Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T17:04:43.207Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Within-individual relationships between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms: a longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 January 2024

Daiki Hiraoka*
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, University of Fukui, Fukui, Japan
Akiko Kawanami
Affiliation:
Center for Preventive Medical Sciences, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan
Kenichi Sakurai
Affiliation:
Center for Preventive Medical Sciences, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan
Chisato Mori
Affiliation:
Center for Preventive Medical Sciences, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Graduate School of Medicine, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan
*
Corresponding author: Daiki Hiraoka; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although the importance of the dynamic intra-individual relationship between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms has been widely recognized, the complex interplay between them is not well understood. Furthermore, the potential role of prenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament in this relationship remains unclear. This study aims to examine the bidirectional influence of mother-to-infant bonding on postpartum depressive symptoms within individuals and to elucidate whether prenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament would influence deviations from stable individual states.

Methods

Longitudinal data were collected from 433 women in early pregnancy. Of these, 360 participants completed the main questionnaires measuring impaired mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms at least once during the postpartum period. Data were collected at early and late pregnancy and several postpartum time points: shortly after birth and at one, four, ten, and 18 months postpartum. We also assessed prenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament. A random-intercept cross-lagged panel model was used.

Results

Within-individual variability in mother-to-infant bonding, especially anger and rejection, significantly predicted subsequent postpartum depressive symptoms. However, the inverse relationship was not significant. Additionally, prenatal depressive symptoms and difficult infant temperament were associated with greater within-individual variability in impaired mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms.

Conclusions

The present study demonstrated that the within-individual relationship between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms is likely non-bidirectional. The significance of the findings is underscored by the potential for interventions aimed at improving mother-to-infant bonding to alleviate postpartum depressive symptoms, suggesting avenues for future research and practice.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Mother-to-infant bonding refers to the affection a mother feels for her infant (Kinsey & Hupcey, Reference Kinsey and Hupcey2013). Although mother-to-infant bonding is a critical aspect of postpartum life, it varies among mothers. Some mothers experience impaired bonding, characterized by decreased affection and parenting behaviors that may affect their children's development (Fuchs, Möhler, Reck, Resch, & Kaess, Reference Fuchs, Möhler, Reck, Resch and Kaess2016; Kinsey & Hupcey, Reference Kinsey and Hupcey2013; Muzik et al., Reference Muzik, Bocknek, Broderick, Richardson, Rosenblum, Thelen and Seng2013; Yoshida, Yamashita, Conroy, Marks, & Kumar, Reference Yoshida, Yamashita, Conroy, Marks and Kumar2012). Understanding the factors contributing to mother-to-infant bonding and its impairment is essential for healthy parent–child relationships and child development.

Mothers with impaired mother-to-infant bonding often have comorbid postpartum depression (Tichelman et al., Reference Tichelman, Westerneng, Witteveen, van Baar, van der Horst, de Jonge and Peters2019). Postpartum depressive symptoms can interfere with mothers' parenting behaviors, resulting in less responsive interactions than mothers without postpartum depression (Barrett & Fleming, Reference Barrett and Fleming2011; O'Hara et al., Reference O'Hara, Mc Cabe, Hara, Mccabe, O'Hara and Mc Cabe2013). Consequently, mothers' depressive symptoms may negatively affect their infants' cognitive, emotional, motor, and language development (Slomian, Honvo, Emonts, Reginster, & Bruyère, Reference Slomian, Honvo, Emonts, Reginster and Bruyère2019).

The relationship between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms involves a complex interplay of potential reciprocal influences. Several cross-sectional studies have suggested such a reciprocal influence by reporting a positive association between impaired mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms (Dubber, Reck, Müller, & Gawlik, Reference Dubber, Reck, Müller and Gawlik2015; Mazúchová, Kelčíková, Maskalová, Malinovská, & Grendár, Reference Mazúchová, Kelčíková, Maskalová, Malinovská and Grendár2021; Motegi et al., Reference Motegi, Watanabe, Fukui, Ogawa, Hashijiri, Tsuboya and Someya2020; Nolvi et al., Reference Nolvi, Karlsson, Bridgett, Pajulo, Tolvanen and Karlsson2016; Nonnenmacher, Noe, Ehrenthal, & Reck, Reference Nonnenmacher, Noe, Ehrenthal and Reck2016; Taylor, Atkins, Kumar, Adams, & Glover, Reference Taylor, Atkins, Kumar, Adams and Glover2005; Tsuchida et al., Reference Tsuchida, Hamazaki, Matsumura, Miura, Kasamatsu, Inadera and Katoh2019). However, the cross-sectional design of these studies inherently limited their ability to delineate the direction of causality or elucidate the mechanisms underlying the relationship.

To address these limitations, longitudinal studies have examined the bidirectional and causal relationships between these two variables (Kasamatsu et al., Reference Kasamatsu, Tsuchida, Matsumura, Shimao, Hamazaki and Inadera2020; Kerstis et al., Reference Kerstis, Aarts, Tillman, Persson, Engström, Edlund and Skalkidou2016; Muzik et al., Reference Muzik, Bocknek, Broderick, Richardson, Rosenblum, Thelen and Seng2013; O'Higgins, Roberts, Glover, & Taylor, Reference O'Higgins, Roberts, Glover and Taylor2013; Radoš, Matijaš, Anđelinović, Čartolovni, & Ayers, Reference Radoš, Matijaš, Anđelinović, Čartolovni and Ayers2020; Stuijfzand, Garthus-Niegel, & Horsch, Reference Stuijfzand, Garthus-Niegel and Horsch2020), and some show that postpartum depressive symptoms can predict later impairment in mother-to-infant bonding (Kasamatsu et al., Reference Kasamatsu, Tsuchida, Matsumura, Shimao, Hamazaki and Inadera2020; O'Higgins et al., Reference O'Higgins, Roberts, Glover and Taylor2013). Wouk et al. (Reference Wouk, Gottfredson, Tucker, Pence, Meltzer-Brody, Zvara and Stuebe2019) found that mothers who did not experience positive emotions during infant feeding at two months postpartum had heightened depressive symptoms at six months and one year. Importantly, when models that include autoregression are used, the progressive relationship between the two variables may disappear. For instance, when both mother-to-infant bonding impairment and Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) scores were considered concurrently as independent variables, EPDS scores were not predictive of mother-to-infant bonding impairment at one year (O'Higgins et al., Reference O'Higgins, Roberts, Glover and Taylor2013). Similarly, a structural equation modeling analysis incorporating autoregressive and bidirectional paths between mother-to-infant bonding impairment and EPDS scores showed that the impact of mother-to-infant bonding impairment on postpartum depressive symptoms was significant but not vice versa (Ohara et al., 2017).

Distinguishing between between-individual and within-individual variation is essential for modeling the dynamic interplay between variables within individuals (Hamaker, Kuiper, & Grasman, Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). Previous studies of the relationship between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms have predominantly used between-individual analyses, which examine the relative positioning of individuals within a population (Dubber et al., Reference Dubber, Reck, Müller and Gawlik2015; Mazúchová et al., Reference Mazúchová, Kelčíková, Maskalová, Malinovská and Grendár2021; Motegi et al., Reference Motegi, Watanabe, Fukui, Ogawa, Hashijiri, Tsuboya and Someya2020; Nolvi et al., Reference Nolvi, Karlsson, Bridgett, Pajulo, Tolvanen and Karlsson2016; Nonnenmacher et al., Reference Nonnenmacher, Noe, Ehrenthal and Reck2016; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Atkins, Kumar, Adams and Glover2005; Tsuchida et al., Reference Tsuchida, Hamazaki, Matsumura, Miura, Kasamatsu, Inadera and Katoh2019). However, these analyses do not account for within-individual relationships, which refer to the associations between variables within each individual. Critically, findings derived from between-individual analyses should not be extrapolated to within-individual, as they may be overestimated or, in certain cases, directionally reversed (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). To understand the psychological mechanisms in postpartum mothers, it is necessary to distinguish and separately examine between- and within-individual relationships.

Beck (Reference Beck2001) underscored antenatal depressive symptoms as a key predictor of postpartum depression, and research showed that antenatal depressive symptoms predict lower postpartum mother-to-infant bonding (Hare, Kroll-Desrosiers, & Deligiannidis, Reference Hare, Kroll-Desrosiers and Deligiannidis2021; Rossen et al., Reference Rossen, Hutchinson, Wilson, Burns, A Olsson, Allsop and Mattick2016). Moreover, infant temperament was also identified as a predictor of postpartum depressive symptoms (Beck, Reference Beck2001; Zhao & Zhang, Reference Zhao and Zhang2020). Temperament reflects an infant's propensity to respond to their environment and is recognized as a factor that shapes parenting behavior (Belsky, Reference Belsky1984). Multiple studies have suggested that difficulty in soothing reduces parenting efficacy, hinders the development of bonding, and exacerbates depressive symptoms (Edhborg, Matthiesen, Lundh, & Widström, Reference Edhborg, Matthiesen, Lundh and Widström2005; Nolvi et al., Reference Nolvi, Karlsson, Bridgett, Pajulo, Tolvanen and Karlsson2016). Understanding the factors contributing to deviations from stable components within individuals can aid in predicting emotional instability and fluctuations in mental state.

Our study aimed to explore the dynamic relationship between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms at both the between- and within-individual levels while considering the roles of antenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament. Thus, we propose the following hypotheses: Based on previous research, we anticipated a positive correlation between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms at the individual level. However, due to the limitations of generalizing between-individual findings to within-individual relationships (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015), making specific predictions regarding within-individual associations is challenging. This study employs a random intercept cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). The RI-CLPM allows the modeling of stable, between-individual differences and temporal, within-individual dynamics between two or more constructs. Second, we expected antenatal depressive symptoms and perceived difficult infant temperament to positively predict within-individual fluctuations in mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms.

Method

Participants and procedures

This study included women enrolled in the Chiba Study of Mother and Child Health (C-MACH) who agreed to participate during their first trimester between 2014 and 2015. All participants gave birth at three local hospitals in the National Capital Region of Japan. A detailed description of the longitudinal birth study has been published previously (Sakurai et al., Reference Sakurai, Miyaso, Eguchi, Matsuno, Yamamoto and Todaka2016). The survey collected demographic information and depressive symptoms using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) during pregnancy. The Mother-to-Infant Bonding Scale (MIBS) and the EPDS were administered at several time points: shortly after birth (0 mo) and one month (1 mo), four months (4 mo), ten months (10 mo), and 18 months (18 mo) postpartum. At one month and four months postpartum, the participants evaluated their infants' temperaments based on crying patterns and reactions to being held. A total of 433 pregnant women provided written informed consent during early pregnancy. Due to various reasons such as miscarriage, stillbirth, change of hospital, relocation, or non-response to the questionnaire, the number of participants dwindled to 379 during childbirth. The analysis targeted 360 participants who responded to the MIBS and EPDS at least once. See Tables 1 and 2 for the number of responses collected for each variable.

Table 1. Demographic information

aMean (s.d.); n (%).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations of MIBS and EPDS at each time point

Note: MIBS, mother-to-infant bonding scale; EPDS, Edinburgh postnatal depression scale; mo, month; α, Cronbach's alpha.

We ran a sensitivity power analysis using the powRICLPM R package (Mulder, Reference Mulder2023), which allows power analysis for RI-CLPM. Our primary objective was to determine the minimum standardized cross-lagged coefficient at which the power exceeds 80%. We based our simulations on Model 2 (see Statistical analysis). Based on the Monte Carlo simulations, our sample size (N = 360) exceeded 80% power to detect cross-lagged effects of 0.10 in most parameter combinations. For a detailed procedure and result, please refer to the online Supplementary Information and Table S1.

The Biomedical Research Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Medicine, Chiba University, approved the study (ID:451, 1199, 1218, and 1239). This study was also approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Fukui (ID:20220091).

Measures

Mother-to-infant bonding scale

The MIBS is a self-rated questionnaire designed to assess bonding feelings with infants (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Atkins, Kumar, Adams and Glover2005; Yoshida et al., Reference Yoshida, Yamashita, Conroy, Marks and Kumar2012). A high score indicates poor mother-to-infant bonding. This study used the total score (0–30 points) of the 10 items. The MIBS showed adequate reliability in the present study at each time point (see Table 2). Yoshida et al. (Reference Yoshida, Yamashita, Conroy, Marks and Kumar2012) suggested that the MIBS has a two-factor structure. Lack of affection, composed of four items, reflects a lack of affection toward one's baby (lack of affection's α = 0.72 at the hospital; 0.66 at 1 month postpartum; 0.64 at 4 months postpartum; 0.7 at 10 months postpartum; 0.6 at 18 months postpartum). Anger/rejection, composed of four items, reflects feelings of anger or rejection toward one's baby (Anger/rejection's α = 0.35 at the hospital; 0.56 at 1 month postpartum; 0.59 at 4 months postpartum; 0.57 at 10 months postpartum; 0.57 at 18 months postpartum). The present study explored these two factors, in addition to the total score, to investigate the aspects of mother-to-infant bonding associated with postpartum depressive symptoms in greater detail.

Edinburgh postnatal depression scale

The EPDS is a commonly utilized self-rating tool for screening postnatal depression (Cox, Holden, & Sagovsky, Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987; Okano, Reference Okano1996). It includes 10 items and the total score ranged from 0 to 30, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. The EPDS was used to quantify postpartum depressive symptoms as a continuous variable. The reliability of the EPDS in this study was satisfactory at each time point (see Table 2).

Center for epidemiological studies depression scale

The CES-D is a self-reported instrument used to measure depressive symptoms in the general population. This scale has been shown to exhibit moderate to high positive correlations with depression symptoms rated by clinicians through interviews (Weissman, Sholomskas, Pottenger, Prusoff, & Locke, Reference Weissman, Sholomskas, Pottenger, Prusoff and Locke1977). It comprises 20 itemsFootnote Footnote 1, with respondents rating the frequency of their depression-related symptoms in the previous week. The total score ranged from 0 to 60, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. The CES-D was used to assess depressive symptoms during pregnancy. It was administered twice (during the first and last trimesters of pregnancy), and the average score was used in the analyses. The reliability of the CES-D in this study was satisfactory (early pregnancy: α = 0.82; late pregnancy: α = 0.83).

Infant temperament

Infant temperament was assessed at one and four months postpartum using three items: the frequency and ease of soothing the baby's crying; perceived difficulty in holding the baby due to fussiness, crying, or stiffness; and whether there were instances where the baby's crying could not be comforted. The items were adapted from the checklist utilized in The Japan Environment and Children's Study, a large-scale birth study (Morokuma et al., Reference Morokuma, Michikawa, Kato, Sanefuji, Shibata, Tsuji and Kusuhara2018; Nakahara et al., Reference Nakahara, Michikawa, Morokuma, Ogawa, Kato and Sanefuji2020). The answers were combined into a single variable reflecting the baby's difficult temperament using principal component analysis (PCA). A higher score on this composite variable indicates a more irritable temperament, characterized by frequent crying, difficulty soothing, and difficulty holding. The parallel analysis indicated that a single principal component was appropriate. The eigenvector from the one-month PCA was used to create a principal component score for temperament at four months.

Statistical analysis

We first calculated the bivariate correlations between the MIBS and EPDS scores at each time point. We also calculated the intraclass correlation coefficients for MIBS and EPDS. It represents the proportion of between-individual variance in the total variance. This step confirms the existence of significant within-individual variance, thereby justifying the use of the RI-CLPM.

The RI-CLPM, which splits the observed variables into between- and within-individual components, was employed to analyze the temporal dynamics between mother-to-infant bonding impairment and postpartum depressive symptoms (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015; Mulder & Hamaker, Reference Mulder and Hamaker2021). The random intercepts were latent variables with the observed MIBS and EPDS scores as its indicators, and fixing all the factor loadings to 1. Within-individual components are the deviations between expected and observed scores for an individual at each time point, and the RI-CLPM allows us to focus on the within-individual association by separating out the random intercepts.

We compare the goodness of fit of the four RI-CLPM models using the Satorra–Bentler scaled chi-square difference test. Model 1, the baseline model, included no constraints on the time-invariant parameters. Models 2, 3, and 4 included time-invariant constraints for the correlations and cross-lagged and autoregressive coefficients between within-individual components, grand means, and both coefficients and grand means, respectively.

Subsequent models introduced the MIBS subscale scores (lack of affection and anger/rejection) instead of the total MIBS score (Model 5), depressive symptoms during pregnancy (Model 6), and infant temperament (Model 7) as predictors. Time-invariant constraints were imposed on the paths from depressive symptoms during pregnancy to the within-individual components of the MIBS and EPDS at all five-time points.

Finally, sensitivity analyses were performed by introducing control variables, such as the age at early pregnancy, education, household income, parity, gestational age, child sex, birth weight, and depression history. In addition, to consider the influence of marital status on the results, we omitted two unmarried participants and conducted the analysis.

We employed the maximum likelihood robust (MLR) estimator to accommodate modest deviations from normality (Kline, Reference Kline2015) and handled missing data using full information maximum likelihood estimation. As delineated in Table 2, all indicators exhibited slightly positive values for skewness and kurtosis, yet they did not surpass the thresholds indicative of serious non-normality (skewness beyond ± 3.0 and kurtosis beyond ± 10.0; Kline, Reference Kline2015).

All analyses were conducted using R 4.2.1 and the lavaan package (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics of MIBS and EPDS at each time point are presented in Table 2. MIBS and EPDS scores were positively correlated at the same time points. Furthermore, the MIBS scores positively correlated with the EPDS scores of the subsequent wave and vice versa.

Intra-class correlation coefficients

The intra-class correlations for the MIBS and EPDS scores were 0.55 and 0.46, respectively. The intraclass correlations indicated a well-balanced representation of both between-individual variance (variations among different mothers) and within-individual variance (changes within the same mother over time) in the MIBS and EPDS scores, thereby highlighting the significance of considering both inter-and intra-individual variability in the analysis.

RI-CLPM of MIBS and EPDS

Table 3 presents the results of the model comparisons of each RI-CLPM model. Among the different models tested, Model 2, which included time-invariant constraints on the cross-lagged and autoregressive coefficients and correlations, did not show a significant decrease in model fit compared with the baseline model and had the lowest Bayesian information criterion values. Based on these findings, we selected Model 2 for subsequent analyses and interpretations.

Table 3. Model fit indices and comparisons of the RI-CLPM

Note: RI-CLPM, random intercept cross-lagged panel model; CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean squared error of approximation; AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion.

The relationship between the random intercepts of the MIBS and the EPDS was examined within the context of the RI-CLPM to explore the association between the two factors at the between-individual level. Consistent with previous research and our hypotheses, we observed a significant positive correlation between the time-invariant components of MIBS and EPDS (online Supplementary Table S2 and Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Standardized path coefficients from Model 2 for the Mother-to-infant Bonding Scale (MIBS) and the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. RI, random intercept; W, within-individual component.

Online Supplementary Table S2 and Fig. 1 present the significant positive correlations observed between the within-individual components of MIBS and EPDS at each time point. These findings indicate that an individual's deviation from the average level of mother-to-infant bonding at a specific point likely corresponds to a similar deviation in postpartum depressive symptoms.

2Analysis of the dynamics among variables at the within-individual level and the cross-lagged coefficients of the within-individual components showed that changes in the MIBS within an individual significantly predicted subsequent changes in the EPDS. This finding suggests that if an individual's mother-to-infant bonding deteriorates at a specific time point beyond the individual-average level, postpartum depressive symptoms at the subsequent time point are expected to be more severe than the individual-average. In contrast, the cross-lagged coefficients from the EPDS to the MIBS were not significant. This indicates that the temporary exacerbation of postpartum depressive symptoms beyond the individual-average level may not significantly influence fluctuations in mother-to-infant bonding.

RI-CLPM of the MIBS subscales and EPDS

We conducted an analysis using the RI-CLPM with two MIBS subscales. For Model 5, the total MIBS score in Model 2 was replaced with the lack of affection and anger/rejection scores. This model demonstrated an acceptable fit (CFI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.043).

Regarding within-individual variations, deviations from the individual-average level of anger/rejection at one-time point positively predicted next deviations from the individual-average level of postpartum depressive symptoms (online Supplementary Table S3 and Fig. 2). This finding suggests that if an individual experiences a higher level of anger/rejection toward their child than in their usual state, their postpartum depressive symptoms will be more severe at the next time point. However, deviations from the individual-average lack of affection levels did not significantly contribute to deviations from individual-average EPDS levels at the next time point.

Figure 2. Standardized path coefficients from Model 5 for the Lack of affection (LA) and Anger/rejection (AR) from the Mother-to-infant Bonding Scale and the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). Correlations between within-individual components are omitted for improved visibility. See online Supplementary Table S3 for their values. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. RI, random intercept; W, within-individual component.

Impact of prenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament on within-individual variations of postpartum MIBS and EPDS scores

Prenatal depressive symptoms

We examined the impact of prenatal depressive symptoms on within-individual variations in MIBS and EPDS scores. In Model 2, we introduced the prenatal CES-D score as an explanatory variable and assessed its paths to the within-individual components of the MIBS and EPDS. Model 6 demonstrated an acceptable fit (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.046).

Notably, the CES-D scores significantly and positively predicted the within-individual component scores of the MIBS and EPDS (online Supplementary Table S4). This indicates that higher levels of antenatal depressive symptoms are associated with greater deviations from the average levels of both mother-to-infant bonding impairment and postpartum depressive symptoms.

Infant temperament

We subsequently examined the impact of infant temperament at one and four months postpartum on the within-individual components of the MIBS and EPDS at the same time points. Model 7 demonstrated an acceptable fit (CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.059).

At one and four months postpartum, a difficult infant temperament positively predicted deviations from stable MIBS and EPDS scores (online Supplementary Table S5). This finding suggests that mothers who perceived their infants difficult displayed lower levels of mother-to-infant bonding and increased postpartum depressive symptoms compared to their own average levels.

Sensitivity analysis

Sensitivity analyses included control variables in each RI-CLPM model. The models showed a good fit (online Supplementary Table S6). Generally, higher income levels correlated with lower MIBS scores, and older maternal age and no depression history correlated with decreased EPDS scores, aligning with previous research (Bottino, Nadanovsky, Moraes, Reichenheim, & Lobato, Reference Bottino, Nadanovsky, Moraes, Reichenheim and Lobato2012; Tichelman et al., Reference Tichelman, Westerneng, Witteveen, van Baar, van der Horst, de Jonge and Peters2019). Overall, including these control variables produced similar results with main analyses (online Supplementary Tables S7–S10). Online Supplementary Table S11 shows the results of Model 2 in the data set except for two unmarried participants. Again, this model did not significantly change the results. Thus, our findings remain robust when considering the influence of these control variables.

Discussion

This study investigated the relationship between mother-to-infant bonding impairment and postpartum depressive symptoms at the within-individual level. Using the RI-CLPM, we demonstrated that at the between-individual level, mothers who experienced mother-to-infant bonding impairment were more likely to display postpartum depressive symptoms. In addition to the between-individual associations, our results further demonstrated that when mothers exhibited a higher degree of mother-to-infant bonding impairment than the individual-average levels, these were followed by occasions when they experienced higher postpartum depressive symptoms than the individual-average levels. However, deviations in postpartum depressive symptoms did not significantly influence mother-to-infant bonding impairment at subsequent time points. In addition, prenatal depressive symptoms and difficult infant temperament were associated with exacerbations of mother-to-infant bonding impairments and postpartum depressive symptoms.

This study employed RI-CLPM to examine the separate contributions of between- and within-individual variations. Consistent with previous studies (Dubber et al., Reference Dubber, Reck, Müller and Gawlik2015; Mazúchová et al., Reference Mazúchová, Kelčíková, Maskalová, Malinovská and Grendár2021; Motegi et al., Reference Motegi, Watanabe, Fukui, Ogawa, Hashijiri, Tsuboya and Someya2020; Nolvi et al., Reference Nolvi, Karlsson, Bridgett, Pajulo, Tolvanen and Karlsson2016; Nonnenmacher et al., Reference Nonnenmacher, Noe, Ehrenthal and Reck2016; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Atkins, Kumar, Adams and Glover2005; Tsuchida et al., Reference Tsuchida, Hamazaki, Matsumura, Miura, Kasamatsu, Inadera and Katoh2019), we observed a positive correlation between time-invariant mother-to-infant bonding impairments and postpartum depressive symptoms. Our results revealed a novel within-individual relationship: Increases in mother-to-infant bonding impairment predicted subsequent increases in postpartum depressive symptoms within individuals. Notably, this relationship was unidirectional because changes in postpartum depressive symptoms did not significantly predict subsequent changes in mother-to-infant bonding impairments. According to Orth et al. (Reference Orth, Meier, Bühler, Dapp, Krauss, Messerli and Robins2022), benchmarks for interpreting standardized cross-lagged effects in RI-CLPM are 0.03 for small, 0.07 for medium, and 0.12 for large effects. In our study, the standardized cross-lagged effects from the MIBS to the EPDS ranged from 0.10 to 0.12, indicating medium to large effect sizes. The analysis revealed a particularly notable influence when focusing on the anger/rejection subscale of the MIBS, with standardized cross-lagged effects ranging from 0.14 to 0.20. This is greater than either the effects of total MIBS or the lack of affection subscale. Contrastingly, the standardized cross-lagged effects from the EPDS to the MIBS were less than the small effect (0.03) and were not statistically significant. These findings suggest focusing on anger/rejection toward the infant may be worthwhile to better understand their influence on postpartum depressive symptoms. Previous meta-analyses have shown that self-esteem is a robust factor and mechanism underlying postpartum depression (Beck, Reference Beck2001; Zhao & Zhang, Reference Zhao and Zhang2020). Self-esteem refers to one's satisfaction with oneself and the degree of alignment between one's ideal and actual selves (Silber & Tippett, Reference Silber and Tippett1965). Low self-esteem is associated with feelings of guilt and shame (Budiarto & Helmi, Reference Budiarto and Helmi2021), and parental guilt has been linked to depressive symptoms (Derella & Milan, Reference Derella and Milan2021). The highest reports of guilt tend to occur when caregivers direct their anger and actions toward the child (Rotkirch & Janhunen, Reference Rotkirch and Janhunen2010). In our study, mothers may have experienced guilt and shame when they felt anger/rejection and there was a significant discrepancy between their ideal image of parenting and their actual experience. This decrease in self-esteem may have triggered an increase in depressive symptoms.

The finding that postpartum depressive symptoms do not necessarily exacerbate mother-to-infant bonding impairments in the same individual may initially appear inconsistent with the results of many cross-sectional studies. However, O'Higgins et al. (Reference O'Higgins, Roberts, Glover and Taylor2013) reported that the effect of postpartum depressive symptoms becomes non-significant when postpartum depressive symptoms and mother-to-infant bonding are simultaneously entered as independent variables, with later mother-to-infant bonding as the dependent variable. Moreover, several interventional studies indicated that improvements in postpartum depressive symptoms do not directly lead to enhanced mother-to-infant bonding (O'Mahen et al., Reference O'Mahen, Richards, Woodford, Wilkinson, McGinley, Taylor and Warren2014; Posmontier, Neugebauer, Stuart, Chittams, & Shaughnessy, Reference Posmontier, Neugebauer, Stuart, Chittams and Shaughnessy2016). This suggests that the experience of postpartum depressive symptoms alone may not be sufficient to determine the formation of mother-to-infant bonding at the between- and within-individual levels.

The current findings also indicate that prenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament play a significant role in the intra-individual variations in postnatal mental health. Mothers with higher prenatal depressive symptoms experience greater fluctuations in their mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms. Furthermore, the model incorporating prenatal depressive symptoms revealed an interesting trend where the cross-lagged effect from MIBS to EPDS within individuals became non-significant. This study focused on the nearly 50% intra-individual variance observed in the MIBS and EPDS scores. However, it is equally critical to consider the remaining variance accounted for by inter-individual differences. Prenatal depressive symptoms might play a significant role in explaining both inter-individual and intra-individual variances. Prenatal depression is a significant risk factor for postpartum depression (Beck, Reference Beck2001; Zhao & Zhang, Reference Zhao and Zhang2020), and while the necessity for early intervention is widely acknowledged, the mechanisms underlying the persistence of depressive symptoms throughout the perinatal period remain largely unexplored. Our findings pave the way for a more nuanced understanding of instability in postpartum mental health, particularly by highlighting the significance of deviations from a stable state.

Additionally, the perception of an infant having a difficult temperament plays a role in the temporary exacerbation of mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms. Dealing with a baby who frequently cries and has difficulty soothing can be stressful and emotionally taxing. Crying is a parenting stressor; when a baby cannot be soothed, parents' sense of efficacy can decrease situationally (Verhage, Oosterman, & Schuengel, Reference Verhage, Oosterman and Schuengel2013). Furthermore, mothers who frequently experience an inability to soothe their crying infants often report more severe postpartum depressive symptoms (Radesky et al., Reference Radesky, Zuckerman, Silverstein, Rivara, Barr, Taylor and Barr2013). These results emphasize the need for individualized support according to the temperament of the infant.

Our study had several limitations. First, the MIBS, EPDS, and CES-D utilized in this study are all self-reporting measures. While these measures are widely used and have been validated, self-report methods might not fully capture the nuances and complexities of mental health. Future research should integrate clinical interviews to provide a more comprehensive assessment of mothers' mental health. Second, our study lacked a control group or an examination of other populations. Our sample predominantly comprised married and well-educated Japanese women, which may constrain the broader applicability of our findings, as cultural variations might influence parenting norms and associated feelings of guilt (Rotkirch & Janhunen, Reference Rotkirch and Janhunen2010). Additionally, due to sample size and power issues, we were unable to conduct a moderation analysis to split the sample in this study. In the future, it would be significant to examine whether the association between mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms is moderated for groups with depressive symptom severity, marital status, and parity in a larger sample size. Third, in this study, the three-item checklist used to assess infant difficult temperament has not been confirmed for reliability and validity, lacking the extensive item range found in standardized tools. Consequently, it is not guaranteed to fully encapsulate the multifaceted nature of infant temperament. Future research is encouraged to utilize tools for a more valid and comprehensive evaluation of infant temperament. Finally, our study focused exclusively on the mother-to-infant dyad and neglected the potential influence of other family members. Future research should investigate the dynamics of these relationships within the broader family context.

This study applied the RI-CLPM to mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms, key postnatal mental health concerns. We found that deterioration in mother-to-infant bonding was significantly predictive of subsequent increases in postpartum depressive symptoms. Specifically, feelings of anger/rejection emerged as significant predictors. However, the effects of postpartum depressive symptoms on mother-to-infant bonding were not statistically significant. This underscores that their interactions are not reciprocal, shedding light on facets that differ from inter-individual perspectives. These findings have profound implications for practical interventions. Interventions targeting mother-to-infant bonding may improve subsequent mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms. Focusing on intra-individual dynamics makes it plausible that interventions aimed at improving mother-to-infant bonding, particularly reducing feelings of anger/rejection toward the child, might alleviate postpartum depressive symptoms. For example, given that reappraisal techniques and mindfulness interventions have been shown to be effective in reducing anger (Szasz, Szentagotai, & Hofmann, Reference Szasz, Szentagotai and Hofmann2011; Wright, Day, & Howells, Reference Wright, Day and Howells2009), their use postnatally could potentially enhance mother-to-infant bonding, thereby preventing escalation of postpartum depressive symptoms. In light of existing research demonstrating a significant link between prenatal and postnatal bonding experiences (Tichelman et al., Reference Tichelman, Westerneng, Witteveen, van Baar, van der Horst, de Jonge and Peters2019), interventions for pregnant women regarding feelings of bonding with their fetus may also be effective. In addition, our research has demonstrated that prenatal depressive symptoms and difficult child temperament contribute to pronounced fluctuations in both mother-to-infant bonding and postpartum depressive symptoms. Recent research has highlighted that mood unpredictability and instability during the perinatal period have a negative impact on child development (Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Howland, Sandman, Davis, Phelan, Baram and Stern2018; Ugarte & Hastings, Reference Ugarte and Hastings2023). Therefore, interventions tailored to prenatal depressive symptoms and infant temperament that may precede mood instability may promote postpartum emotional stability and benefit both mother and child; future empirical studies are warranted.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723003707

Author contributions

AK, KS, and CM were responsible for planning and implementing the birth longitudinal studies and data collection. DH designed the analysis plan and performed the analyses. DH and AK interpreted the results and drafted the manuscript. All the authors have reviewed and revised the manuscript.

Funding statement

This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers 20241016, 15H06087, 16H01781, 21KK0236, and 21K13548 and grants from the Chiba Foundation for Health Promotion & Disease Prevention.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work complied with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Data availability

The R code and Rmarkdown files used for data analysis and manuscript preparation are shared in the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/7pu8r/?view_only=444ad74e63574341a15f1041ac5efeef). The raw data were part of an ongoing large longitudinal study and are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Because of an error in the questionnaire setup, only 18 of the 20 items of the CES-D were used. To rectify this, the full 20-item CES-D was administered to the same sample 18 months postpartum. The correlation between the total scores of the 20 items and 18 items was very high (r = 0.98, p < 0.001), suggesting that the 18-item version was an adequate reflection of depressive symptoms. Consequently, we decided to proceed with the analyses using the 18-item version.

References

Barrett, J., & Fleming, A. S. (2011). Annual research review : All mothers are not created equal: Neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 52, 368397. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02306.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, C. T. (2001). Predictors of postpartum depression: An update. Nursing Research, 50, 275285. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200109000-00004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 8396. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00275.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottino, M. N., Nadanovsky, P., Moraes, C. L., Reichenheim, M. E., & Lobato, G. (2012). Reappraising the relationship between maternal age and postpartum depression according to the evolutionary theory: Empirical evidence from a survey in primary health services. Journal of Affective Disorders, 142, 219224. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Budiarto, Y., & Helmi, A. F. (2021). Shame and self-esteem: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Psychological Assessment: Official Organ of the European Association of Psychological Assessment, 17, 131145. doi: 10.5964/ejop.2115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10–item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 150, 782786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derella, O. J., & Milan, S. (2021). I felt like a terrible mom: Parenting-related cognitive processes maintaining maternal depression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 30, 24272439. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-02053-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubber, S., Reck, C., Müller, M., & Gawlik, S. (2015). Postpartum bonding: The role of perinatal depression, anxiety and maternal-fetal bonding during pregnancy. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 18, 187195. doi: 10.1007/s00737-014-0445-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edhborg, M., Matthiesen, A.-S., Lundh, W., & Widström, A.-M. (2005). Some early indicators for depressive symptoms and bonding 2 months postpartum – a study of new mothers and fathers. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 8, 221231. doi: 10.1007/s00737-005-0097-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, A., Möhler, E., Reck, C., Resch, F., & Kaess, M. (2016). The early mother-to-child bond and Its unique prospective contribution to child behavior evaluated by mothers and teachers. Psychopathology, 49, 211216. doi: 10.1159/000445439CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glynn, L. M., Howland, M. A., Sandman, C. A., Davis, E. P., Phelan, M., Baram, T. Z., & Stern, H. S. (2018). Prenatal maternal mood patterns predict child temperament and adolescent mental health. Journal of Affective Disorders, 228, 8390. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102116. doi: 10.1037/a0038889CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, M. M., Kroll-Desrosiers, A., & Deligiannidis, K. M. (2021). Peripartum depression: Does risk versus diagnostic status impact mother–infant bonding and perceived social support? Depression and Anxiety, 38, 390399. doi: 10.1002/da.23121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasamatsu, H., Tsuchida, A., Matsumura, K., Shimao, M., Hamazaki, K., Inadera, H., & Japan Environment and Children's Study Group. (2020). Understanding the relationship between postpartum depression one month and six months after delivery and mother-infant bonding failure one-year after birth: Results from the Japan Environment and Children's study (JECS). Psychological Medicine, 50, 161169. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719002101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerstis, B., Aarts, C., Tillman, C., Persson, H., Engström, G., Edlund, B., … Skalkidou, A. (2016). Association between parental depressive symptoms and impaired bonding with the infant. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 19, 8794. doi: 10.1007/s00737-015-0522-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinsey, C. B., & Hupcey, J. E. (2013). State of the science of maternal–infant bonding: A principle-based concept analysis. Midwifery, 29, 13141320. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2012.12.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mazúchová, L., Kelčíková, S., Maskalová, E., Malinovská, N., & Grendár, M. (2021). Mother-infant bonding and its associated factors during postpartum period. Kontakt, 23, 126132. doi: 10.32725/kont.2021.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morokuma, S., Michikawa, T., Kato, K., Sanefuji, M., Shibata, E., Tsuji, M., … Kusuhara, K. (2018). Non-reassuring foetal status and neonatal irritability in the Japan environment and children's study: A cohort study. Scientific Reports, 8, 15853. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34231-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motegi, T., Watanabe, Y., Fukui, N., Ogawa, M., Hashijiri, K., Tsuboya, R., … Someya, T. (2020). Depression, anxiety and primiparity are negatively associated with mother-infant bonding in Japanese mothers. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 16, 31173122. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S287036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, J. D. (2023). Power analysis for the random intercept cross-lagged panel model using the powRICLPM R-package. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 30, 645658. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2022.2122467CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28, 638648. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muzik, M., Bocknek, E. L., Broderick, A., Richardson, P., Rosenblum, K. L., Thelen, K., & Seng, J. S. (2013). Mother-infant bonding impairment across the first 6 months postpartum: The primacy of psychopathology in women with childhood abuse and neglect histories. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 16, 2938. doi: 10.1007/s00737-012-0312-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakahara, K., Michikawa, T., Morokuma, S., Ogawa, M., Kato, K., Sanefuji, M., … Japan Environment and Children's Study Group. (2020). Association of maternal sleep before and during pregnancy with preterm birth and early infant sleep and temperament. Scientific Reports, 10, 11084. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-67852-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolvi, S., Karlsson, L., Bridgett, D. J., Pajulo, M., Tolvanen, M., & Karlsson, H. (2016). Maternal postnatal psychiatric symptoms and infant temperament affect early mother-infant bonding. Infant Behavior & Development, 43, 1323. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nonnenmacher, N., Noe, D., Ehrenthal, J. C., & Reck, C. (2016). Postpartum bonding: The impact of maternal depression and adult attachment style. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 19, 927935. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0648-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Hara, M. W., Mc Cabe, J. E., Hara, M. W. O., Mccabe, J. E., O'Hara, M. W., & Mc Cabe, J. E. (2013). Postpartum depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 379407. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Higgins, M., Roberts, I. S. J., Glover, V., & Taylor, A. (2013). Mother-child bonding at 1 year; associations with symptoms of postnatal depression and bonding in the first few weeks. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 16, 381389. doi: 10.1007/s00737-013-0354-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okano, T. (1996). Validation and reliability of Japanese version of the EPDS. Archives of Psychiatric Diagnostics and Clinical Evaluation, 7, 525533. Retrieved from https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1570291225900397312Google Scholar
O'Mahen, H. A., Richards, D. A., Woodford, J., Wilkinson, E., McGinley, J., Taylor, R. S., & Warren, F. C. (2014). Netmums: A phase II randomized controlled trial of a guided internet behavioural activation treatment for postpartum depression. Psychological Medicine, 44, 16751689. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orth, U., Meier, L. L., Bühler, J. L., Dapp, L. C., Krauss, S., Messerli, D., & Robins, R. W. (2022). Effect size guidelines for cross-lagged effects. Psychological Methods, 13. doi: 10.1037/met0000499.Google ScholarPubMed
Posmontier, B., Neugebauer, R., Stuart, S., Chittams, J., & Shaughnessy, R. (2016). Telephone-administered interpersonal psychotherapy by nurse-midwives for postpartum depression. Journal of Midwifery & Women's Health, 61, 456466. doi: 10.1111/jmwh.12411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radesky, J. S., Zuckerman, B., Silverstein, M., Rivara, F. P., Barr, M., Taylor, J. A., … Barr, R. G. (2013). Inconsolable infant crying and maternal postpartum depressive symptoms. Pediatrics, 131, e1857e1864. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-3316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radoš, S. N., Matijaš, M., Anđelinović, M., Čartolovni, A., & Ayers, S. (2020). The role of posttraumatic stress and depression symptoms in mother-infant bonding. Journal of Affective Disorders, 268, 134140. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Retrieved from http://www.jstatsoft.org/v48/i02/Google Scholar
Rossen, L., Hutchinson, D., Wilson, J., Burns, L., A Olsson, C., Allsop, S., … Mattick, R. P. (2016). Predictors of postnatal mother-infant bonding: The role of antenatal bonding, maternal substance use and mental health. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 19, 609622. doi: 10.1007/s00737-016-0602-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotkirch, A., & Janhunen, K. (2010). Maternal guilt. Evolutionary Psychology: An International Journal of Evolutionary Approaches to Psychology and Behavior, 8, 90106. doi: 10.1177/147470491000800108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakurai, K., Miyaso, H., Eguchi, A., Matsuno, Y., Yamamoto, M., Todaka, E., … Chiba study of Mother and Children's Health Group. (2016). Chiba study of Mother and Children's Health (C-MACH): Cohort study with omics analyses. BMJ Open, 6, e010531. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010531CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silber, E., & Tippett, J. S. (1965). Self-Esteem: Clinical assessment and measurement validation. Psychological Reports, 16, 10171071. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1965.16.3c.1017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slomian, J., Honvo, G., Emonts, P., Reginster, J. Y., & Bruyère, O. (2019). Consequences of maternal postpartum depression: A systematic review of maternal and infant outcomes. Women's Health, 15, 1745506519844044. doi: 10.1177/1745506519844044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuijfzand, S., Garthus-Niegel, S., & Horsch, A. (2020). Parental birth-related PTSD symptoms and bonding in the early postpartum period: A prospective population-based cohort study. Frontiers in Psychiatry / Frontiers Research Foundation, 11, 570727. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.570727CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szasz, P. L., Szentagotai, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2011). The effect of emotion regulation strategies on anger. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 114119. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, A., Atkins, R., Kumar, R., Adams, D., & Glover, V. (2005). A new mother-to-infant bonding scale: Links with early maternal mood. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 8, 4551. doi: 10.1007/s00737-005-0074-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tichelman, E., Westerneng, M., Witteveen, A. B., van Baar, A. L., van der Horst, H. E., de Jonge, A., … Peters, L. L. (2019). Correlates of prenatal and postnatal mother-to-infant bonding quality: A systematic review. PloS One, 14, e0222998. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0222998CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuchida, A., Hamazaki, K., Matsumura, K., Miura, K., Kasamatsu, H., Inadera, H., … Katoh, T. (2019). Changes in the association between postpartum depression and mother-infant bonding by parity: Longitudinal results from the Japan environment and children's study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 110, 110116. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.11.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ugarte, E., & Hastings, P. D. (2023). Assessing unpredictability in caregiver–child relationships: Insights from theoretical and empirical perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 120. doi: 10.1017/S0954579423000305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhage, M. L., Oosterman, M., & Schuengel, C. (2013). Parenting self-efficacy is associated with cry perception, not autonomic responses, during a cry response task. Parenting, Science and Practice, 13, 253265. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2013.832570CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissman, M. M., Sholomskas, D., Pottenger, M., Prusoff, B. A., & Locke, B. Z. (1977). Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: A validation study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 106, 203214. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wouk, K., Gottfredson, N. C., Tucker, C., Pence, B. W., Meltzer-Brody, S., Zvara, B., … Stuebe, A. M. (2019). Positive emotions during infant feeding and postpartum mental health. Journal of Women's Health, 28, 194202. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2017.6889CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, S., Day, A., & Howells, K. (2009). Mindfulness and the treatment of anger problems. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 396401. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2009.06.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshida, K., Yamashita, H., Conroy, S., Marks, M., & Kumar, C. (2012). A Japanese version of mother-to-infant bonding scale: Factor structure, longitudinal changes and links with maternal mood during the early postnatal period in Japanese mothers. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 15, 343352. doi: 10.1007/s00737-012-0291-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, X.-H., & Zhang, Z.-H. (2020). Risk factors for postpartum depression: An evidence-based systematic review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 53, 102353. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations of MIBS and EPDS at each time point

Figure 2

Table 3. Model fit indices and comparisons of the RI-CLPM

Figure 3

Figure 1. Standardized path coefficients from Model 2 for the Mother-to-infant Bonding Scale (MIBS) and the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. RI, random intercept; W, within-individual component.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Standardized path coefficients from Model 5 for the Lack of affection (LA) and Anger/rejection (AR) from the Mother-to-infant Bonding Scale and the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). Correlations between within-individual components are omitted for improved visibility. See online Supplementary Table S3 for their values. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. RI, random intercept; W, within-individual component.

Supplementary material: File

Hiraoka et al. supplementary material

Hiraoka et al. supplementary material
Download Hiraoka et al. supplementary material(File)
File 122.8 KB