Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-08T12:10:24.419Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Thurstone's Discriminal Processes Fifty Years Later

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

R. Duncan Luce*
Affiliation:
Harvard University
*
Requests for reprints should be addressed to R, Duncan Luce, Department of Psychology and Social Relations, William James Halt, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138.

Abstract

Four issues are discussed concerning Thurstone's discriminal processes: the distributions governing the representation, the nature of the response decision rules, the relation of the mean representation to physical characteristics of the stimulus, and factors affecting the variance of the representation. A neural schema underlying the representation is proposed which involves samples in time of pulse trains on individual neural fibers, estimators of parameters of the several pulse trains, samples of neural fibers, and an aggregation of the estimates over the sample. The resulting aggregated estimate is the Thurstonian representation. Two estimators of pulse rate, which is monotonic with signal intensity, are timing and counting ratios and two methods of aggregation are averaging and maximizing. These lead to very different predictions in a speed-accuracy experiment; data indicate that both estimators are available and the aggregation is by averaging. Magnitude estimation data are then used both to illustrate an unusual response rule and to study the psychophysical law. In addition, the pattern of variability and correlation of magnitude estimates on successive trials is interpreted in terms of the sample size over which the aggregation takes place. Neural sample size is equated with selective attention, and is an important factor affecting the variability of the representation. It accounts for the magical number seven phenomenon in absolute identification and predicts the impact of nonuniform distributions of intensities on the absolute identification of two frequencies.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1977 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1977 Psychometric Society Presidential Address.

This work was supported in part by a grant of the National Science Foundation to Harvard University. I wish to express my appreciation to S. Burbeck, D. M. Green, M. Shaw, and B. Wandell for their useful comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

References

Reference Notes

Wandell, B., & Luce, R. D. Pooling peripheral information: averages versus extreme values. Unpublished manuscript, 1977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. M., Luce, R. D., & Duncan, J. E. Variability and sequential effects in magnitude production and estimation of auditory intensity. Perception & Psychophysics. Manuscript submitted for publication, 1977.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., & Green, D. M.Manuscript in preparation, 1977.Google Scholar

References

Bock, R. D., & Jones, L. V. The measurement and prediction of judgment and choice, 1968, San Francisco: Holden-Day.Google Scholar
Braida, L. D., & Durlach, N. I. Intensity perception. II. Resolution in one interval paradigms. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1972, 51, 483502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corso, J. F. The neural quantum theory of sensory discrimination. Psychological Bulletin, 1956, 53, 371393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durlach, N. I., & Braida, L. D. Intensity perception. I. Preliminary theory of intensity perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1969, 46, 372383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, H. Auditory patterns. Review of Modern Physics, 1940, 12, 4765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, W. R. An informational analysis of absolute judgments of loudness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1953, 46, 373380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gravetter, F., & Lockhead, G. R. Criterial range as a frame of reference for stimulus judgment. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 203216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. M., & Luce, R. D. Speed-accuracy tradeoff in auditory detection. In Kornblum, S.(Eds.), Attention and performance, IV. New York: Academic Press. 1973, 347569.Google Scholar
Green, D. M., & Luce, R. D. Variability of magnitude estimates: A timing theory analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 1974, 15, 291300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gumbel, E. J. Statistics of extremes, 1958, New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jesteadt, W., Luce, R. D., & Green, D. M. Sequential effects in judgments of loudness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1977, 3, 92104.Google ScholarPubMed
Jesteadt, W., Wier, C. C., & Green, D. M. Intensity discrimination as a function of frequency and sensation level. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1977, 61, 169177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiang, N. Y.-S. Discharge patterns of single fibers in the cat's auditory nerve, 1965, Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Krantz, D. H. Threshold theories of signal detection. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 308324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D. Individual choice behavior: A theoretical analysis, 1959, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D. A threshold theory for simple detection experiments. Psychological Review, 1963, 70, 6179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D., & Green, D. M. A neural timing theory for response times and the psychophysics of intensity. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 1457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D., & Green, D. M. The response ratio hypothesis for magnitude estimation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1974, 11, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D., & Green, D. M. Neural coding and psychophysical discrimination data. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1974, 56, 15541564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D., Green, D. M., & Weber, D. L. Attention bands in absolute identification. Perception & Psychophysics, 1976, 20, 4954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFadden, D. Conditional logit analysis of quantitative choice behavior. In Zarembka, P.(Eds.), Frontiers in econometrics. New York: Academic Press. 1974, 105142.Google Scholar
McGill, W. J. Neural counting mechanisms and energy detection in aduition. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1967, 4, 351376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 1956, 63, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D. A. Sensory thresholds, response biases, and the neural quantum theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1964, 1, 88120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, J. E., Brugge, J. F., Anderson, D. J., & Hind, J. E. Phase-locked response to low-frequency tones in single auditory nerve fibers of the squirrel monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1967, 30, 769793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanderson, A. C. Discrimination of neural coding parameters in the auditory system. IEEE Transactions, Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 1975, SMC-5, 533542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanderson, A. C. Comments on the paper ‘Discrimination of neural coding parameters in the auditory system.’ IEEE Transactions, Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 1977, in press.Google Scholar
Scharf, B. Critical bands. In Tobias, J. V.(Eds.), Foundations of modern auditory theory. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press. 1970, 159202.Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 153181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. A neural quantum in sensory discrimination. Science, 1972, 177, 749762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. Psychophysics, 1975, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S., Morgan, C. T., & Volkman, J. Theory of the neural quantum in the discrimination of loudness and pitch. American Journal of Psychology, 1941, 54, 315335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swets, J. A. Is there a sensory threshold?. Science, 1961, 134, 168177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. Psychophysical analysis. American Journal of Psychology, 1927, 38, 368389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review, 1927, 34, 273286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. Three psychophysical laws. Psychological Review, 1927, 34, 424432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgerson, W. S. Theory and methods of scaling, 1968, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Uttal, W. R., & Krissoff, M. Response of the somethetic system to patterned trains of electrical stimuli: an approach to the problem of sensory coding. In Kenshalo, D. R.(Eds.), The Skin Senses, 1965, Springfield, Ill.: Thomas.Google Scholar
Wandell, B. A. Speed-accuracy tradeoff in visual detection: Applications of neural counting and timing. Vision Research, 1977, 17, 217225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yellott, J. I. Jr. The relationship between Luce's choice axiom, Thurstone's theory of comparative judgment, and the double exponential distribution. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1977, 15, 109144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar