Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:45:49.828Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association between language use and food insecurity among Hispanic adults residing in the USA depends on nativity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 May 2023

Miguel Angel Lopez
Affiliation:
Department of Social, Behavioral and Population Sciences, School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA
Melissa Fuster
Affiliation:
Department of Social, Behavioral and Population Sciences, School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA
Julia M Fleckman
Affiliation:
Department of Social, Behavioral and Population Sciences, School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA
Amy George
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish and Portuguese, School of Liberal Arts, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA
M Pia Chaparro*
Affiliation:
Department of Social, Behavioral and Population Sciences, School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA Nutritional Sciences Program, Department of Health Systems and Population Health, School of Public Health, University of Washington, 305J Raitt Hall, Box 353410, Seattle, WA 98195, USA (current affiliation)
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

To examine the association between language use – predominantly English, English and Spanish equally and predominantly Spanish – and food insecurity among Hispanic adults residing in the USA, 1999–2018.

Design:

Pooled cross-sectional study design.

Setting:

United States.

Participants:

15 073 Hispanic adults.

Results:

Compared with Hispanic adults who predominantly spoke English and after adjusting for age, sex, family income-to-poverty ratio, education level and employment status, Hispanic adults who spoke English and Spanish equally (OR = 1·28, 95 % CI = 1·05, 1·56) or predominantly Spanish (OR = 1·25, 95 % CI = 1·04, 1·49) had higher odds of food insecurity. After stratifying by country of birth, language use was associated with higher odds of food insecurity only for Hispanic adults born outside of the USA, but not for Hispanic adults born in the USA. Hispanic adults born outside of the USA who spoke English and Spanish equally (OR = 1·27, 95 % CI = 1·04, 1·55) or spoke predominantly Spanish (OR = 1·24, 95 % CI = 1·04, 1·48) had higher odds of food insecurity when compared with those who predominantly spoke English.

Conclusion:

Foreign-born Hispanic adults who speak predominantly Spanish, or English and Spanish equally, have higher odds of food insecurity. Food and nutrition assistance programmes that serve Hispanic immigrants should make sure to provide linguistically and culturally appropriate services to this population.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

A household is considered food insecure when it does not have economic and physical access to sufficient, safe and nutrient-dense food that meets dietary needs and food preferences in a socially acceptable manner(1). In 2020, about 38 million (10·5 %) households in the USA were food insecure, with Hispanic households having a disproportionally higher prevalence of food insecurity (17·2 %) when compared with non-Hispanic White households (7·1 %)(Reference Coleman-Jensen, Rabbitt and Gregory2). Among adults, food insecurity is associated with inadequate nutrient intake(Reference Laraia3); an increased risk for chronic diseases like obesity(Reference Morales and Berkowitz4,Reference Larson and Story5) , type II diabetes mellitus(Reference Laraia3), hypertension(Reference Laraia3,Reference Liu and Eicher-Miller6) and all-cause mortality(Reference Sun, Liu and Rong7).

At the turn of the 21st century, there were about 44 million Hispanic residents in the USA(8), and it is estimated that 78 % of them spoke predominantly Spanish at home(Reference Perez-Escamilla9). Limited evidence indicates that Spanish-speaking Hispanic individuals have higher rates of food insecurity than those who speak English(Reference Mazur, Marquis and Jensen10Reference Kaiser, Melgar-Quinonez and Lamp12). A similar language disparity is also present in research comparing health outcomes among Hispanic individuals with varying levels of language use in the USA(Reference Martinez13). This linguistic gradient suggests a pattern where Hispanic individuals who predominantly speak Spanish have worse health outcomes than those who speak Spanish and English equally, and bilingual Hispanic individuals have worse health outcomes than those who predominantly speak English(Reference Martinez13). The linguistic gradient has been observed for self-reported physical and mental health(Reference Martinez13Reference Ponce, Hays and Cunningham15); practicing preventative measures, like undergoing cancer screenings and receiving vaccines(Reference Jacobs, Karavolos and Rathouz16,Reference Cheng, Chen and Cunningham17) and being able to afford and schedule healthcare visits(Reference Sentell, Shumway and Snowden18). Therefore, in respect to language, the degree to which a Hispanic individual experiences negative health outcomes in the USA has more to do with the presence and predominant use of a second language rather than the exclusive absence of English(Reference Martinez13).

However, to our knowledge, studies investigating the association between language use and food insecurity have explored language as a binary variable (English proficient v. not English proficient or Spanish proficient), and primarily as a proxy for acculturation(Reference Mazur, Marquis and Jensen10Reference Kaiser, Melgar-Quinonez and Lamp12,Reference Dave, Evans and Saunders19) . The use of language as a proxy of acculturation, or the process individuals experience to adopt a different culture, usually the more dominant one(Reference Perez-Escamilla9), has been heavily criticised in recent years for being linear, static, bidirectional and unethical(Reference Gallo, Penedo and Espinosa de los Monteros20Reference Velasco-Mondragon, Jimenez and Palladino-Davis23), thus suggesting that acculturation is not appropriately captured by language use. In contrast, the present study explores three levels of language use (predominantly Spanish, Spanish and English equally and predominantly English) as an independent risk factor for food insecurity among Hispanic adults living in the USA, under the postulation that existing systematically racist barriers that centre food and nutrition information, assistance and practice around non-Hispanic White’s dominant language and culture may make it difficult for Hispanic adults, particularly predominantly Spanish-speaking Hispanic adults, to procure food and nutrition assistance and services in the USA(Reference Conrad24Reference Bowen, Elliott and Hardison-Moody28). Moreover, this study explores the differences in the proposed association between language use and food insecurity among Hispanic adults by country of birth, as US-born Hispanic adults’ barriers to structurally incorporate into the US food and nutrition environment, which would impact their food insecurity risk, may differ from that of non-US-born Hispanic adults(Reference Coleman-Jensen, Rabbitt and Gregory2,Reference Myers and Painter27) . In this study’s context, structural incorporation refers to the extent to which a Hispanic individual experiences similar health-related determinants, like food insecurity, to non-Hispanic Whites in the USA. In congruence with the linguistic gradient, the authors hypothesise that Hispanic adults who predominantly speak Spanish will have the highest likelihood of food insecurity, followed by bilingual Hispanic adults and tailed by their predominantly English-speaking counterparts, with these associations being stronger among the foreign-born.

Methods

Study design

Data for this cross-sectional study were pooled from the National Health Assessment and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES)(29), 1999–2018. NHANES is a nationally representative health and nutrition survey of the noninstitutionalised US’ population that combines in-person interviews with health examinations, such as physical measures and blood draws. NHANES’ study design allows it to collect data across the USA using the same physical instruments across recruitment sites to ensure reliability. NHANES’ in-person interviews include demographic, socio-economic and health- and diet-related questions carried out through stratified and multistage probability sampling(29). While the survey is nationally representative, our analysis was restricted to respondents who self-identified as Hispanic, were at least 18 years of age at the time of the survey, and had all the necessary data to estimate food (in)security. NHANES’ protocol was approved by the National Center for Health Statistics Research Ethics Review Board, and all participants provided informed consent. Additional details are available elsewhere(29).

Measures

Food insecurity

NHANES includes the eighteen-item United States Department of Agriculture’s (USDA) Household Food Security Survey Module (HFSSM)(Reference Rabbitt and Coleman-Jensen30), which includes questions about a household’s food access limitations. Responses to the eighteen items are coded as affirmative (a positive response) or dissenting (a negative response). Affirmative responses are summed to calculate a raw score of the scale. Based on the raw scores, households are classified as high food security (0 affirmative responses for households with and without children), marginal food security (1–2 affirmative responses for households with and without children), low food security (3–7 affirmative responses for households with children and 3–5 affirmative responses for households without children) and very low food security (8–18 affirmative responses for households with children and 6–10 affirmative responses for households without children). The present study’s analysis explored the association between language use at home and food insecurity as a collapsed and dichotomised measure, which consisted of food security (high and marginal food security combined) and food insecurity (low food security and very low food security combined).

Language use

Language use at home was used to represent respondents’ general language use. NHANES’ response options include only English, more English than Spanish, both equally, more Spanish than English and only Spanish. However, response options for this analysis were collapsed according to the linguistic gradient: predominantly English (only English and more English than Spanish), English and Spanish equally (both equally) and predominantly Spanish (more Spanish than English and only Spanish).

Covariates and potential confounders

Demographic covariates included age and gender; potential confounders included family income-to-poverty ratio (IPR), education and employment status. In adjusted models, age and family IPR were each converted into categorical variables of at least four groups to minimise loss of statistical interpretation and improve inference(Reference Altman and Royston31). Also, both age and IPR’s maximum limits were set by NHANES and not the authors of this paper. Age was capped at 80 years old and stratified into quintiles: 18–27 (reference), 28–38, 39–50, 51–63 and ≥ 64 years. Family IPR was top coded at 5·00 and classified according to IPR eligibility for federal food assistance programmes(Reference Bitler, Gennetian and Gibson-Davis32). Family IPR was calculated as a ratio of monthly family income to the federal poverty level specific to the respondents’ family size. Family IPR was categorised as < 1·00, 1·00–1·30, 1·30–1·85 and > 1·85. Family IPR reference was identified as falling below the federal poverty line for the respective respondent’s survey year, or < 1·00. Sex was categorised as men (reference) and women. Education was categorised as less than a high school degree (reference), high school or General Educational Development equivalent, some college or an Associate in Arts (AA) degree and 4-year college graduate or above. Current employment status was categorised as unemployed (reference), part-time employed (< 40 h per week) and full-time employed (≥ 40 h per week). Further, the respondent’s country of birth was explored as a potential effect modifier. Country of birth was categorised as being born in the USA (i.e. born in the 50 US’ states or Washington D.C.) and elsewhere (including Puerto Rico).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics of sample characteristics of respondents (n 15 073) were reported by the presence of food (in)security. All analyses were carried out using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute) and a P < 0·05 was used to establish statistical significance in all associations, apart from interaction terms. For interaction terms, a P < 0·10 was used to establish statistical significance.

A series of multivariable logistic regression models – crude and adjusted – were used to predict the association between language use at home and food insecurity. Adjusted models controlled for age, gender, education, family IPR and employment. In addition, an adjusted multivariable logistic regression model with an interaction term between language use and country of birth was used to determine if effect modification by country of birth was present. After confirming that the association between a respondent’s language use at home and food insecurity was dependent on their country of birth, adjusted multivariable logistic regression models were stratified by the respondent’s country of birth, with language use at home as the exposure of interest and food insecurity as the outcome. All models included NHANES’ sampling weights and had options specified to control for NHANES’ design effects of stratification, clustering and unequal probability sampling. Standard errors and hypothesis tests adjusted for NHANES’ complex survey design using proc survey commands in SAS.

Results

More than one in four respondents (28·00 %) reported living in food-insecure households. Most of these respondents spoke predominantly Spanish at home (66·91 %) and were born outside of the USA (70·26 %). Further, food insecure respondents were younger, as the majority reported to be between 18 and 27 years old (29·65 %); predominantly women (52·70 %) and unemployed (47·76 %); had less than a high school education (64·23 %) and a family IPR below the federal poverty line (54·09 %). Table 1 presents a more detailed account of the sample’s demographic characteristics by the presence of food (in)security.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of NHANES’ Hispanic respondents by food (in)security, 1999–2018

* Household food security was estimated with the eighteen-item Household Food Security Survey Module(Reference Rabbitt and Coleman-Jensen30). Households with < 3 affirmative responses were classified as food secure and those with ≥ 3 affirmative responses as food insecure.

United States encompasses the 50 U.S.’ states and Washington D.C. and excludes US territories such as Puerto Rico.

Family IPR was calculated as a ratio of monthly family income to the federal poverty level specific to the respondents’ family size.

The crude and adjusted associations between language use and food insecurity are presented in Table 2. In crude models, those who spoke English and Spanish equally and spoke predominantly Spanish at home were associated with 1·70 and 2·30 higher odds of reporting food insecurity, respectively, when compared with those who spoke predominantly English. In fully adjusted models, the association remained though it was attenuated: Hispanic respondents who both spoke English and Spanish equally or spoke predominantly Spanish had 1·28 and 1·25 higher odds of experiencing food insecurity, respectively, when compared with Hispanic respondents who spoke predominantly English.

Table 2 Multivariable logistic regression model between language use at home and food insecurity among NHANES’ Hispanic respondents, 1999–2018

* Household food security was estimated with the eighteen-item Household Food Security Survey Module(Reference Rabbitt and Coleman-Jensen30). Households with < 3 affirmative responses were classified as food secure and those with ≥ 3 affirmative responses as food insecure.

Family IPR was calculated as a ratio of monthly family income to the federal poverty level specific to the respondents’ family size.

After confirming that the association between language use and food insecurity was dependent on the country of birth (interaction P-values: English and Spanish equally = 0·09; predominantly Spanish < 0·01), we stratified the analysis by respondents’ country of birth (Table 3). Among US-born Hispanic respondents, language use at home was not significantly associated with food insecurity. In contrast, Figure 1 illustrates how foreign-born Hispanic respondents who spoke Spanish and English equally had 1·27 higher odds of reporting food insecurity compared with those who spoke predominantly English at home. Further, foreign-born Hispanic respondents who spoke predominantly Spanish had 1·24 higher odds of experiencing food insecurity when compared with those who predominantly spoke English at home (Fig. 1).

Table 3 Adjusted multivariable logistic regression model between language use at home and food insecurity, and stratified by country of birth, among NHANES’ Hispanic respondents, 1999–2018

* Household food security was estimated with the eighteen-item Household Food Security Survey Module(Reference Rabbitt and Coleman-Jensen30). Households with < 3 affirmative responses were classified as food secure and those with ≥ 3 affirmative responses as food insecure.

United States encompasses the 50 US’ states and Washington D.C. and excludes US territories such as Puerto Rico.

Family IPR was calculated as a ratio of monthly family income to the federal poverty level specific to the respondents’ family size.

Fig. 1 Results of Adjusted Multivariable Logistic Regression Model between Language Use and Food Insecurity, Stratified by Country of Birth, among National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys’ Hispanic Respondents, 1999–2018. aUnited States encompasses the 50 US’ states and Washington D.C. and excludes US territories such as Puerto Rico

Discussion

In this study, we used NHANES data to explore the association between the linguistic gradient and food insecurity among Hispanic respondents living in the USA. Through adjusted multivariable logistic regression modelling, we identified that (1) speaking English and Spanish equally or predominantly Spanish was associated with higher odds of food insecurity, but (2) this association was only significant among foreign-born Hispanic respondents once the sample was stratified by country of birth. Those who predominantly spoke Spanish and who spoke English and Spanish equally had a similar likelihood of food insecurity, in conflict with our hypothesis and previous research exploring health and healthcare-related outcomes and the linguistic gradient(Reference Martinez13Reference Sentell, Shumway and Snowden18).

Prior studies’ findings on language use and food insecurity are similar to the present study: Spanish-speaking Hispanic individuals have higher odds of food insecurity when compared to their English-speaking counterparts(Reference Mazur, Marquis and Jensen10Reference Kaiser, Melgar-Quinonez and Lamp12,Reference Dave, Evans and Saunders19) . These previous studies present language use as a bivariate exposure, either as English proficiency v. not English proficiency(Reference Capps, Horowitz and Fortuny11), or English speaking v. Spanish speaking(Reference Mazur, Marquis and Jensen10,Reference Kaiser, Melgar-Quinonez and Lamp12,Reference Dave, Evans and Saunders19) , and do not consider bilingual Hispanic individuals. Doing so limits the interpretability of results as bilingual Hispanic individuals may have unique experiences that differ from predominantly Spanish-speaking and predominantly English-speaking Hispanic individuals(Reference Martinez13). It is possible that bilingual Hispanic individuals are able to navigate the US’ food and nutrition environment as well as those who predominantly speak English, but may hold strong cultural ties to their ethnic heritage like those of predominantly Spanish-speaking Hispanic individuals(Reference Hammons, Hannon and Teran-Garcia33Reference Horowitz, Tuzzio and Rojas35). Meaning, bilingual Hispanic individuals may be able to translate and understand most food and nutrition information presented to them, but may not culturally identify with that information if not ethnically tailored to the Latin American culture with which they identify(Reference Hammons, Hannon and Teran-Garcia33Reference Horowitz, Tuzzio and Rojas35). In practice, this may result in additional barriers a bilingual Hispanic individual must overcome to procure food as well as food and nutrition assistance and services.

Our analysis found similar food insecurity odds among bilingual and predominantly Spanish-speaking Hispanic immigrants. It is possible that both groups experience similar stigma resulting from their accents when speaking English(Reference Morey36), which may place them at a socio-economic disadvantage, affecting their food security. For example, when applying to high-income positions, Spanish-speaking Hispanic adults with noticeable accents were perceived as being less suitable for the job, and fewer were hired when compared to White-presenting English-speaking applicants with no accents(Reference Gluszek and Dovidio37). Of those that were hired, Spanish-accented Hispanic employees were less likely to be promoted(Reference Gluszek and Dovidio37). Further, Hispanic individuals born outside of the USA may have more prominent accents when compared with US-born Hispanic individuals, as they may have learned English at an older age or have fewer opportunities to practice, resulting in greater discrimination. Indeed, a study examining foreign accents in English sentences has found that immigrants who lived in the USA longer had less notable accents when compared with more recently arrived immigrants, but more noticeable accents than their US-born counterparts(Reference Flege, Birdsong and Bialystok38). However, not all second-language accents are stigmatised. A similar study found that French-accented applicants were perceived just as favourably, or more, than White-presenting American English-speaking applicants with no accents(Reference Hosoda, Dietz and Stone-Romero39). This linguistic iniquity seems to transcend employment. Low-income Hispanic adults with limited English language skills are less likely to apply for the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP), the US’ largest federal food safety net, when compared with those who are English proficient. Low-income Hispanic adults with limited English language skills report difficulty understanding current eligibility restrictions, not comprehending changes in eligibility requirements, a lengthy and complex application process and overall lack of federal food assistance programme knowledge(Reference Algert, Reibel and Renvall40). Increases in English language skills have been previously associated with greater use of nutrition assistance programs and food security(Reference Winham and Armstrong Florian41). Further, Spanish-speaking Hispanic adults who receive federal food assistance have reported encountering discrimination from store employees when redeeming programme benefits(Reference Bertmann, Barroso and Ohri-Vachaspati42), which may reduce future participation.

Previous studies also either limited their sample to Hispanic immigrants(Reference Capps, Horowitz and Fortuny11) or did not account for country of birth in their analysis(Reference Mazur, Marquis and Jensen10,Reference Kaiser, Melgar-Quinonez and Lamp12,Reference Dave, Evans and Saunders19) . However, previous systematic reviews and studies of food insecurity among Hispanic individuals have pointed out that Hispanic immigrants face unique risks of food insecurity when compared with US-born Hispanic individuals(Reference Coleman-Jensen, Rabbitt and Gregory2,Reference Myers and Painter27) . By not exploring the intricacies behind a respondent’s language use and country of birth, previous studies have restricted the utility of their results as their findings may mask nuances in the association. We found no association between language use and food insecurity for US-born Hispanic adults. In addition to learning English since early childhood, predominantly Spanish-speaking and bilingual US-born Hispanic individuals have distinct circumstances that may result in the lack of observed association between language use and food insecurity. First, US-born Hispanic individuals are US citizens and eligible to apply for federal food assistance programs if they meet other eligibility criteria(Reference Munger, Lloyd and Speirs43). Second, US-born Hispanic individuals who predominantly speak Spanish, or English and Spanish equally, at home may do so because they are first-generation Americans who live with a predominantly Spanish-speaking family member, like a grandparent; or practice Spanish at home to retain their cultural heritage. Therefore, speaking predominantly Spanish, or Spanish and English equally, at home may be a result of a personal choice rather than a linguistic limitation.

Despite this accumulating body of evidence, no studies to date have applied causal inference to determine if the association between language use and food insecurity is a causal one. Language use is strongly correlated with unmeasured and/or unobserved factors that may influence food insecurity. For example, in the present study, we were unable to account for Hispanic respondents’ immigration status. When compared with an undocumented immigrant, documented immigrants may have greater resources before coming to the USA(Reference Munger, Lloyd and Speirs43), possibly including the ability to study and practice the English language prior to arrival. Concomitantly, speaking English proficiently, lack of a Spanish accent and being a documented immigrant in the country may provide the Hispanic individual better employment or educational opportunities as well as eligibility to apply for government assistance, like federal food assistance programs(Reference Munger, Lloyd and Speirs43).

This study has some strengths and limitations. Our ability to explore three levels of language use – predominantly English, English and Spanish equally and predominantly Spanish – among Hispanic adults residing in the USA is a strength. Doing so provided us with a more comprehensive account of how language use is associated with food insecurity among Hispanic individuals and compared it to previous findings concerning the linguistic gradient. Second, accounting for the respondents’ country of birth further revealed nuances in the association not reported in previous studies, to the best of our knowledge. Yet, despite these strengths, the study also has limitations. First, we had to pool almost 20 years of cross-sectional data to achieve the sample size needed to run a stratified analysis. Ensuing studies examining the association between language and food insecurity among Hispanic adults in the USA should investigate the relationship in a longitudinal manner to explore if the observed relationship is stable across time. Second, given the cross-sectional nature of the data, we were not able to identify risk or infer causality of food insecurity and had to limit the interpretability of our results accordingly. We were also constrained by the response options of NHANES’ language items. The authors of this paper recognise that Spanish is not the only language spoken among Hispanic individuals and, given data limitations, we were unable to explore the association between indigenous languages and food insecurity among Hispanic individuals in the USA. Further, despite being used in previous studies(Reference Mazur, Marquis and Jensen10,Reference Dave, Evans and Saunders19) , language use at home may not be representative of an individual’s linguistic abilities and comfort(Reference Siegel, Martin and Bruno44). For example, a respondent may live with someone who is more comfortable with Spanish than English (like an immigrant parent) and speak predominantly Spanish at home but speak English in all other spheres of their life. While this has the potential to slightly skew our results, it is likely that it would bring the odds ratios closer to one and dilute the association, as there are fewer scenarios where a Hispanic adult would speak predominantly English at home but Spanish elsewhere. Also, to protect respondents’ privacy, NHANES does not publicly release immigration status of foreign-born Hispanic adults. Therefore, an additional analysis exploring how federal food assistance eligibility and participation may influence the association between language use and food insecurity could not be done. Finally, while prior evidence supports the aetiological explanation hypothesised(Reference Martinez13Reference Bowen, Elliott and Hardison-Moody28), the authors had no concrete variable that allowed us to explore how linguistic discrimination may influence the association between Spanish use and food insecurity in our models. Future research trying to elucidate the association between language and food insecurity should use causal inference methods and examine language proficiency, as opposed to use, while accounting for other established determinants of food insecurity, like (un)documented status and participation in food assistance programs. More research is needed to examine how contextual factors, such as structural racism and food environments, influence the association between language proficiency and food insecurity among Hispanic adults attempting to procure linguistically and culturally appropriate food and nutrition assistance/services in the USA.

Conclusion

Hispanic individuals are one of the largest and fastest-growing ethnic groups in the USA and continue to face increased rates of food insecurity when compared with non-Hispanic Whites(Reference Coleman-Jensen, Rabbitt and Gregory2), even when traditional risk factors, like socio-economic status, are accounted for(Reference Myers and Painter27). In accordance with this expected population growth, it is imperative that public health professionals continue to explore determinants of food insecurity. Our study identified an association between language use, an understudied social determinant of health and food insecurity among Hispanic adults residing in the USA. We did not find support for the linguistic gradient – in which predominantly Spanish speakers would have had greater odds of food insecurity than bilinguals, whom in turn would have had greater odds of food insecurity than predominantly English speakers. Instead, we found that Spanish-speaking and bilingual Hispanic adults had increased odds of food insecurity when compared with their predominantly English-speaking counterparts only if they were born outside of the USA. Based on these findings, public health nutrition programs and policies should focus on removing structural barriers to food access and on providing linguistically and culturally appropriate food and nutrition assistance/services to individuals who need them.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Dr. Katherine P. Theall and Dr. Charles Stoecker, Professors at the Tulane School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine, for early input in the analysis plan. Authorship: Conceptualisation: M.A.L., M.P.C., M.F., J.M.F., A.G.; Methodology: M.A.L., M.P.C., M.F., J.M.F., A.G.; Data curation: M.A.L., M.P.C., M.F.; Formal analysis: M.A.L., M.P.C.; Supervision: M.P.C., M.F., J.M.F., A.G.; Original draft: M.A.L.; Draft review and edits: M.P.C., M.F., J.M.F., A.G.. Ethics of human subject participation: Not applicable.

Financial support:

This work was supported by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (NIMHDH) Minority Health and Health Disparities Research Training (MHRT) Program (T37) #T37MD001424 (Lopez). NIMHDH had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

Conflict of interest:

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (1996) Rome Declaration and Plan of Action. Rome, Italy. http://www.fao.org/docrep/003/w3613e/w3613e00.htm (accessed January 2022).Google Scholar
Coleman-Jensen, A, Rabbitt, MP, Gregory, CA et al. (2020) Household Food Security in the United States, 2019. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service.Google Scholar
Laraia, BA (2013) Food insecurity and chronic disease. Adv Nutr 4, 203212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, ME & Berkowitz, SA (2016) The relationship between food insecurity, dietary patterns, and obesity. Curr Nutr Rep 5, 5460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, NI & Story, MT (2011) Food insecurity and weight status among U.S. children and families: a review of the literature. Am J Prev Med 40, 166173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y & Eicher-Miller, HA (2021) Food insecurity and cardiovascular disease risk. Curr Atheroscler Rep 23, 24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, Y, Liu, B, Rong, S et al. (2020) Food insecurity is associated with cardiovascular and all-cause mortality among adults in the United States. J Am Heart Assoc 9, e014629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bureau. UC (2009) Hispanics in the United States. http://www.census.gov/population/www/socdemo/hispanic/files/Internet_Hispanic_in_US_2006.pdf (accessed January 2022).Google Scholar
Perez-Escamilla, R (2009) Dietary quality among Latinos: is acculturation making us sick? J Am Diet Assoc 109, 988991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazur, RE, Marquis, GS & Jensen, HH (2003) Diet and food insufficiency among Hispanic youths: acculturation and socioeconomic factors in the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 11201127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capps, R, Horowitz, A, Fortuny, K et al. (2009) Young children in immigrant families face higher risk of food insecurity. Child Trends 7, 17.Google Scholar
Kaiser, LL, Melgar-Quinonez, HR, Lamp, CL et al. (2002) Food security and nutritional outcomes of preschool-age Mexican-American children. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 924929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez, GA (2010) Language and power in healthcare: towards a theory of language barriers among linguistic minorities in the United States. Readings Lang Stud: Lang Power 2, 5974.Google Scholar
Kandula, NR, Lauderdale, DS & Baker, DW (2007) Differences in self-reported health among Asians, Latinos, and non-Hispanic whites: the role of language and nativity. Ann Epidemiol 17, 191198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponce, NA, Hays, RD & Cunningham, WE (2006) Linguistic disparities in health care access and health status among older adults. J Gen Intern Med 21, 786791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, EA, Karavolos, K, Rathouz, PJ et al. (2005) Limited English proficiency and breast and cervical cancer screening in a multiethnic population. Am J Public Health 95, 14101416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, EM, Chen, A & Cunningham, W (2007) Primary language and receipt of recommended health care among Hispanics in the United States. J Gen Intern Med 22, 283288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sentell, T, Shumway, M & Snowden, L (2007) Access to mental health treatment by English language proficiency and race/ethnicity. J Gen Intern Med 22, 289293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dave, JM, Evans, AE, Saunders, RP et al. (2009) Associations among food insecurity, acculturation, demographic factors, and fruit and vegetable intake at home in Hispanic Children. J Am Dietetic Assoc 109, 697701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, LC, Penedo, FJ, Espinosa de los Monteros, K et al. (2009) Resiliency in the face of disadvantage: do Hispanic cultural characteristics protect health outcomes? J Pers 77, 17071746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerman-Garber, I, Villa, AR & Caballero, E (2004) Diabetes and cardiovascular disease. Is there a true Hispanic paradox? Rev Invest Clin 56, 282296.Google Scholar
Franzini, L, Ribble, JC & Keddie, AM (2001) Understanding the Hispanic paradox. Ethn Dis 11, 496518.Google ScholarPubMed
Velasco-Mondragon, E, Jimenez, A, Palladino-Davis, AG et al. (2016) Hispanic health in the USA: a scoping review of the literature. Public Health Rev 37, 31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, A (2020) Identifying and countering white supremacy culture in food systems. World Food Policy Center 1, 110.Google Scholar
Burt, KG, Delgado, K, Chen, M et al. (2019) Strategies and recommendations to increase diversity in dietetics. J Acad Nutr Diet 119, 733738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, K (2021) A historical analysis of the Mediterranean Diet’s rise to prominence through the lens of Critical Race Theory. Crit Diet 5, 4152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, AM & Painter, MA (2017) Food insecurity in the United States of America: an examination of race/ethnicity and nativity. Food Secur 9, 14191432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowen, S, Elliott, S & Hardison-Moody, A (2021) The structural roots of food insecurity: how racism is a fundamental cause of food insecurity. Sociol Compass 15, e12846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CDC & National Center for Health Statistics (2012) National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Data CDC 2003–2012. Hyattsville (MD): US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, MP & Coleman-Jensen, A (2017) Rasch analyses of the standardized Spanish translation of the U.S. household food security survey module. J Econ Social Meas 42, 171187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altman, DG & Royston, P (2006) The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ 332, 10801081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bitler, M, Gennetian, LA, Gibson-Davis, C et al. (2021) Means-tested safety net programs and hispanic families: evidence from Medicaid, SNAP, and WIC. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci 696, 274305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammons, AJ, Hannon, BA, Teran-Garcia, M et al. (2019) Effects of culturally tailored nutrition education on dietary quality of Hispanic mothers: a randomized control trial. J Nutr Educ Behav 51, 11681176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, AG & Grinter, RE (2014) Collectivistic health promotion tools: accounting for the relationship between culture, food and nutrition. Int J Hum Comput Stud 72, 185206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, CR, Tuzzio, L, Rojas, M et al. (2004) How do Urban African Americans and Latinos view the influence of diet on hypertension? J Health Care for Poor Underserved 15, 631644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morey, BN (2018) Mechanisms by which anti-immigrant stigma exacerbates Racial/Ethnic Health Disparities. Am J Public Health 108, 460463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluszek, A & Dovidio, JF (2010) The way they speak: a social psychological perspective on the stigma of nonnative accents in communication. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 14, 214237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, JE, Birdsong, D, Bialystok, E et al. (2006) Degree of foreign accent in English sentences produced by Korean children and adults. J Phonetics 34, 153175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosoda, M, Dietz, J & Stone-Romero, E (2010) The effects of foreign accents on employment-related decisions. J Manag Psychol 25, 113132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Algert, SJ, Reibel, M & Renvall, MJ (2006) Barriers to participation in the food stamp program among food pantry clients in Los Angeles. Am J Public Health 96, 807809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winham, DM & Armstrong Florian, TL (2015) Nativity, not acculturation, predicts SNAP usage among low-income Hispanics with food insecurity. J Hunger Environ Nutr 10, 202213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertmann, FMW, Barroso, C, Ohri-Vachaspati, P et al. (2014) Women, infants, and children Cash Value Voucher (CVV) use in Arizona: a qualitative exploration of barriers and strategies related to fruit and vegetable purchases. J Nutr Educ Behavior 46, S53S58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munger, AL, Lloyd, TDS, Speirs, KE et al. (2015) More than just not enough: experiences of food insecurity for Latino immigrants. J Immigrant Minority Health 17, 15481556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, P, Martin, E & Bruno, R (2001) Language Use and Linguistic Isolation: Historical Data and Methodological Issues. Statistical Policy Working Paper 32: 2000 Seminar on Integrating Federal Statistical Information and Processes. Washington, DC: Federal Committee on Statistical Methodology, Office of Management and Budget. pp. 167190.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of NHANES’ Hispanic respondents by food (in)security, 1999–2018

Figure 1

Table 2 Multivariable logistic regression model between language use at home and food insecurity among NHANES’ Hispanic respondents, 1999–2018

Figure 2

Table 3 Adjusted multivariable logistic regression model between language use at home and food insecurity, and stratified by country of birth, among NHANES’ Hispanic respondents, 1999–2018

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Results of Adjusted Multivariable Logistic Regression Model between Language Use and Food Insecurity, Stratified by Country of Birth, among National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys’ Hispanic Respondents, 1999–2018. aUnited States encompasses the 50 US’ states and Washington D.C. and excludes US territories such as Puerto Rico