scores by race and no race interactions emerged. This finding was surprising given the aforementioned literature. Both racial groups had HEI-2010 scores just above 40, indicating poor overall diet quality for both of the predominant racial groups in urban Baltimore, despite possible cultural differences in eating patterns( Reference Fanelli Kuczmarski, Mason and Beydoun 49 ). It is possible that a lack of sensitivity of the HEI-2010 may have played a role in non-significant mean differences in HEI-2010 scores between race and poverty status groups. While the total score relies on whole food groups, other indices of diet quality that consider macronutrients and micronutrients may be more sensitive to subtle differences in the nutritional content of the diet between races. Future studies should consider more sensitive measures of diet quality such as the mean adequacy ratio to test this hypothesis. Non-significant mean differences in HEI-2010 total score between poverty status groups are also contrary to the literature supporting an association between diet quality and SES. There were more participants that fell into the higher income group than the low income group which may have played a role. Future research may consider factors that promote low diet quality among higher-SES groups.

In general, effect sizes for significant associations were small; therefore, interpretation of findings should be approached with caution. Yet, the pattern of associations suggests that diet quality and cognitive function are likely related at the population level, although it is not yet clear whether the association is clinically significant for individuals. It is premature, therefore, to suggest that interventions aimed at improving overall diet quality would improve cognitive function in this population. Furthermore, it is important to view our findings in the context of the sample’s poor overall diet quality for both African Americans and Whites, as well as for those above and below the poverty threshold. Individuals in the study showed poor adherence to the DGA. A prior analysis showed that HANDLS participants had low adherence to other healthful dietary patterns such as the Mediterranean diet and DASH eating pattern, but adhered mainly to a Western dietary pattern( Reference Kuczmarski, Mason and Allegro 50 ). Therefore, our findings are unlikely to generalize to individuals with more healthful dietary patterns. Future studies are needed to test these associations in a larger, more heterogeneous sample with respect to diet quality, SES and race.

Our study has several strengths. First, the HANDLS study consists of a large representative sample of urban-dwelling adults. Importantly, the sample had a large proportion of African Americans, a population that is often under-recruited and understudied. Few studies of cognitive function provide such rich data on African Americans. Second, diet quality was based on two 24 h recalls. Most studies that assess diet quality include only one recall. Lastly, the diet quality and cognitive function relationship has been examined in prior studies using the HEI-2005, and those studies showed a null and positive association between diet quality and cognitive function, respectively( Reference Tangney, Kwasny and Li 51 , Reference Ye, Scott and Gao 52 ). However, we are unaware of similar studies that have employed the HEI-2010 score, an index that has undergone significant improvements from its previous iteration. As such, our study makes a unique contribution to both the nutrition and cognition literature.

Our study has some limitations. The directionality of the relationships that emerged in the study can only be inferred due to the cross-sectional nature of the analysis. It is possible that reduced cognitive performance may precede poor diet quality. Future analyses will examine the prospective association between changes in diet quality and cognitive performance. Another limitation of the current study is its reliance on memory-based dietary recall. Human memory is an acceptable but inexact method for assessing past food consumption behaviour. Finally, although the findings cannot be generalized to a national population, independent demographic analyses found this sample is representative of urban populations from US cities with similar population densities and racial distributions (J Lepkowski, unpublished results).

Conclusion

Overall, our findings lend insight into understanding the potential influence of overall diet quality on cognitive function in an urban-dwelling population and how the association varies by SES group membership. Poor diet quality in our sample suggests a need for heightened awareness of optimal dietary patterns, including more healthful dietary patterns that can be achieved across varying socio-economic groups with potentially limited food access. Diet quality measures that are more comprehensive and more sensitive to cultural dietary patterns may help to further illuminate links between diet and cognitive function.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors thank the staff of the HANDLS study group for collection and availability of the data. Financial support: This publication was made possible by a grant from the National Institute on Aging’s Intramural Research Program (M.F.K., M.K.E., A.B.Z., grant number Z01–AG000194); Delaware INBRE programme grants from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (R.S.W., R.T.P., C.S.G., grant number NIH-NIGMS: P20 GM103446) and the State of Delaware; and a National Institute on Aging extramural grant (S.R.W., grant number R01 AG03416). None of the funders had a role in the design, analysis or writing of this manuscript. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: R.S.W. formulated the research question and has overall responsibility for this paper. R.S.W., S.R.W., M.F.K., M.K.E. and A.B.Z. contributed to the study design. M.F.K., M.K.E. and A.B.Z. directed data acquisition. R.S.W., S.R.W., M.F.K., C.S.G. and B.G. wrote the manuscript. R.S.W., S.R.W., M.F.K., R.T.P., M.K.E. and A.B.Z. helped analyse and interpret the data. All authors were involved in critically revising the manuscript and approving the final version for publication. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki guidelines and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the human Institutional Review Boards at MedStar Health Research Institute, University of Delaware and the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, National Institutes of Health. All HANDLS participants provided written informed consent and were compensated monetarily.

References

1. Royall, DR, Palmer, R, Chiodo, LK et al. (2004) Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: the Freedom House study. J Am Geriatr Soc 52, 346352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Waldstein, SR & Wendell, CR (2010) Neurocognitive function and cardiovascular disease. J Alzheimers Dis 20, 833842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Waldstein, SR (2000) Health effects on cognitive aging. In The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research, pp. 189217 [PC Stern and LL Carstensen, editors]. Washington, DC: Committee on Future Directions for Cognitive Research on Aging, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Academy Press.Google Scholar
4. Salthouse, TA (2009) When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging 30, 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Singh-Manoux, A, Kivimaki, M, Glymour, MM et al. (2012) Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 344, d7622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Gillette-Guyonnet, S, Van Kan, GA, Andrieu, S et al. (2007) IANA task force on nutrition and cognitive decline with aging. J Nutr Health Aging 11, 132.Google ScholarPubMed
7. King, JC (2007) An evidence-based approach for establishing dietary guidelines. J Nutr 137, 480483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Creavin, ST, Gallacher, J, Pickering, J et al. (2012) High caloric intake, poor cognition and dementia: the Caerphilly prospective study. Eur J Epidemiol 27, 197203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Ye, X, Gao, X, Scott, T et al. (2011) Habitual sugar intake and cognitive function among middle-aged and older Puerto Ricans without diabetes. Br J Nutr 106, 14231432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Lourida, I, Soni, M, Thompson-Coon, J et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet, cognitive function, and dementia: a systematic review. Epidemiology 24, 479489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Kuczmarski, MF, Allegro, D & Stave, E (2014) The association of healthful diets and cognitive function: a review. J Nutr Gerontol Geriatr 33, 6990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Martínez-Lapiscina, EH, Clavero, P, Toledo, E et al. (2013) Virgin olive oil supplementation and long-term cognition: the PREDIMED-NAVARRA randomized trial. J Nutr Health Aging 17, 544552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Berr, C, Portet, F, Carriere, I et al. (2009) Olive oil and cognition: results from the three-city study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 28, 357364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Beydoun, MA, Fanelli-Kuczmarski, MT, Kitner-Triolo, MH et al. (2015) Dietary antioxidant intake and its association with cognitive function in an ethnically diverse sample of US adults. Psychosom Med 77, 6882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. Hiza, HA, Casavale, KO, Guenther, PM et al. (2013) Diet quality of Americans differs by age, sex, race/ethnicity, income, and education level. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 297306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16. Sijtsma, FP, Meyer, KA, Steffen, LM et al. (2012) Longitudinal trends in diet and effects of sex, race, and education on dietary quality score change: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults study. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 580586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Raffensperger, S, Kuczmarski, MF, Hotchkiss, L et al. (2010) The effect of race and predictors of socioeconomic status on diet quality in the Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study sample. J Natl Med Assoc 102, 923930.Google ScholarPubMed
18. Larson, NI, Story, MT & Nelson, MC (2009) Neighborhood environments: disparities in access to healthy foods in the US. Am J Prev Med 36, 7481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19. Moser, RP, Green, V, Weber, D et al. (2005) Psychosocial correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption among African American men. J Nutr Educ Behav 37, 306314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Mwendwa, DT, Gholson, G, Sims, RC et al. (2011) Coping with perceived racism: a significant factor in the development of obesity in African American women? J Natl Med Assoc 103, 602608.Google ScholarPubMed
21. Darmon, N & Drewnowski, A (2008) Does social class predict diet quality? Am J Clin Nutr 87, 11071117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Kirkpatrick, SI, Dodd, KW, Reedy, J et al. (2012) Income and race/ethnicity are associated with adherence to food-based dietary guidance among US adults and children. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 624635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23. Gordon-Larsen, P, Nelson, MC, Page, P et al. (2006) Inequality in the built environment underlies key health disparities in physical activity and obesity. Pediatrics 117, 417424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Ranjit, N, Wilkinson, AV, Lytle, LM et al. (2015) Socioeconomic inequalities in children’s diet: the role of the home food environment. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12, Suppl. 1, S4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Evans, MK, Lepkowski, JM, Powe, NR et al. (2010) Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS): overcoming barriers to implementing a longitudinal, epidemiologic, urban study of health, race, and socioeconomic status. Ethn Dis 20, 267275.Google ScholarPubMed
26. Wood, RY, Giuliano, KK, Bignell, CU et al. (2006) Assessing cognitive ability in research: use of MMSE with minority populations and elderly adults with low education levels. J Gerontol Nurs 32, 4554.Google ScholarPubMed
27. Conway, JM, Ingwersen, LA & Moshfegh, AJ (2004) Accuracy of dietary recall using the USDA five-step multiple-pass method in men: an observational validation study. J Am Diet Assoc 104, 595603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Conway, JM, Ingwersen, LA, Vinyard, BT et al. (2003) Effectiveness of the US Department of Agriculture 5-step multiple-pass method in assessing food intake in obese and nonobese women. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 11711178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. McBride, J (2001) Was it a slab, a slice, or a sliver? High-tech innovations take food survey to a new level. Agric Res 49, 47.Google Scholar
30. US Department of Agriculture (2008) USDA Food and Nutrient Database for Dietary Studies, 3.0. Beltsville, MD: US Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, Food Surveys Research Group; available at http://www.ars.usda.gov/ba/bhnrc/fsrg Google Scholar
31. Guenther, PM, Casavale, KO, Reedy, J et al. (2013) Update of the healthy eating index: HEI-2010. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 569580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. US Department of Agriculture & US Department of Health and Human Services (2010) Dietary Guidelines for Americans. http://health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2010/ (accessed January 2016).Google Scholar
33. Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) (2010) Healthy Eating Index 2010 calculation. https://handls.nih.gov/06Coll-w01HEI.htm (accessed January 2016).Google Scholar
34. Delis, D, Kramer, J, Kaplan, E et al. (2000) CVLT-II. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
35. Benton, AL (1992) Benton Visual Retention Test, 5th ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
36. Strauss, E, Sherman, EM & Spreen, O (2006) A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
37. Wechsler, D (1981) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
38. Reitan, RM (1992) Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory.Google Scholar
39. Ekstrom, RB, French, JW, Harman, HH et al. (1976) Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests, revised ed. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
40. US Census Bureau, Social, Economic, and Housing Statistics Division (2014) Poverty Thresholds, 2004. http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/poverty/data/threshld/thresh04.html (accessed January 2016).Google Scholar
41. Radloff, LS (1977) The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42. Karlamangla, AS, Miller-Martinez, D, Aneshensel, CS et al. (2009) Trajectories of cognitive function in late life in the United States: demographic and socioeconomic predictors. Am J Epidemiol 170, 331342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43. Crews, DC, Kuczmarski, MF, Miller, ER et al. (2015) Dietary habits, poverty, and chronic kidney disease in an urban population. J Ren Nutr 25, 103110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
44. Kesse-Guyot, E, Andreeva, VA, Jeandel, C et al. (2012) A healthy dietary pattern at midlife is associated with subsequent cognitive performance. J Nutr 142, 909915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Sims, RC, Madhere, S, Clark, E Gordon et al. (2008) Relationships between blood pressure, triglycerides, and verbal learning in African Americans. J Natl Med Assoc 100, 11931198.Google ScholarPubMed
46. Sims, RC, Levy, ST, Katzel, LI et al. (2015) Fasting glucose, glucose tolerance, and neurocognitive function relations among non-diabetic older adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 37, 4960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47. Hite, AH & Schoenfeld, P (2015) Open letter to the secretaries of the US Departments of Agriculture and Health and Human Services on the creation of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Nutrition 31, 776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
48. Zamora, D, Gordon-Larsen, P, Jacobs, DR et al. (2010) Diet quality and weight gain among black and white young adults: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study (1985–2005). Am J Clin Nutr 92, 784793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49. Fanelli Kuczmarski, M, Mason, MA, Beydoun, MA et al. (2013) Dietary patterns and sarcopenia in an urban African American and white population in the United States. J Nutr Gerontol Geriatr 32, 291316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
50. Kuczmarski, MF, Mason, MA, Allegro, D et al. (2015) Dietary quality and nutritional biomarkers associated with dietary patterns of socioeconomically diverse urban African American and White population. Procedia Food Sci 4, 104113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
51. Tangney, CC, Kwasny, MJ, Li, H et al. (2011) Adherence to a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and cognitive decline in a community population. Am J Clin Nutr 93, 601607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
52. Ye, X, Scott, T, Gao, X et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet, healthy eating index 2005, and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Puerto Rican adults. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 276281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for sample characteristics and cognitive test scores for the total sample and poverty status groups: White and African American men and women (n 2090) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

Figure 1

Table 2 Unadjusted associations between Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) scores and cognitive test scores among White and African American men and women (n 2090; 57 % female, mean age 47·9 years) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

Figure 2

Table 3 Associations of Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) scores and race and poverty interaction terms with cognitive outcomes in sequential linear regression models among White and African American men and women (n 2090; 57 % female, mean age 47·9 years) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Interaction between Healthy Eating Index-2010 (HEI-2010) score and poverty status (, above the poverty line; , below the poverty line) among White and African American men and women (n 2090; 57 % female, mean age 47·9 years) from urban neighbourhoods in Baltimore, MD, USA; Healthy Aging in Neighborhoods of Diversity across the Life Span (HANDLS) study. HEI-2010 score by poverty status interaction predicting performance on: (a) the Trail Making Test B (illustrating the relationship of HEI-2010 scores with time to complete the test in both poverty groups); (b) the Card Rotations Test (illustrating the relationship of HEI-2010 scores with number correct on the test in both poverty groups); and (c) the Identical Pictures Test (illustrating the relationship of HEI-2010 scores with number correct on the test in both poverty groups)