Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T09:09:31.595Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stimulus valence, episodic memory, and the priming of brain activation profiles in borderline personality disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2021

Morgan Szczepaniak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University, Detroit, USA
Asadur Chowdury
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University, Detroit, USA
Paul H. Soloff
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, USA
Vaibhav A. Diwadkar*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University, Detroit, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Vaibhav A. Diwadkar, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is characterized by instability in affective regulation that can result in a loss of cognitive control. Triggers may be neuronal responses to emotionally valenced context and/or stimuli. ‘Neuronal priming’ indexes the familiarity of stimuli, and may capture the obligatory effects of affective valence on the brain's processing system, and how such valence mediates responses to the repeated presentation of stimuli. We investigated the effects of affective valence of stimuli on neuronal priming (i.e. changes in activation to repeated presentation of stimuli), and if these effects distinguished BPD patients from controls.

Methods

Forty BPD subjects and 25 control subjects (age range: 18–44) participated in an episodic memory task during fMRI. Stimuli were presented in alternating epochs of encoding (six images of positive, negative, and neutral valence) and recognition (six images for ‘old’ v. ‘new’ recognition). Analyses focused on inter-group differences in the change in activation to repeated stimuli (presented during Encoding and Recognition).

Results

Relative to controls, BPD showed greater priming (generally greater decrease from encoding to recognition) for negatively valenced stimuli. Conversely, BPD showed less priming for positively valenced stimuli (generally greater increase from encoding to recognition).

Conclusion

Plausibly, the relative familiarity of negative valence to patients with BPD exerts an influence on obligatory responses to repeated stimuli leading to repetition priming of neuronal profiles. The specific effects of valence on memory and/or attention, and consequently on priming can inform the understanding of mechanisms of altered salience for affective stimuli in BPD.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is characterized by instability in affective regulation and self-image (Lieb, Zanarini, Schmahl, Linehan, & Bohus, Reference Lieb, Zanarini, Schmahl, Linehan and Bohus2004). These psychological traits express themselves in abnormal functional responses and network interactions in the BPD brain (Sala et al., Reference Sala, Caverzasi, Marraffini, De Vidovich, Lazzaretti, d'Allio and Brambilla2009). Task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies are particularly valuable in evoking altered response profiles in BPD, and relying on behavioral domains like episodic memory to evoke responses can be particularly useful (Yonelinas, Ranganath, Ekstrom, & Wiltgen, Reference Yonelinas, Ranganath, Ekstrom and Wiltgen2019). Episodic memory requires participants to encode and subsequently recognize previously presented stimuli, and can evoke implicit responses to stimulus valence. Stimulus repetition leads to adaptations in fMRI-measured responses (Grill-Spector & Malach, Reference Grill-Spector and Malach2001; Krekelberg, Boynton, & van Wezel, Reference Krekelberg, Boynton and van Wezel2006) that can be relatively invariant of task demands (Xu, Turk-Browne, & Chun, Reference Xu, Turk-Browne and Chun2007). Thus, changes in fMRI-indexed responses provide viable mesoscopic representations of how neuronal responses attenuate to repeated exposure to stimuli (a process generally referred to as neuronal priming) (de Aquino Ferreira, Queiroz Pereira, Neri Benevides, & Aguiar Melo, Reference de Aquino Ferreira, Queiroz Pereira, Neri Benevides and Aguiar Melo2018). Processes of adaptation (or enhancement) under stimulus repetition are a mechanism that in their sensitivity to stimulus valence, may reveal fundamental properties of cognitive/affective architectures in health and illness (Barron, Garvert, & Behrens, Reference Barron, Garvert and Behrens2016). In the context of BPD, stimulus valence interferes with effortful task processing (Gvirts et al., Reference Gvirts, Braw, Harari, Lozin, Bloch, Fefer and Levkovitz2015; Soloff, Abraham, Ramaseshan, Burgess, & Diwadkar, Reference Soloff, Abraham, Ramaseshan, Burgess and Diwadkar2017b; Winter et al., Reference Winter, Krause-Utz, Lis, Chiu, Lanius, Schriner and Schmahl2015) but the modulatory effects of stimulus valence on priming may be a highly noteworthy area of inquiry.

Neuronal priming

The repeated presentation of a stimulus leads to changes in behavioral or neuronal responses, with each subsequent presentation increasing stimulus familiarity and processing efficiency. This process is generally referred to as priming (Fiebach, Gruber, & Supp, Reference Fiebach, Gruber and Supp2005). Priming is thought to result from a ‘sharpening’ effect on neurons (an increase in the efficiency of processing) that encode specific (and repeated) stimuli in neuronal networks (Martens & Gruber, Reference Martens and Gruber2012). This sharpening results in faster response times and generally, reductions in direct or surrogate (e.g. fMRI) measures of neuronal activity. Familiarity exerts similar effects because neuronal representations of familiar stimuli can be more efficiently processed during subsequent recall/recognition. In experimental settings, (repetition) priming typically leads to an attenuation of activation from the first presentation to the second presentation (Garcia-Marques, Prada, & Mackie, Reference Garcia-Marques, Prada and Mackie2016; Lebreton et al., Reference Lebreton, Villain, Chételat, Landeau, Seghier, Lazeyras and Ibanez2012).

Priming and familiarity are likely to be mediated by affect or affective salience. For instance, healthy subjects exhibit a strong preference for benevolent experiences or positive affect (Garcia-Marques et al., Reference Garcia-Marques, Prada and Mackie2016), resulting in positively valenced stimuli being tagged as more salient and therefore more familiar. Indeed, healthy subjects exhibit neuronal priming (attenuation of activation) in the presence of positive affect (Biss & Hasher, Reference Biss and Hasher2011), evidence in support of a relationship in the healthy brain between positive affect and familiarity. These effects relating to positive valence motivate the possibility of converse effects related to negative valence in the context of disorders of emotion dysregulation such as BPD. BPD patients (as with patients of other disorders of emotion dysregulation) are characterized by a ‘negative bias’, wherein negatively valenced stimuli are more salient (and presumably more ‘familiar’, see below) (Bortolla et al., Reference Bortolla, Galli, Ramella, Sirtori, Visintini and Maffei2020). This contraposition in the relevance of stimuli motivates the possibility that neuronal priming in BPD may be greater for negatively valenced stimuli (though this question has not been directly tested). In contrast to priming, repetition enhancement relates to an increase in neuronal responses to the subsequent presentation of stimuli. This phenomenon has been observed in the context of willful or spontaneous episodic memory retrieval, and in regions such as the parietal and posterior cingulate cortices (Weymar, Bradley, Sege, & Lang, Reference Weymar, Bradley, Sege and Lang2018). Outside of tasks with episodic memory demands, enhancement of neuronal responses is observed in the early stages of the repetition of unfamiliar stimuli (e.g. words) (Lebreton et al., Reference Lebreton, Villain, Chételat, Landeau, Seghier, Lazeyras and Ibanez2012; Martens & Gruber, Reference Martens and Gruber2012). This enhancement appears to be a pre-cursor in the process of familiarity, wherein an early sharpening of neuronal responses is needed before a subsequent level of familiarity (and presumably priming) is reached for priming to occur (Henson et al., Reference Henson, Goshen-Gottstein, Ganel, Otten, Quayle and Rugg2003; Subramaniam, Faust, Beeman, & Mashal, Reference Subramaniam, Faust, Beeman and Mashal2012). It is plausible that just as priming is an index of implicit or obligatory familiarity, enhancement reflects implicit (short-term) novelty.

Relevance for borderline personality disorder

BPD results from a combination of biological temperament, genetics, and psychosocial influences. Childhood trauma and sexual abuse are factors in its development suggesting that most patients are frequently exposed to negative life experiences (de Aquino Ferreira et al., Reference de Aquino Ferreira, Queiroz Pereira, Neri Benevides and Aguiar Melo2018). Indeed, as noted negative affect and traumatic life experiences are hypothesized to be more ‘familiar’ to BPD patients (Bertsch, Hillmann, & Herpertz, Reference Bertsch, Hillmann and Herpertz2018), a familiarity that certainly impacts activation to negative stimuli in specific experimental contexts (de Aquino Ferreira et al., Reference de Aquino Ferreira, Queiroz Pereira, Neri Benevides and Aguiar Melo2018). Multiple studies confirm that negatively valenced stimuli are more salient in BPD, and exert a multitude of diverse effects in episodic memory, attention, and affective processing tasks (Silbersweig et al., Reference Silbersweig, Clarkin, Goldstein, Kernberg, Tuescher, Levy and Stern2007; Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, Abraham, Burgess, Ramaseshan, Chowdury and Diwadkar2017a, Reference Soloff, Abraham, Ramaseshan, Burgess and Diwadkar2017b).

Selected studies in BPD patients have used fMRI to quantify activation changes associated with the repeated presentation of negatively valenced stimuli. These studies have relied on explicit emotional subjectivity in an effort to understand the relationship between behavioral ‘habituation’, that is attenuation of the behavioral response to repeated stimuli, and changes in fMRI activation (Denny et al., Reference Denny, Fan, Fels, Galitzer, Schiller and Koenigsberg2018; Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Denny, Fan, Liu, Guerreri, Mayson and Siever2014). Traditional studies of repetition priming rarely enforce the explicit evaluation of the attribute of interest. Rather, they rely on the implicit effects of such attributes under repetition (Poldrack & Gabrieli, Reference Poldrack and Gabrieli2001). Accordingly, the current investigation focused on assessing the implicit effects of un-evaluated/un-rated stimulus valence on priming effects assessed in the context of a traditional episodic memory task. Following the presentation of scenes during Encoding epochs, participants judged whether subsequently presented scenes in Recognition epochs were shown in the preceding Encoding epoch (i.e. ‘old’ or ‘new’). No other explicit evaluations were demanded from participants. By focusing on inter-group differences in priming as they related to the normed valence of the stimuli, it was possible for us to evaluate the modulatory effects of positive or negative valence on ‘neuronal’ priming in BPD. If priming in this context is driven by the ‘familiarity’ associated with positively valenced stimuli (in healthy controls) but with negatively valenced stimuli (in BPD), we would predict greater priming for positive valence in controls, but greater priming for negative valence in BPD. Thus, the nature of the task permits the assessment of the obligatory effects of valence on the priming of brain activation profiles in BPD.

Methods

Inclusion criteria

This study's procedures were approved by both Wayne State University and the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Boards. Sixty-five subjects' data were utilized from an ongoing longitudinal study of BPD subjects recruited through psychiatric outpatient clinics and advertisements in the surrounding community (Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan, and Diwadkar, Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015). The study was comprised of 25 HC and 40 BPD subjects between the ages of 18 and 44. The HC group was made up of 23 females and two males, and the BPD group was made up of 34 females and six males (see online Supplementary Table S1). These numbers are nearly representative of the morbidity of BPD, as women comprise approximately 75% of all clinical BPD patients (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). All subjects gave written informed consent. BPD subjects were required to meet the criteria for a probable or definite lifetime diagnosis of BPD from the International Personality Disorders Examination (IPDE) (Loranger, Reference Loranger, Sartorius, Andreoli, Berger, Buchheim, Channabasavanna and Coid1994). In addition, they were required to have a definite current diagnosis of BPD from the Diagnostic Interview for Borderline Patients-Revised (DIB-R), using a 2-year timeframe (Zanarini, Gunderson, Frankenburg, & Chauncey, Reference Zanarini, Gunderson, Frankenburg and Chauncey1989). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) was used to determine the co-morbidity of Axis I disorders (First, Reference First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams and Benjamin1997). Healthy controls were free of current or lifetime Axis I or Axis II diagnoses, and were therefore free of psychoactive medications, although BPD subjects could remain on any current psychoactive medications. All subjects were required to test negative to abusive drugs through urine toxicology, and to pregnancy tests if applicable (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015).

Exclusion criteria

Exclusion criteria included the following: (1) a lifetime (past or current) Axis I diagnosis of schizophrenia, delusional (paranoid) disorder, schizoaffective disorder, any bipolar disorder (I, II, mixed, manic, or depressed), or psychotic depression; (2) a current DSM-IV diagnosis of substance dependence or any current drug- or alcohol-related CNS deficits (a DSM-IV diagnosis of substance abuse was permitted so long as the subject had been abstinent for one week, showed no signs of withdrawal, and had a clean urine toxicology drug screen (MedTox) at the time of the scan); (3) clinical evidence of CNS pathology of any etiology, including acquired or developmental deficits or seizure disorder; (4) physical disorders or treatments with the known psychiatric consequence (e.g. hypothyroidism, steroid medications); (5) borderline mental retardation (IQ < 70 on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale); (6) standard exclusion criteria for MRI scans include the following: ferromagnetic implants such as cardiac pacers, cochlear implants, aneurysm clips, history of metal in eyes or other ferromagnetic body artifacts; inability to fit in the scanner due to obesity, claustrophobia or inability to tolerate brief confinement in the scanner; inability to co-operate with instructions (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015).

Imaging specifications

All anatomical images were acquired on a 3.0 T Siemens Trio system in the axial plane parallel to the AC-PC line using a 3D MPRAGE sequence (TE/TI/TR = 3.29 m/900 m/2200 m, flip angle = 9, isotropic 1 mm3 voxel, 192 axial slices, matrix size = 256 × 192). The fMRI data for this experiment were acquired in the axial plane using gradient-echo EPI (TR = 2000 m, TE = 30 m, flip angle = 70 deg, 30 slices, slice thickness = 3.1 mm, 3 mm × 3 mm in-plane, matrix size = 64 × 64) (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015).

fMRI paradigm procedure

An episodic memory task with a typical experimental design was used to assess fMRI activation profiles associated with the ENCoding and RECognition of episodic memories for International Affective Pictures System (IAPS) pictures (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, Reference Lang, Bradley and Cuthbert2008; Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015). A total of 81 pictures were used in the entire experiment, 27 each were classified as negative, neutral, or positive valence. The images drawn for the polar valences (negative and positive) that were the focus of this inquiry differed in their normative ratings of valence (1.95 v. 7.37) but were chosen to be equivalent in terms of their rated arousal (6.87 v. 6.13). During the paradigm, 54 pictures were presented once during each of nine encoding epochs. Twenty-seven were reused in the subsequent recognition epochs as targets, and an additional 27 pictures were used as foils. During each encoding epoch, six pictures (two from each of Negative and Positive valence, and two Neutral images used as fillers) were presented for 4 s with a 500–1500 ms randomly jittered inter-stimulus interval, for a total of 27 s per epoch. A brief retention interval (8 s) preceded the recognition epoch, in which three of the pictures were targets (one of each valence class) and three were foils (one of each valence class). Participants were asked to indicate if the pictures were presented in the encoding epoch immediately preceding it (‘old’ for targets, ‘new’ for foils). Nine pairs of encoding/recognition epochs were employed. For each valence class, a total of nine pictures were presented during both encoding and recognition (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015). An event-related design was deployed to increase the flexibility of measurements by controlling for changes in baseline activation and to account for any confounding effects attributed to stimulus expectation or shifts in attention (Barron et al., Reference Barron, Garvert and Behrens2016).

Image and fMRI data analyses

All fMRI data were pre-processed using typical methods with Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8). These methods included realignment to remove head movement artifact, normalization of the images into a spatially standard and ideal model, and smoothing to suppress noise or residual effects from differences in function. Low-frequency fluctuations were removed using a high-pass filter (1/256).

Responses to individual pictures were modeled using event-related models, with time and dispersion derivatives added to the model. Each participant contributed nine pairs of pictures to each valence (Negative, Positive). At the first level, contrast images (Encoding > Recognition) were used to quantify intra-subject effects of stimulus repetition for each valence class (Negative, Positive). Each participant thus contributed two contrast images each of which represented priming effects for Negative stimuli or priming effects for Positive stimuli. These images were carried forward to second-level random-effects analyses where Group (HC v. BPD) was modeled as an independent factor, Valence (Negative or Positive) was modeled as a non-independent factor, and participant age was modeled as a covariate. These analyses allowed for the age-controlled assessment of within-subject differences in activation changes (e.g. BPD−ve[ENC>REC]), and the subsequent assessment of between-group differences (BPD−ve[ENC>REC] ≠ HC−ve[ENC>REC]; BPD+ve[ENC>REC] ≠ HC+ve[ENC>REC]) in the effects of valence on priming. Significant clusters were identified using 104 Monte Carlo probability simulations of the data (p < 0.05 cluster level) within each region of interest of an identified peak (Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft, & Burdette, Reference Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft and Burdette2003) to compute the probability of a random field of noise (after spatial correlations of voxels based on image smoothness are accounted for). These simulations generate a minimum cluster size threshold for significance within a region of interest after thresholding noise to a certain level (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Ravishankar, Pivetta, Chowdury, Falco, Damoiseaux and Diwadkar2018; Muzik, Baajour, Bressler, & Diwadkar, Reference Muzik, Baajour, Bressler and Diwadkar2020); therefore, the analyses used both height thresholding (p < 0.05) and a minimum cluster size thresholding in arriving at a statistically robust threshold for identifying inter-group differences. The statistical approach is based on an underlying and tenable assumption that activation occurs over contiguous voxels whereas noise does not aggregate in clusters (Ward, Reference Ward2000).

Results

Subject characteristics

The sample included 25 healthy control and 40 BPD participants (see online Supplementary Table S1 for demographic information).

Episodic memory task fMRI results

We organize the presentation of results as follows: (1) We first present results of between-group analyses associated with negative valence (BPD−ve[ENC>REC] ≠ HC−ve[ENC>REC]) (Fig. 1), followed by (2) similar analyses for positive valence (BPD+ve[ENC>REC] ≠ HC+ve[ENC>REC]) (Fig. 2). (3) These analyses were extended (Fig. 3) to explore potential sources of the effects under (1) and (2). (4) Finally, for selected cluster peaks (Fig. 4), a graphical representation of the directionality of the effect provides a depiction of complementary patterns of priming or enhancement for each group (BPD, HC) under each valence (Negative, Positive).

Fig. 1. Negative valence and priming. The paradigm and conditions are schematically depicted (upper left). Inter-group differences in priming for negatively valenced pictures are overlaid on bilateral cortical surfaces, which are then unfolded onto a flat map for comprehensive rendition. As shown in the legend (bottom), warm colors reflect greater priming in BPD (BPD−ve[ENC>REC] > HC−ve[ENC>REC]), while cool colors reflect the converse (HC−ve[ENC>REC] > BPD−ve[ENC>REC]). To emphasize, warm colors depict greater priming for negatively valenced pictures in BPD participants. The significance peaks are denoted in (parenthesis). The numbers in the parenthesis are indexed in Table 1.

Fig. 2. Positive valence and priming. The paradigm and conditions are schematically depicted (upper left). Inter-group differences in priming for positively valenced pictures are overlaid on bilateral cortical surfaces, which are then unfolded onto a flat map for comprehensive rendition. As shown in the legend (bottom), warm colors reflect greater priming in BPD (BPD+ve[ENC>REC] > HC+ve[ENC>REC]), while cool colors reflect the converse (HC+ve[ENC>REC] > BPD+ve[ENC>REC]). To emphasize, cool colors depict greater priming for positively valenced pictures in HC participants. The significance peaks are denoted in (parenthesis). The numbers in the parenthesis are indexed in Table 1.

Fig. 3. Inter-group differences are depicted in an overall condition (Encoding v. Recognition) × Valence (Negative v. Positive) framework, to provide a comprehensive overview of the observed effects. As with the previous convention, warm colors depict increases in BPD, while cool colors depict increases in HC. The upper left quadrant shows significantly greater activation among BPD subjects for negatively valenced stimuli compared to HC. The lower left quadrant shows significantly greater activation among HC subjects for positively valenced stimuli compared to BPD. The upper right quadrant shows significantly greater activation among HC subjects for negatively valenced stimuli compared to BPD. The lower right quadrant shows significantly greater activation among BPD subjects for positively valenced stimuli compared to HC.

Fig. 4. (a) Effects related to priming (BPD) but enhancement (HC) for negatively valenced stimuli are depicted under the peak in the inferior parietal cortex (see Table 1). The bar graph reflects the mean signal change from Encoding to Recognition for each group under the peak (extracted from sphere, 4 mm radius, error bars are ±s.e.m.). As denoted, leftward bars denote the effects of repetition priming under the peak, whereas rightward bars denote the effects of repetition enhancement. (b) The converse effects are depicted for positively valenced stimuli, with the graph elucidating changes under the peak in the thalamus. Figure 4 supplements Fig. 3, elucidating the nature of our observed effects as a function of valence.

In assessing inter-group differences for negative valence (BPD−ve[ENC>REC] ≠ HC−ve[ENC>REC]), multiple significant clusters (see Methods) were observed under the BPD−ve[ENC>REC] > HC−ve[ENC>REC] contrast. These were observed in frontal, cingulate, and motor regions including the pars triangularis, cingulum, pre-frontal cortex, and supplemental motor area. This result indicates that in BPD, the decrease in fMRI responses from the first to the second presentation of the negative stimuli was greater than that observed in HC, suggesting a sharper attenuation of the response between successive presentations of negative stimuli in BPD participants. The effects are depicted in Fig. 1 with statistical reporting of the peaks and cluster extents in Table 1. Peak localization in Table 1 was based on deterministic masks in stereotactic space (Maldjian et al., Reference Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft and Burdette2003), and the cluster extent reflects the size of the cluster around each observed peak.

Table 1. Information regarding the activation levels for each region of interest (ROI)

N/A, not applicable.

The number of the anatomical ROIs corresponds to the number used to label the previous Figs 1 and 2 for negatively and positively valenced stimuli. The cluster extent indicates the voxel size of the activation region. The voxel peak represents the coordinates of the denoted peak.

Online Supplementary Table S1. Demographic information of subjects. Because groups differed in age (t 59 = 4.15, p < 0.01), age was added as a covariate in all analyses of fMRI data.

In assessing inter-group differences for positive valence (BPD+ve[ENC>REC] ≠ HC+ve[ENC>REC]), we observed multiple clusters under the BPD+ve[ENC>REC] < HC+ve[ENC>REC] contrast. These were observed in multiple frontal and parietal regions, as well as the thalamus. This result implies that in BPD, the decrease in fMRI responses from the first to the second presentation of the positive stimuli was less than that observed in HC, suggesting a shallower attenuation of the response between successive presentations of positive stimuli in BPD participants. The effects are depicted in Fig. 2 with statistical reporting of the peaks and cluster extents in Table 1.

Further elaboration of effects

Further analyses were designed to explore the potential source(s) of effects for both positive and negative valence. In these secondary analyses, we investigated if the effects under the clusters shown in Figs 1 and 2 were driven by differences in activation profiles during the initial presentation of stimuli (Encoding) (BPD[ENC] ≠ HC[ENC]) and/or the subsequent presentation of stimuli (Recognition) (BPD[REC] ≠ HC[REC]). To conduct these secondary analyses, we first constructed functional regions of interest (from Figs 1 and 2), used as spatial masks under which we assessed inter-group effects during Encoding and during Recognition.

Figure 3 (top row, left) shows that during the initial presentation (Encoding) of negatively valenced stimuli, BPD showed notably greater activation (BPD−ve[ENC] > HC−ve[ENC]), under the clusters in Fig. 1 (BPD−ve[ENC>REC] > HC−ve[ENC>REC]). This difference is less marked during the subsequent presentation (Recognition) though there is some evidence of reduced activation in key clusters. The elaboration of this effect suggests that the greater priming for negative valence in BPD was in part driven by greater activation during the initial presentation that may be related to the increased salience of negative valence in this group (see Introduction).

By comparison, during the initial presentation of positively valenced stimuli (bottom row, left), BPD showed notably lower activation (BPD+ve[ENC] < HC+ve[ENC]), under the clusters in Fig. 2 (BPD+ve[ENC>REC] < HC+ve[ENC>REC]). This effect is the converse of that observed under negative valence, and is consistent with the notion that positively valenced stimuli have increased salience in the healthy brain. The effects during the subsequent presentation (bottom row, right) suggest a notable increase in BPD participants, consistent with a notion of enhancement under repetition.

To better articulate these dissociated effects relating to valence, Fig. 4 (a subset of Figs 1 and 2) depicts the dissociated effects of priming and enhancement under the same cluster for negative valence (a) and positive valence (b), but with adjoining graphs that elucidate the nature of the effect for each. The crosshairs across each orthoview are centered on the significance peak for the inferior parietal cortex (a) and the thalamus (b) (see Table 1). The bar graphs reflect the mean signal change from Encoding to Recognition for each group under the peak (extracted from sphere, 4 mm radius). As denoted, leftward bars denote the effects of repetition priming under the peak, whereas rightward bars denote the effects of repetition enhancement.

Discussion

In the context of an episodic memory task with stimuli of varying valence, we investigated the effects of valence on neuronal priming in BPD patients compared to healthy controls. Our salient results were: (1) BPD subjects displayed neuronal priming for negatively valenced stimuli (Fig. 1). This effect was driven by greater activation to negatively valenced stimuli during encoding (the initial presentation) compared to recognition (the second presentation) (Fig. 3, top row); (2) conversely, BPD patients evinced repetition enhancement for positively valenced stimuli (Fig. 2). This effect was driven by greater activation to positively valenced stimuli during recognition rather than encoding (Fig. 3, bottom row).

We forward a plausible explanation of our results: (a) the heightened salience of negatively valenced stimuli (which leads to greater activation during Encoding) evokes a state of affective familiarity in BPD. Evidence for affective familiarity has been documented in the literature in studies with healthy controls (Garcia-Marques et al., Reference Garcia-Marques, Prada and Mackie2016); (b) this familiarity in turn drives the effect of neuronal priming for negatively valenced stimuli in this clinical group (Fiebach et al., Reference Fiebach, Gruber and Supp2005); (c) by comparison, reduced salience for positively valenced stimuli (which leads to greater activation in HC during Encoding) evokes a state of affective novelty in BPD, which in turn drives the effect of repetition enhancement in this clinical group (Subramaniam et al., Reference Subramaniam, Faust, Beeman and Mashal2012). In the remainder of the Discussion, we motivate the basis for these interpretations (based on extant studies) and highlight the clinical significance of our findings. We first place our results in the context of previous imaging studies in repetition priming/enhancement, and relate the specific brain regions that the effects were observed in, with some of the extant literature. Next, we sample evidence from (both functional and structural) imaging studies in BPD to provide a clinical framework for our effects. We conclude with a section on the clinical relevance of our results, and some limitations that constrain the interpretation and generalizability of the findings.

Comparison to previous imaging studies

Normative studies of brain function

In experimental contexts, both positive and negative valence hold substantial salience in the typical human brain and underpin brain networks that drive social interactions (Nummenmaa et al., Reference Nummenmaa, Glerean, Viinikainen, Jaaskelainen, Hari and Sams2012). However, whereas positively valenced stimuli are ‘preferred’ in healthy controls (Nielen et al., Reference Nielen, Heslenfeld, Heinen, Van Strien, Witter, Jonker and Veltman2009; Winecoff et al., Reference Winecoff, Clithero, Carter, Bergman, Wang and Huettel2013), negatively valenced stimuli are more salient to patients with BPD (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015; Soloff, Chowdury, & Diwadkar, Reference Soloff, Chowdury and Diwadkar2019). Positive valence carries higher emotional salience presumably because the presentation of stimuli with positive valence activates latent representations of existing connections associated with positive life experiences (Lempert, Speer, Delgado, & Phelps, Reference Lempert, Speer, Delgado and Phelps2017; Pool, Brosch, Delplanque, & Sander, Reference Pool, Brosch, Delplanque and Sander2016; Reggev, Bein, & Maril, Reference Reggev, Bein and Maril2016). Indeed, meta-analyses of over 200 unique studies, which have employed a combination of valences with attention tasks, have suggested that this salience reflects an attentional bias in favor of positive over negative or neutral stimuli (Pool et al., Reference Pool, Brosch, Delplanque and Sander2016). By inference, such a bias reflects and/or results in a pattern of affective familiarity with this stimulus class (Garcia-Marques et al., Reference Garcia-Marques, Prada and Mackie2016). Indeed, the directionality of our effects [Priming(HC, +ve valence) > Priming(BPD, +ve valence); Priming(BPD, −ve valence) > Priming(HC, −ve valence)] motivates similar explanations, but only distinguished by the role of stimulus valence for each group.

Familiarity (and novelty) shows demonstrable representations in fMRI-measured brain activity. For example, familiar stimuli are more efficiently recalled, suggested by the fact that during their retrieval/recognition, evoked responses are reduced in multiple regions including frontal, parietal, and temporal cortices (Gilmore, Kalinowski, Milleville, Gotts, & Martin, Reference Gilmore, Kalinowski, Milleville, Gotts and Martin2019; Reggev et al., Reference Reggev, Bein and Maril2016). This change in responses implies not only memory, but also shifts in attention away from familiar stimuli may be at play (Thakral, Jacobs, & Slotnick, Reference Thakral, Jacobs and Slotnick2017). Recent work on attention shifts has shown that the BOLD response in areas like the parietal cortex changes when target features in the stimulus (e.g. stimulus color or stimulus location) are repeated (Brinkhuis, Kristjansson, Harvey, & Brascamp, Reference Brinkhuis, Kristjansson, Harvey and Brascamp2020), suggesting that the neuronal signatures of priming are related to attention shifts away from repeated stimuli. In our study, the ‘feature’ that repeated was the stimulus identity itself, but the interactive role of attention with memory mechanisms is a known property of memorial processing. During memory processing, attention appears to exert substantial network effects (Ray et al., Reference Ray, Ragland, MacDonald, Gold, Silverstein, Barch and Carter2020), and appears to amplify priming effects in the parietal cortex. Effects related to attentional shifts may also explain the observed peaks in the thalamus, the principal sensorial gateway to the cortex (Barron, Eickhoff, Clos, & Fox, Reference Barron, Eickhoff, Clos and Fox2015; Saalmann & Kastner, Reference Saalmann and Kastner2011). Indeed, response properties of the thalamus are susceptible to the effects of attentional control (Jagtap & Diwadkar, Reference Jagtap and Diwadkar2016), and the evidence of priming in this structure suggests links with attention or pre-attention. Thus, the effects observed in healthy controls are consistent with extant knowledge on the representation of positive valence in the healthy brain. The converse of these effects was observed for negative valence in BPD patients (Fig. 1), largely in the same regions with additional differences observed in the mid cingulate cortex and the supplementary motor area. There is precedence for these effects in prior work in which priming and familiarity are associated with common activity in both the superior parietal and motor cortices (Thakral, Kensinger, & Slotnick, Reference Thakral, Kensinger and Slotnick2016), where the latter has been associated with facilitation in motor processing that may lead to reduced activity during familiarity and priming (Soldan, Zarahn, Hilton, & Stern, Reference Soldan, Zarahn, Hilton and Stern2008). This rationale is likely to apply to the mid cingulate effects as well, given that this region occupies cortical space adjacent to the supplementary motor area, and is associated with motor control (Hoffstaedter et al., Reference Hoffstaedter, Grefkes, Caspers, Roski, Palomero-Gallagher, Laird and Eickhoff2014). Regardless of the specific mechanisms, all the observed inter-group effects related to priming (Figs 1, 2, and 4 and Table 1) were observed in regions previously associated with priming, attention shifts, and memory (regardless of stimulus valence) (Dahlgren, Ferris, & Hamann, Reference Dahlgren, Ferris and Hamann2020; Simons & Spiers, Reference Simons and Spiers2003). Thus, the plausible familiarity/salience of positively valenced stimuli induces efficient recall of the memory trace in healthy controls. In effect, this places our interpretation of our results firmly in the domain of extant research that employs priming to identify the forward effects of memorial representations either using behavior or neuroimaging (Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Goodman, Burock, Rotte, Koutstaal, Schacter and Dale1998; McNamara & Diwadkar, Reference McNamara and Diwadkar1996).

Borderline personality disorder studies

BPD has diverse origins but the resultant long-term effect of the diagnosis is the disruption of mechanisms of emotion regulation particularly in the context of negatively valenced stimuli (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, White, Omari, Ramaseshan and Diwadkar2015). In terms of a mechanism, dysregulation may be coupled with/result from a heightened salience for negative stimuli, expressed through excessive rumination of, and disproportionate (and presumably more efficient) access to, negative memories (Baer, Peters, Eisenlohr-Moul, Geiger, & Sauer, Reference Baer, Peters, Eisenlohr-Moul, Geiger and Sauer2012). Indeed, BPD subjects have an attentional bias toward negative stimuli, specifically faces expressing fear, and show a faster response time to congruent fearful face pairs and a slower response time to incongruent pairs or other emotions (Jovev et al., Reference Jovev, Green, Chanen, Cotton, Coltheart and Jackson2012). They also experience a stronger cognitive appraisal of emotion when presented with negative affective stimuli (Levine, Marziali, & Hood, Reference Levine, Marziali and Hood1997).

Behavioral habituation is a well-established phenomenon in healthy controls, with several studies examining habituation-related impairments in BPD patients (Hazlett et al., Reference Hazlett, Zhang, New, Zelmanova, Goldstein, Haznedar and Chu2012). The notion of habituation is strongly related to emotional/behavioral processes, but is underpinned in variable ways by neuronal priming. Thus, Koenigsberg et al. (Reference Koenigsberg, Denny, Fan, Liu, Guerreri, Mayson and Siever2014) observed a lack of habituation to the repeated presentation of negatively valenced pictures in BPD patients. Whereas controls showed a significant increase in activation in areas such as the mid cingulate, BPD patients showed a non-significant decrease in the reduction of the fMRI response in the same region (Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Denny, Fan, Liu, Guerreri, Mayson and Siever2014, Fig. 1). From our perspective, their results showed greater ‘neuronal priming’ in BPD (though their effects did not reach significance, whereas our effects did). Notably, these (and other) studies have addressed subjective distress in association with the neuronal responses to negative stimuli, though as noted earlier, we relied on implicit stimulus-driven changes in neuronal activation to the presentation of repeated stimuli in the context of episodic memory.

An increase in the cognitive appraisal of negative stimuli in addition to heightened salience could contribute to the dysregulation in the effective processing of negatively valenced stimuli that BPD subjects experience. Studies have found that BPD subjects exhibit greater affective instability over healthy controls, marked by the lack of an increase in activation upon repeated presentation of negative stimuli (Denny et al., Reference Denny, Fan, Fels, Galitzer, Schiller and Koenigsberg2018; Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Denny, Fan, Liu, Guerreri, Mayson and Siever2014). Thus, negatively valenced stimuli appear more likely to evoke negative representations that are inherently more familiar to patients, and may impede emotional regulation, an interpretation consistent with the studies of Koenigsberg et al. (Reference Koenigsberg, Denny, Fan, Liu, Guerreri, Mayson and Siever2014). Consistent with this speculation is evidence that BPD participants show more neuronal priming for negatively valenced stimuli (though less behavioral habituation). Thus, our results are broadly consistent with the evidence of the heightened salience of negative stimuli in BPD subjects (Baer et al., Reference Baer, Peters, Eisenlohr-Moul, Geiger and Sauer2012).

A converse of the ‘negative bias’ is an anti-bias to positively valenced stimuli, because the same life experiences that heighten the salience for negative stimuli also diminish the salience of positive stimuli. Studies have shown that BPD subjects evaluate positive experiences and situations as being less positive (Reichenberger et al., Reference Reichenberger, Eibl, Pfaltz, Wilhelm, Voderholzer, Hillert and Blechert2017). For instance, when watching a video with an actor saying a positive statement, BPD subjects describe their emotions to be higher on scales of anxiety, guilt, or embarrassment and lower on scales of happiness or pride compared to healthy controls (Reichenberger et al., Reference Reichenberger, Eibl, Pfaltz, Wilhelm, Voderholzer, Hillert and Blechert2017). This has been attributed to an inability to recognize those emotions in situations that are not as frequently experienced by patients. BPD subjects would therefore be expected to display an increase in activation following the repeated presentation of (relatively unfamiliar) positive stimuli. Indeed, this repetition enhancement was noted in BPD subjects for positively valenced stimuli. Although there is no study directly investigating the effects of repetition enhancement in BPD subjects, the evidence to support the basis for stimulus novelty as a mechanism for repetition enhancement in healthy control subjects is abundant, as is the evidence that BPD subjects are more familiar with/have a bias toward negative stimuli and are therefore less familiar with positive ones.

Clinical relevance

Our results are clinically relevant to the study of BPD subjects, not just for memory processing and recall, but also with respect to differences in processing and recall associated with valence. These results provide us with some insight into the inner functioning of the BPD brain and potentially how regional and network differences between BPD and healthy brain function may stem from environmental (life experiences) as well as structural (neuronal processing) differences. Further investigation into the mechanisms underlying these results could provide a valuable understanding of BPD and its effects.

Limitations

Extraneous and confounding effects

As mentioned previously, BPD cannot be randomly assigned to subjects, increasing the effects of extraneous variables on the results. BPD subjects may react or respond to the task differently than HC subjects due to the characteristics of the disorder itself. The use of clinical control groups could be added to future studies to help account for any effects contributing to BPD subjects' results. It would likely decrease the limitation, but not fully eliminate it. Additionally, BPD subjects were far more likely to be victims of abuse in comparison to the healthy control subjects (60.0% and 0.0%, respectively). This could lead to a confounding relationship between the effects seen for negatively valenced stimuli, as well as differences in BPD responses between those that experienced abuse and those that did not (de Aquino Ferreira et al., Reference de Aquino Ferreira, Queiroz Pereira, Neri Benevides and Aguiar Melo2018), though we point out that this abuse is itself a cardinal driver for the emergence of the disorder. Finally, our study was not designed to distinguish the explicit effects of the task (i.e. the task itself was an explicit episodic memory task) from the inferred implicit effects of valence (i.e. what implicit effect the stimulus valence exerted on emotional responses, and therefore priming). Disambiguating these issues would demand extensive psychophysical and behavioral studies.

Psychoactive medication

Forty-five percent of the BPD subjects were taking psychotropic medications at the time of the scan (n = 18). To investigate if medicated status exerted an influence on fMRI measures, two-sample t tests were performed to compare parameter estimates between BPD sub-groups (medicated v. non-medicated) at significant peaks (Table 1). No significant differences (p's = 0.31–0.43, Cohen's d ≤ 0.137 on all tests) were observed between BPD sub-groups. We submit that while participants were currently symptomatic, any differences between medication sub-types would be too small (and our current sample underpowered) to be identified in our analysis.

Clinical ‘specificity’

A significant percentage of our participants (>80%) with BPD also had a history of, or current major depressive and/or substance use disorder. Having a history of MDD is characteristic of participants with BPD, and past and recent magisterial surveys of the literature have directly addressed the conceptual bases of such co-morbidity (Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Anwunah, New, Mitropoulou, Schopick and Siever1999; Winter, Elzinga, & Schmahl, Reference Winter, Elzinga and Schmahl2014). Koenigsberg et al. have suggested that BPD and MDD co-occur because both phenotypes share biological features. Subsequent meta-analyses provide some support for the functional bases of these hypotheses, particularly during the processing of negative affective valence, where BPD and MDD patients show similarly altered activations in cingulate, frontal, and occipital cortices (Schulze, Schulze, Renneberg, Schmahl, & Niedtfeld, Reference Schulze, Schulze, Renneberg, Schmahl and Niedtfeld2019). These findings have strengthened the notion of BPD being a disorder of ‘emotion dysregulation’ (Schulze, Schmahl, & Niedtfeld, Reference Schulze, Schmahl and Niedtfeld2016), and this dysregulation often leads to substance use as a maladaptive coping strategy (Snow, Balling, & Zimmerman, Reference Snow, Balling and Zimmerman2020). The issue of co-morbidity in BPD is challenging to tease out precisely because the ‘psychosocial sequelae’ of MDD and BPD can contribute to the development of the other disorders and the ordering of diagnosis onset can be challenging to assess (Gunderson et al., Reference Gunderson, Morey, Stout, Skodol, Shea, McGlashan and Bender2004). In our case, patients were recruited from a longitudinal BPD study (see Methods) wherepatients with a primary diagnosis of BPD were well-characterized. While the rates of MDD in such patients decline over time, the prevalence remains high (Shah & Zanarini, Reference Shah and Zanarini2018), confirming notions of a possible inextricable relationship between the two phenotypes. It is impossible for us (given the characteristics of our sample's co-morbidity) to assess the marginal additions (or interactions) of MDD and SUD to the primary BPD diagnosis, and the impact on our results.

In this vein, the examination of other dimensional contributions including suicidality and childhood trauma (particularly sexual abuse) will be warranted in future investigations. We have shown that the lethality of suicidal attempts is related to fMRI responses to negative valenced stimuli used during sustained attention tasks (Soloff et al., Reference Soloff, Chowdury and Diwadkar2019), consistent with altered fMRI profiles during self-reflection on aversive memories (Silvers et al., Reference Silvers, Hubbard, Chaudhury, Biggs, Shu, Grunebaum and Stanley2016). Childhood trauma (and particularly sexual abuse) is a known risk factor for BPD, with impacts particularly on medial temporal lobe structures such as the amygdala and hippocampus (Soloff, Nutche, Goradia, & Diwadkar, Reference Soloff, Nutche, Goradia and Diwadkar2008), and also on brain network profiles during social cognition tasks (Duque-Alarcon, Alcala-Lozano, Gonzalez-Olvera, Garza-Villarreal, & Pellicer, Reference Duque-Alarcon, Alcala-Lozano, Gonzalez-Olvera, Garza-Villarreal and Pellicer2019). The impacts of these long-term risk factors on brain responses are a very understudied area of the field. Understanding their effects on reflexive measures likef priming may help further sharpen the clinical focus of this research.

Conclusion

Our results elucidate how ‘obligatory’ neuronal responses related to repetition priming in BPD are shaped by stimulus valence, and can motivate a more robust understanding of the impact of pathology on neuronal architectures. Our future studies are motivated by an interest in the discovery of the network bases of the observed effects, and may illuminate the bases of ‘dysconnection’ (a dominant theme in disorders such as schizophrenia) in BPD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001136

Acknowledgements

The present study was financially supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH048063, MH111177), the Mark Cohen Neuroscience Endowment, the Ethel and James Flinn Foundation, the DMC Foundation, and the Lycaki-Young Funds (State of Michigan). The content is the sole responsibility of the authors. The funding agencies had no role in the conduct of the research and therefore, the work does not necessarily represent their official views.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Borderline personality disorder, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5) (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Baer, R. A., Peters, J. R., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Geiger, P. J., & Sauer, S. E. (2012). Emotion-related cognitive processes in borderline personality disorder: A review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 359369. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.03.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barron, D. S., Eickhoff, S. B., Clos, M., & Fox, P. T. (2015). Human pulvinar functional organization and connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 24172431. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barron, H. C., Garvert, M. M., & Behrens, T. E. (2016). Repetition suppression: A means to index neural representations using BOLD? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Biological Sciences, 371. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertsch, K., Hillmann, K., & Herpertz, S. C. (2018). Behavioral and neurobiological correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 51, 7682. doi: 10.1159/000487363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biss, R. K., & Hasher, L. (2011). Delighted and distracted: Positive affect increases priming for irrelevant information. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 11, 14741478. doi: 10.1037/a0023855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bortolla, R., Galli, M., Ramella, P., Sirtori, F., Visintini, R., & Maffei, C. (2020). Negative bias and reduced visual information processing of socio-emotional context in borderline personality disorder: A support for the hypersensitivity hypothesis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 69. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2020.101589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brinkhuis, M. A. B., Kristjansson, A., Harvey, B. M., & Brascamp, J. W. (2020). Temporal characteristics of priming of attention shifts are mirrored by BOLD response patterns in the frontoparietal attention network. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 30, 22672280. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L., Goodman, J., Burock, M., Rotte, M., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D., … Dale, A. M. (1998). Functional-anatomic correlates of object priming in humans revealed by rapid presentation event-related fMRI. Neuron, 20, 285296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlgren, K., Ferris, C., & Hamann, S. (2020). Neural correlates of successful emotional episodic encoding and retrieval: An SDM meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 143, 107495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Aquino Ferreira, L. F., Queiroz Pereira, F. H., Neri Benevides, A. M. L., & Aguiar Melo, M. C. (2018). Borderline personality disorder and sexual abuse: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research, 262, 7077. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denny, B. T., Fan, J., Fels, S., Galitzer, H., Schiller, D., & Koenigsberg, H. W. (2018). Sensitization of the neural salience network to repeated emotional stimuli following initial habituation in patients with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 657664. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17030367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duque-Alarcon, X., Alcala-Lozano, R., Gonzalez-Olvera, J. J., Garza-Villarreal, E. A., & Pellicer, F. (2019). Effects of childhood maltreatment on social cognition and brain functional connectivity in borderline personality disorder patients. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiebach, C. J., Gruber, T., & Supp, G. G. (2005). Neuronal mechanisms of repetition priming in occipitotemporal cortex: Spatiotemporal evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 34143422. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4107-04.2005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Gibbon, M, Spitzer, R. L, Williams, J. B. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis II personality disorders. Biometrics Research Department, NYSPI: New York.Google Scholar
Garcia-Marques, T., Prada, M., & Mackie, D. M. (2016). Familiarity increases subjective positive affect even in non-affective and non-evaluative contexts. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 638645. doi: 10.1007/s11031-016-9555-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, A. W., Kalinowski, S. E., Milleville, S. C., Gotts, S. J., & Martin, A. (2019). Identifying task-general effects of stimulus familiarity in the parietal memory network. Neuropsychologia, 124, 3143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2001). fMR-adaptation: A tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychologica, 107, 293321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunderson, J. G., Morey, L. C., Stout, R. L., Skodol, A. E., Shea, M. T., McGlashan, T. H., … Bender, D. S. (2004). Major depressive disorder and borderline personality disorder revisited: Longitudinal interactions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65, 10491056. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v65n0804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gvirts, H. Z., Braw, Y., Harari, H., Lozin, M., Bloch, Y., Fefer, K., & Levkovitz, Y. (2015). Executive dysfunction in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. European Psychiatry, 30, 959964. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.12.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hazlett, E. A., Zhang, J., New, A. S., Zelmanova, Y., Goldstein, K. E., Haznedar, M. M., … Chu, K. W. (2012). Potentiated amygdala response to repeated emotional pictures in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 448456. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henson, R. N., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Ganel, T., Otten, L. J., Quayle, A., & Rugg, M. D. (2003). Electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of face perception, recognition and priming. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 13, 793805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffstaedter, F., Grefkes, C., Caspers, S., Roski, C., Palomero-Gallagher, N., Laird, A. R., … Eickhoff, S. B. (2014). The role of anterior midcingulate cortex in cognitive motor control: Evidence from functional connectivity analyses. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 27412753. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jagtap, P., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2016). Effective connectivity of ascending and descending frontalthalamic pathways during sustained attention: Complex brain network interactions in adolescence. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 25572570. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jovev, M., Green, M., Chanen, A., Cotton, S., Coltheart, M., & Jackson, H. (2012). Attentional processes and responding to affective faces in youth with borderline personality features. Psychiatry Research, 199, 4450. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigsberg, H. W., Anwunah, I., New, A. S., Mitropoulou, V., Schopick, F., & Siever, L. J. (1999). Relationship between depression and borderline personality disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 10, 158167.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigsberg, H. W., Denny, B. T., Fan, J., Liu, X., Guerreri, S., Mayson, S. J., … Siever, L. J. (2014). The neural correlates of anomalous habituation to negative emotional pictures in borderline and avoidant personality disorder patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 8290. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krekelberg, B., Boynton, G. M., & van Wezel, R. J. (2006). Adaptation: From single cells to BOLD signals. Trends in Neuroscience, 29, 250256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL.Google Scholar
Lebreton, K., Villain, N., Chételat, G., Landeau, B., Seghier, M. L., Lazeyras, F., … Ibanez, V. (2012). Repetition enhancement and perceptual processing of visual word form. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lempert, K. M., Speer, M. E., Delgado, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2017). Positive autobiographical memory retrieval reduces temporal discounting. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 15841593. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, D., Marziali, E., & Hood, J. (1997). Emotion processing in borderline personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 240246. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199704000-00004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieb, K., Zanarini, M. C., Schmahl, C., Linehan, M. M., & Bohus, M. (2004). Borderline personality disorder. Lancet (London, England), 364, 453461. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16770-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loranger, A. W., Sartorius, N, Andreoli, A, Berger, P, Buchheim, P, Channabasavanna, S. M., & Coid, B. (1994). The International Personality Disorder Examination. The World Health Organization/Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration international pilot study of personality disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 51, 215224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage, 19, 12331239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martens, U., & Gruber, T. (2012). Sharpening and formation: Two distinct neuronal mechanisms of repetition priming. European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 29892995. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08222.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNamara, T. P., & Diwadkar, V. A. (1996). The context of memory retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 877892. doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, A., Ravishankar, M., Pivetta, L., Chowdury, A., Falco, D., Damoiseaux, J. S., … Diwadkar, V. A. (2018). Response hand and motor set differentially modulate the connectivity of brain pathways during simple uni-manual motor behavior. Brain Topography, 31, 9851000. doi: 10.1007/s10548-018-0664-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muzik, O., Baajour, S., Bressler, S., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2020). Directional interactions between constituents of the human large-scale thermoregulatory network. Brain Topography, 33(4), 489503. doi: 10.1007/s10548-020-00778-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielen, M. M., Heslenfeld, D. J., Heinen, K., Van Strien, J. W., Witter, M. P., Jonker, C., & Veltman, D. J. (2009). Distinct brain systems underlie the processing of valence and arousal of affective pictures. Brain and Cognition, 71, 387396. doi: S0278-2626(09)00096-7 [pii]10.1016/j.bandc.2009.05.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Viinikainen, M., Jaaskelainen, I. P., Hari, R., & Sams, M. (2012). Emotions promote social interaction by synchronizing brain activity across individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 109, 95999604. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206095109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Characterizing the neural mechanisms of skill learning and repetition priming: Evidence from mirror reading. Brain, 124, 6782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 79106. doi: 10.1037/bul0000026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ray, K. L., Ragland, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Gold, J. M., Silverstein, S. M., Barch, D. M., & Carter, C. S. (2020). Dynamic reorganization of the frontal parietal network during cognitive control and episodic memory. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 20, 7690. doi: 10.3758/s13415-019-00753-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reggev, N., Bein, O., & Maril, A. (2016). Distinct neural suppression and encoding effects for conceptual novelty and familiarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 14551470. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichenberger, J., Eibl, J. J., Pfaltz, M., Wilhelm, F. H., Voderholzer, U., Hillert, A., & Blechert, J. (2017). Don't praise me, don't chase me: Emotional reactivity to positive and negative social-evaluative videos in patients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31, 7589. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2016_30_238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saalmann, Y. B., & Kastner, S. (2011). Cognitive and perceptual functions of the visual thalamus. Neuron, 71, 209223. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sala, M., Caverzasi, E., Marraffini, E., De Vidovich, G., Lazzaretti, M., d'Allio, G., … Brambilla, P. (2009). Cognitive memory control in borderline personality disorder patients. Psychological Medicine, 39, 845853. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, L., Schmahl, C., & Niedtfeld, I. (2016). Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A multimodal meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 97106. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, L., Schulze, A., Renneberg, B., Schmahl, C., & Niedtfeld, I. (2019). Neural correlates of affective disturbances: A comparative meta-analysis of negative affect processing in borderline personality disorder, major depressive disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 4, 220232. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, R., & Zanarini, M. C. (2018). Comorbidity of borderline personality disorder: Current status and future directions. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 41, 583593. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2018.07.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silbersweig, D., Clarkin, J. F., Goldstein, M., Kernberg, O. F., Tuescher, O., Levy, K. N., … Stern, E. (2007). Failure of frontolimbic inhibitory function in the context of negative emotion in borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 18321841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvers, J. A., Hubbard, A. D., Chaudhury, S., Biggs, E., Shu, J., Grunebaum, M. F., … Stanley, B. (2016). Suicide attempters with borderline personality disorder show differential orbitofrontal and parietal recruitment when reflecting on aversive memories. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 81, 7178. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.06.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, J. S., & Spiers, H. J. (2003). Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 637648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, M. B., Balling, C., & Zimmerman, M. (2020). Re-examining borderline personality disorder and substance use disorder: The role of emotion dysregulation. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 32, 170175.Google ScholarPubMed
Soldan, A., Zarahn, E., Hilton, H. J., & Stern, Y. (2008). Global familiarity of visual stimuli affects repetition-related neural plasticity but not repetition priming. Neuroimage, 39, 515526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soloff, P. H., Abraham, K., Burgess, A., Ramaseshan, K., Chowdury, A., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2017a). Impulsivity and aggression mediate regional brain responses in borderline personality disorder: An fMRI study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 260, 7685. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.12.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soloff, P. H., Abraham, K., Ramaseshan, K., Burgess, A., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2017b). Hyper-modulation of brain networks by the amygdala among women with borderline personality disorder: Network signatures of affective interference during cognitive processing. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 88, 5663. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.12.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soloff, P. H., Chowdury, A., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2019). Affective interference in borderline personality disorder: The lethality of suicidal behavior predicts functional brain profiles. Journal of Affective Disorders, 252, 253262. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.04.050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soloff, P., Nutche, J., Goradia, D., & Diwadkar, V. (2008). Structural brain abnormalities in borderline personality disorder: A voxel-based morphometry study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 164, 223236. doi: S0925-4927(08)00026-7 [pii]10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soloff, P. H., White, R., Omari, A., Ramaseshan, K., & Diwadkar, V. A. (2015). Affective context interferes with brain responses during cognitive processing in borderline personality disorder: fMRI evidence. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 233, 2335. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.04.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subramaniam, K., Faust, M., Beeman, M., & Mashal, N. (2012). The repetition paradigm: Enhancement of novel metaphors and suppression of conventional metaphors in the left inferior parietal lobe. Neuropsychologia, 50, 27052719. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thakral, P. P., Jacobs, C. M., & Slotnick, S. D. (2017). An attention account of neural priming. Memory (Hove, England), 25, 856864. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1233283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thakral, P. P., Kensinger, E. A., & Slotnick, S. D. (2016). Familiarity and priming are mediated by overlapping neural substrates. Brain Research, 1632, 107118. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.12.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, B. D. (2000). Simultaneous inference for fMRI data. Technical Report. Milwaukee, WI: Medical College of Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Weymar, M., Bradley, M. M., Sege, C. T., & Lang, P. J. (2018). Neural activation and memory for natural scenes: Explicit and spontaneous retrieval. Psychophysiology, 55, e13197. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winecoff, A., Clithero, J. A., Carter, R. M., Bergman, S. R., Wang, L., & Huettel, S. A. (2013). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex encodes emotional value. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1103211039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4317-12.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D., Elzinga, B., & Schmahl, C. (2014). Emotions and memory in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 47, 7185. doi: 10.1159/000356360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, D., Krause-Utz, A., Lis, S., Chiu, C. D., Lanius, R. A., Schriner, F., … Schmahl, C. (2015). Dissociation in borderline personality disorder: Disturbed cognitive and emotional inhibition and its neural correlates. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 233, 339351. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.05.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Y., Turk-Browne, N. B., & Chun, M. M. (2007). Dissociating task performance from fMRI repetition attenuation in ventral visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 59815985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5527-06.2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P., Ranganath, C., Ekstrom, A. D., & Wiltgen, B. J. (2019). A contextual binding theory of episodic memory: Systems consolidation reconsidered. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20, 364375. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0150-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanarini, M. C., Gunderson, J. G., Frankenburg, F. R., & Chauncey, D. L. (1989). The revised diagnostic interview for borderlines: Discriminating BPD from other axis II disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 3, 1018. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1989.3.1.10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Negative valence and priming. The paradigm and conditions are schematically depicted (upper left). Inter-group differences in priming for negatively valenced pictures are overlaid on bilateral cortical surfaces, which are then unfolded onto a flat map for comprehensive rendition. As shown in the legend (bottom), warm colors reflect greater priming in BPD (BPD−ve[ENC>REC] > HC−ve[ENC>REC]), while cool colors reflect the converse (HC−ve[ENC>REC] > BPD−ve[ENC>REC]). To emphasize, warm colors depict greater priming for negatively valenced pictures in BPD participants. The significance peaks are denoted in (parenthesis). The numbers in the parenthesis are indexed in Table 1.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Positive valence and priming. The paradigm and conditions are schematically depicted (upper left). Inter-group differences in priming for positively valenced pictures are overlaid on bilateral cortical surfaces, which are then unfolded onto a flat map for comprehensive rendition. As shown in the legend (bottom), warm colors reflect greater priming in BPD (BPD+ve[ENC>REC] > HC+ve[ENC>REC]), while cool colors reflect the converse (HC+ve[ENC>REC] > BPD+ve[ENC>REC]). To emphasize, cool colors depict greater priming for positively valenced pictures in HC participants. The significance peaks are denoted in (parenthesis). The numbers in the parenthesis are indexed in Table 1.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Inter-group differences are depicted in an overall condition (Encoding v. Recognition) × Valence (Negative v. Positive) framework, to provide a comprehensive overview of the observed effects. As with the previous convention, warm colors depict increases in BPD, while cool colors depict increases in HC. The upper left quadrant shows significantly greater activation among BPD subjects for negatively valenced stimuli compared to HC. The lower left quadrant shows significantly greater activation among HC subjects for positively valenced stimuli compared to BPD. The upper right quadrant shows significantly greater activation among HC subjects for negatively valenced stimuli compared to BPD. The lower right quadrant shows significantly greater activation among BPD subjects for positively valenced stimuli compared to HC.

Figure 3

Fig. 4. (a) Effects related to priming (BPD) but enhancement (HC) for negatively valenced stimuli are depicted under the peak in the inferior parietal cortex (see Table 1). The bar graph reflects the mean signal change from Encoding to Recognition for each group under the peak (extracted from sphere, 4 mm radius, error bars are ±s.e.m.). As denoted, leftward bars denote the effects of repetition priming under the peak, whereas rightward bars denote the effects of repetition enhancement. (b) The converse effects are depicted for positively valenced stimuli, with the graph elucidating changes under the peak in the thalamus. Figure 4 supplements Fig. 3, elucidating the nature of our observed effects as a function of valence.

Figure 4

Table 1. Information regarding the activation levels for each region of interest (ROI)

Supplementary material: File

Szczepaniak et al. supplementary material

Szczepaniak et al. supplementary material

Download Szczepaniak et al. supplementary material(File)
File 230.9 KB