Hostname: page-component-55f67697df-2mk96 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-05-08T21:32:00.023Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A single dose of lamotrigine induces a positive memory bias in healthy volunteers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2025

Tarek Zghoul
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Pilar Artiach Hortelano
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Alexander Kaltenboeck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK Clinical Division of Social Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
Lucy Wright
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Guy M. Goodwin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
Liliana P. Capitão*
Affiliation:
Psychological Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychology Research Centre (CIPsi), School of Psychology, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
Catherine J. Harmer*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
*
Corresponding authors: Liliana P. Capitão and Catherine J. Harmer; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Liliana P. Capitão and Catherine J. Harmer; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Lamotrigine has been shown to be effective in the long-term treatment and relapse prevention of depression in bipolar disorder. However, the neuropsychological mechanisms underlying these effects are unclear. We investigated the effects of lamotrigine on a battery of emotional processing tasks in healthy volunteers, previously shown to be sensitive to antidepressant drug action in similar experimental designs.

Methods

Healthy volunteers (n = 36) were randomized in a double-blind design to receive a single dose of placebo or 300 mg lamotrigine. Mood and subjective effects were monitored throughout the study period, and emotional processing was assessed using the Oxford Emotional Test Battery (ETB) 3 hours post-administration.

Results

Participants receiving lamotrigine showed increased accurate recall of positive versus negative self-descriptors, compared to those in the placebo group. There were no other significant effects on emotional processing in the ETB, and lamotrigine did not affect ratings of mood or subjective experience.

Conclusions

Lamotrigine did not induce widespread changes in emotional processing. However, there was increased positive bias in emotional memory, similar to the effects of antidepressants reported in previous studies. Further work is needed to assess whether similar effects are seen in the clinical treatment of patients with bipolar disorder and the extent to which this is associated with its clinical action in relapse prevention.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) is characterized by recurrent presentations of elevated mood episodes, in the form of either mania or hypomania and depressive episodes (APA, 2022). Affecting over 1% of the global population, this severe chronic affective illness is among the leading causes of worldwide disability (Global Burden of Disease Study, 2015; Judd et al., Reference Judd, Akiskal, Schettler, Endicott, Maser, Solomon and Keller2002). BD’s predominant mood state is bipolar depression (Malhi, Mitchell, & Salim, Reference Malhi, Mitchell and Salim2003). However, most available treatments for BD such as mood stabilizers (i.e., lithium), antiepileptic drugs (i.e., carbamazepine and valproate sodium), or atypical antiepileptic drugs (i.e., olanzapine) exert their effects primarily on mania/hypomania and, to a lesser extent, on depression (Goldsmith, Wagstaff, Ibbotson, & Perry, Reference Goldsmith, Wagstaff, Ibbotson and Perry2003).

Lamotrigine is a relatively new drug that is associated with prevention of depressive episodes within the clinical presentation of BD. First licensed as an add-on treatment for epilepsy, lamotrigine was quickly seen to have beneficial effects in BD, gaining approval in 2003 as a maintenance treatment for BD by the US FDA (Goldsmith et al., Reference Goldsmith, Wagstaff, Ibbotson and Perry2003; Vajda, Dodd, & Horgan, Reference Vajda, Dodd and Horgan2013). The mechanisms underlying the effects of lamotrigine in BD are not fully understood but have been suggested to involve inhibition of voltage-gated sodium channels and reductions in presynaptic glutamate release (Ketter, Manji, & Post, Reference Ketter, Manji and Post2003; Lee, Fu, Chen, Su, & Liou, Reference Lee, Fu, Chen, Su and Liou2008; Prica, Hascoet, & Bourin, Reference Prica, Hascoet and Bourin2008; Sills & Rogawski, Reference Sills and Rogawski2020). Such effects would be expected to counter enhanced excitatory neurotransmission in bipolar disorder (Sanacora, Zarate, Krystal, & Manji, Reference Sanacora, Zarate, Krystal and Manji2008).

BD is associated with impairments in cognition and emotional cognition (Bogie, Persaud, Smith, Kapczinski, & Frey, Reference Bogie, Persaud, Smith, Kapczinski and Frey2019; Fijtman et al., Reference Fijtman, Bücker, Strange, Martins, Passos, Hasse-Sousa and Kauer-Sant’Anna2020). Generally, patients with BD depression and unipolar depression show an impairment in recall and recognition processes, which seem to be related to poor encoding rather than rapid forgetting (Bearden et al., Reference Bearden, Glahn, Monkul, Barrett, Najt, Villarreal and Soares2006; Deckersbach et al., Reference Deckersbach, Savage, Reilly-Harrington, Clark, Sachs and Rauch2004). There is also evidence that emotional processing changes in BD may be mood-dependent. For example, Bilderbeck et al. (Reference Bilderbeck, Reed, McMahon, Atkinson, Price, Geddes and Harmer2016) found improved performance in categorizing positive personality adjectives as a function of increased symptoms of mania, while depression was associated with enhanced categorization of negative personality adjectives, in a sample of patients with BD. This is in line with the idea that manic symptomatology in BD is associated with a positive bias. whereas those with depressive symptomatology show a more negative bias in emotional processing (Paykel, Reference Paykel1987). Interestingly, patients with BD depression showed a similar negative bias to those with unipolar depression in the categorization of ambiguous facial expressions and self-referential emotional words (Bilderbeck et al., Reference Bilderbeck, Reed, McMahon, Atkinson, Price, Geddes and Harmer2016).

Despite this evidence, little research has investigated lamotrigine’s effect on emotional processing and memory biases. A study involving a pediatric BD sample found that lamotrigine improved working and verbal memory performance (Stuart, Butler, Munafò, Nutt, & Robinson, Reference Stuart, Butler, Munafò, Nutt and Robinson2015). Lamotrigine treatment also increased neural response associated with verbal working memory and facial expression processing in a sample of 12 adults with BD compared to pretreatment measures (Haldane et al., Reference Haldane, Jogia, Cobb, Kozuch, Kumari and Frangou2008). Interestingly, Bilderbeck et al. (Reference Bilderbeck, Reed, McMahon, Atkinson, Price, Geddes and Harmer2016), who also collected current medication use in their BDI and BDII sample found that the use of medication, including anticonvulsants, was associated with a greater positive bias compared to patients not taking medication. Although lamotrigine was not investigated specifically, it suggests that mood-stabilizing treatment in BD may be associated with the induction of a more positive emotional bias. However, our understanding of the direct effect of lamotrigine on cognitive processes, including emotional processes, is still incomplete. In addition, the lack of a placebo control in previous studies, together with the use of highly heterogeneous clinical populations, limits the interpretation of such results.

Experimental medicine may represent a useful approach in the investigation of lamotrigine’s mechanism of action early within treatment, before changes in symptomatology are typically seen. The use of this approach in healthy volunteers has proven useful in characterizing early effects of conventional antidepressants. Accordingly, an increase in the relative processing of positive rather than negative emotional information has been observed in healthy volunteers following single or repeated antidepressant administration from a variety of pharmacological classes (e.g., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors - SSRIs, atypical antidepressants) (see Harmer, Duman, & Cowen, Reference Harmer, Duman and Cowen2017 for a review). For example, SSRIs have been shown to increase the recognition of positive facial expressions in a recognition task and increase recall for positive over negative information in a free recall test. Interestingly, similar effects have also been found in depressed individuals after a single dose of reboxetine compared to placebo (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O’Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke and Cowen2009). Ultimately, these studies have highlighted that conventional antidepressants may act by reducing negative biases characteristic of depression (Godlewska & Harmer, Reference Godlewska and Harmer2021; Harmer, Reference Harmer2010; Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O’Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke and Cowen2009), with these effects, which are seen rapidly and in the absence of subjective mood changes, contributing to subsequent clinical changes in mood (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O’Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke and Cowen2009; Reference Harmer, Duman and Cowen2017).

The current study therefore aimed to use an experimental medicine approach to investigate the effects of a single dose of the novel drug lamotrigine in a healthy volunteer sample with the use of a well-validated task battery measuring emotional cognition. The use of a healthy volunteer sample can be advantageous as a translational step as it allows for the direct assessment of lamotrigine on neuropsychological mechanisms unconfounded by disorder-related factors. Additionally, the emotional test battery (ETB) has been extensively used in experimental medicine approaches to characterize different antidepressant drug classes both in healthy volunteers and clinical populations. Given that lamotrigine’s predominant effects are on BD depression and that negative biases in emotional cognition have been reported when depressive symptomatology is higher, this research can aid the understanding and identification of surrogate markers for improved targets of depression in BD. It was hypothesized that lamotrigine would induce a positive bias in emotional processing and cognition, with these effects being more pronounced on those tasks including a memory component given the effect of lamotrigine reported in previous studies.

Material and methods

Study sample

Thirty-six healthy volunteers (12 women and 24 men) aged 18–40 years were recruited via print and online announcements and enrolled in this study. All participants gave written informed consent prior to participation and were reimbursed for their time and any other expenses. Participants completed a screening visit to ascertain eligibility prior to proceeding to the experimental session. This included a psychiatric interview (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5; SCID-5) and a general medical screening, with females testing negative on a pregnancy test. Participants were then assessed for estimated intelligence using the National Adult Reading Test (Revised Version, NART-R) (Nelson & Willison, Reference Nelson and Willison1991), trait anxiety using the Spielberger’s State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983), depressive symptoms using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961), and personality traits using the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) (Eysenck & Eysenck, Reference Eysenck and Eysenck1975). Participants were excluded if they had (a) current or past history of psychiatric disorders (as assessed using the SCID-5), (b) insufficient English language skills, (c) current or past history of drug or alcohol dependency, (d) significant medical conditions, (e) previous participation in studies using the same emotional processing tasks, (f) contraindication to MRI, (g) history of recurrent allergies and rashes, (h) consumed any psychoactive medication or folic acid supplements, (I) smoked in excess of five cigarettes a day, (j) had a high intake of caffeinated drinks (>5 cups a day), (k) were pregnant or lactating women, or (l) were taking the contraceptive pill. Ethical approval was obtained from the Central University Research Ethics Committee (CUREC). The study was registered on ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT04396938).

Procedure

In this double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized, parallel-group study participants were assigned to receive either a single oral dose of lamotrigine (300 mg) or placebo. All tablets were encapsulated to provide a uniform appearance and ensure a double-blinded study. The randomization code was created by another member of the lab group not involved in the experiment. Participants were asked to avoid having any alcohol for at least 12 hours and food and caffeine for at least 3 hours prior to the experimental visit. As lamotrigine peak plasma concentrations (Tmax) occur between 1–3 hours following oral intake (Goldsmith et al., Reference Goldsmith, Wagstaff, Ibbotson and Perry2003), all volunteers were tested 3 hours after drug administration (T3). During the first 2 hours post-drug administration, participants stayed in a quiet clinic room, where they were free to relax or engage in any activity such as reading or using their personal electronic devices. During the next hour (T3), participants underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging scan (results reported elsewhere), and were tested with the Oxford ETB (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O’Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke and Cowen2009) (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Cowen, Harmer, Tzortzis, Errington and Burnet2015).

Measures

Demographic and questionnaire measures

Self-reported questionnaires were administered to assess subjective state and side effects at three time points: prior to active drug or placebo intake (Baseline or T1), after 2 hours (T2), and after 3 hours (T3). The following measures were used: the STAI-State (Spielberger et al., Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988), and visual analogue scales (VAS) of subjectively experienced emotions. Side effects/somatic symptoms were measured on 11 symptoms (agitation, dry mouth, nausea, aggression, tremor, headache, drowsiness, dizziness, back or joint pain, vision impairments, and rashes) with the following scale: 0 = absent, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, and 3 = severe.

Emotional test battery

The ETB is a validated tool of five broadly computerized behavioral tasks that allow for the assessment of emotional information processing in different cognitive domains (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O’Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke and Cowen2009), and proved to be sensitive to the early effects of antidepressant medication (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, O’Sullivan, Favaron, Massey-Chase, Ayres, Reinecke and Cowen2009; Warren, Cowen, & Harmer, Reference Warren, Cowen and Harmer2019). With a duration of 45 minutes, it was completed in the following order: facial expression recognition task (FERT), emotional categorization task (ECAT), attentional dot-probe task, emotional recall task (EREC), and emotional recognition memory task (EMEM).

In the FERT, participants were presented with pictures of facial expressions displaying one of the six basic emotions (i.e., anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and surprise) or neutral. Each emotion was displayed at 10 morphed intensity levels from “neutral” (that is, 0%) to “full intensity” (that is, 100%) resulting in a total of 250 pictures (Young et al., Reference Young, Rowland, Calder, Etcoff, Seth and Perrett1997). Facial expressions were presented randomly for 500 ms each, followed by a blank black screen. Participants were instructed to identify each emotion as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing one of the seven correspondingly labelled keys on the keyboard. Main outcomes were number of misclassifications, accuracy, and reaction time (RT).

In the ECAT, participants were presented with 60 words representing personality descriptors that were either agreeable (e.g. honest, thoughtful, etc; N = 30) or disagreeable (e.g. untidy, bossy, etc; N = 30). Each word was randomly presented in the center of a computer screen for 500 ms. Participants were asked to imagine overhearing someone describing them with each word displayed and to indicate as quickly and accurately as possible whether they would like or dislike to be described as such by pressing the correspondingly labeled key on the keyboard. The main outcome measures were RT and classifications.

In the attentional dot-probe task (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Cowen, Harmer, Tzortzis, Errington and Burnet2015), participants were presented with 60 positive and 60 negative words paired with neutral words (distinct from the words used in the earlier tasks). In each trial, a fixation cross was presented in the center of the screen for 500 ms, followed by two words displayed at the bottom and top of the screen. These word pairs were shown for either 500 ms in the unmasked condition or 17 ms under the masked condition and were followed by a mask for 483 ms. Masks were created from letters, non-letter symbols and digits and were matched in word length and position. A dot-probe (one or two stars) then replaced the masks or words in their location. Accordingly, the dot-probe could appear behind the emotional word (congruent trials) or the neutral word (incongruent trial). Participants were asked to identify the probe as accurately and quickly as possible by pressing the corresponding key on the keyboard. There was a total of 180 trials (30 negative-neutral, 30 positive-neutral, and 30 neutral-neutral pairs each for the unmasked and masked conditions) with emotional words displayed at equal frequency at the bottom and top of the screen. Accuracy and RT were noted. Attentional vigilance scores were then calculated by subtracting RT in congruent trials from RT in incongruent trials (Attentional vigilance = RT incongruent trials – RT congruent trials). This task was completed between the ECAT and the EREC.

The EREC took place approximately 15 minutes after the ECAT, from which participants were asked to write down as many words as they could recall from the categorization task in 4 minutes. The outcome measure was the number of correctly and incorrectly recalled words.

In the EMEM, positive and negative personality descriptors (in equal frequency) were presented in the center of the screen for 500 ms. In total, 120 words were randomly presented with 60 being familiar and having already been displayed during ECAT and 60 being novel or unfamiliar. Participants were asked to indicate as accurately and quickly as possible, whether the word displayed was familiar or novel by pressing the corresponding key on the keyboard. The main outcomes included RT, correct responses, and false positives.

Statistics

Analyses were performed using SPSS (version 25.0) with significant levels set at p < 0.05. One participant was excluded from the attentional dot-probe task analysis as there was a computer failure during this task. Changes in subjective ratings (STAI-State, PANAS, VAS) and side effect symptoms were analyzed using mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA), with timepoint (baseline or T1, T2, and T3) as within-subjects factor, and treatment group (lamotrigine or placebo) as between-subjects factor. ETB data were analyzed using mixed ANOVA with emotion/valence as within-subject factors and treatment group (lamotrigine or placebo) as between-subject factors. The attentional dot-probe task was analyzed using mixed ANOVA with valence and masking as within-subject factors and treatment group as between-subject factors. Significant interactions were further explored using independent sample t-tests and/or paired-sample t-tests. Whenever relevant, analyses were repeated with side effects as a covariate (averaged values for timepoint 3).

Results

Baseline measures

The baseline characteristics of our sample can be found in Table 1. Participants were well matched on age, gender distribution, proportion of university-educated participants, ethnicity, verbal IQ, the BDI, and the EPQ scales. However, the lamotrigine group displayed higher scores on the STAI-trait.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the sample

Note: Data given as mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise indicated. N, sample; NART, National Adult Reading Test; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; STAI, State–Trait Anxiety Inventory; EPQ, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire.

Changes in subjective ratings and side effects

Measures of subjective mood were administered to assess subjective state and somatic side effects following treatment administration. There were no significant effects of treatment on the PANAS, VAS, or the STAI-State (all p > 0.05). There was a significant main effect of the group on side effects with the lamotrigine group reporting more side effects across all time points, including more physical symptoms at baseline (F(1.34) = 6.967, p = 0.012). There was no significant interaction between group and time points (p = 1.19).

FERT

For the accuracy of recognizing facial expressions’ emotions and number of misclassifications, there were no significant main effects of treatment nor significant interactions between treatment and emotions (all Fs < 6, all p s > 0.12).

ECAT

For accuracy of the emotional recall and RTs, there were no significant main effects of treatment nor significant interactions between treatment and emotion (all Fs < 1, all p s > 0.31).

Attentional Dot-Probe task

For vigilance scores during the attentional dot-probe task, there was no significant main effect or interaction with treatment (p > 0.10).

EREC

For accuracy rates, there was a significant interaction between group and emotion (F(1,34) = 4.953, p = 0.03). This was driven by the lamotrigine group accurately recalling a significantly higher proportion of positive (8.78) vs negative words (6.44) compared to placebo (F(1,17) = 14.875, p = 0.001, Figure 1). Repeating this analysis with side effects as a covariate yielded a similar pattern of results (F(1,33) = 5.188, p = 0.03).

Figure 1. Accuracy of positive vs negative recalled words between treatment groups. Comparison between the number of recalled words (mean) for positively vs negatively valenced words in participants given lamotrigine vs placebo. Bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent a statistically significant difference between positive and negative words.

EMEM

For accuracy rates and RTs of correctly recalled words, there were no significant main effects of treatment and no significant interaction between group and emotion (all Fs < 1.34, all p s > 0.40).

Discussion

The present study investigated the effects of a single oral dose of lamotrigine (300 mg) on emotional cognition in a healthy volunteer sample following a randomized double-blind, parallel placebo-controlled design. As such, this study aimed to characterize the neuropsychological mechanisms underlying lamotrigine, to ultimately help enlighten how lamotrigine is beneficial in the prevention of depressive episodes in BD. Three hours after the treatment administration, the lamotrigine group had a significantly higher accuracy for the recall of positive versus negative words compared to placebo. There were no other statistically significant effects in the analysis of the remaining ETB measures. These results are in line with a positive bias induction in emotional memory following acute lamotrigine treatment. There were significant differences between groups in subjective ratings of physical symptoms but these were also seen before the administration of lamotrigine and most likely reflect a naturally occurring difficulty of group matching via randomization. Controlling for these differences statistically did not affect the positive bias in emotional memory seen following lamotrigine.

Lamotrigine induced a positive bias in memory recall

Participants in the lamotrigine group significantly recalled more positive than negative self-referential words in the emotional recall task, compared to those in the placebo group. This positive shift in memory has been extensively reported in experimental medicine studies of conventional antidepressant drugs using healthy volunteers. For instance, acute administration of mirtazapine or reboxetine, and 7-day treatment with citalopram or reboxetine increased positive affective memory in healthy participants (see Harmer, Cowen, & Goodwin, Reference Harmer, Cowen and Goodwin2011 for a review). Preclinical models of affective bias have also revealed early changes in negative memory biases with a range of treatments used in depression including mirtazapine and reboxetine (Stuart et al., Reference Stuart, Butler, Munafò, Nutt and Robinson2015). These positive shifts on emotional cognition have been proposed as a mechanism of antidepressant drug action, that is, by reversing negative biases believed to play a key role in maintenance of depression (for reviews see Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, Duman and Cowen2017, Harmer, Cowen, & Goodwin, Reference Harmer, Cowen and Goodwin2011). As such positive biases in affective memory may also be relevant to consider for the prophylactic effects of lamotrigine in depression in bipolar disorder.

It has been suggested that mood-congruent schema affect attention, interpretation, and memories about the world, oneself and the future in bipolar disorder (Paykel, Reference Paykel1987). Indeed in patients with BD with depressive symptomatology, negative biases are typically seen whereas in those with manic symptomatology, there is a bias toward more positive information (Bilderbeck et al., Reference Bilderbeck, Reed, McMahon, Atkinson, Price, Geddes and Harmer2016). A negative cognitive style in people with BD has been reported to be a vulnerability factor that, when activated, triggers a cycle of rumination and negative self-related thoughts that feed into depression (Hitchcock et al., Reference Hitchcock, Newby, Timm, Howard, Golden, Kuyken and Dalgleish2020; Van der Gucht, Morriss, Lancaster, Kinderman, & Bentall, Reference Van der Gucht, Morriss, Lancaster, Kinderman and Bentall2009). Further emotional memory biases in BD have been observed, with patients showing enhanced memory for content that is associated with adverse emotions and for neutral content that preceded the emotional stimulus (Bogie et al., Reference Bogie, Persaud, Smith, Kapczinski and Frey2019; Fijtman et al., Reference Fijtman, Bücker, Strange, Martins, Passos, Hasse-Sousa and Kauer-Sant’Anna2020). Additionally, both in BD depression and unipolar depression, patients show a worse recall for positive autobiographical memories, where these are less specific and more categorical, compared to healthy controls (Young, Bodurka, & Drevets, Reference Young, Bodurka and Drevets2016). Understanding how these affective and cognitive biases in BD interact with both disorder stages and treatment can be helpful to determine treatment efficacy, and explore new potential treatments for BD. Consistent with previous research on conventional antidepressants (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, Duman and Cowen2017), lamotrigine may be shifting depressive-related negative memory biases into more positive ones. These effects may also help stabilize mood and prevent the development of depressive symptomatology in BD. However, it is crucial to investigate whether these effects seen here in healthy participants would also be seen in patients with BD and whether they would be seen across a wider body of emotional processing measures in addition to memory.

Lamotrigine has an effect on inhibition of sodium and calcium channels in presynaptic neurons and the subsequent stabilization of glutamate release. Animal models suggest that the antidepressant behavioral effects of lamotrigine are mediated via downstream increases in brain-derived neurotrophic signaling (Li et al., Reference Li, He, Zhang, Qi, Li, Zhu and He2011). By which mechanism lamotrigine may have affected emotional memory here is unknown and requires further investigation. Nonetheless, animal and human studies have reported a clear effect of lamotrigine on memory. In mice, lamotrigine administration positively affected memory acquisition and spatial memory retrieval (Celikyurt et al., Reference Celikyurt, Ulak, Mutlu, Akar, Erden and Komsuoglu2012). In humans, 14 weeks of lamotrigine treatment in a pediatric BD sample improved working and verbal memory (Pavuluri, Passarotti, Mohammed, Carbray, & Sweeney, Reference Pavuluri, Passarotti, Mohammed, Carbray and Sweeney2010). In an adult BD sample, Haldane et al. (Reference Haldane, Jogia, Cobb, Kozuch, Kumari and Frangou2008) found that 12 weeks of lamotrigine monotherapy had positive effects on working memory, with increased brain activity when performing the N-back task in regions typically activated with this task. Our results suggest that changes in emotional memory may also be seen following lamotrigine compared to placebo in a double-blind randomized design.

Strength, limitations, and future directions

The present study is the first experimental medicine study investigating the effects of a single oral dose of lamotrigine in healthy volunteers. The use of healthy volunteers allows the characterization of the effects of a drug on cognitive-emotional functions that are relevant to a disorder, avoiding potential confounding factors such as differences in disorder progression or past treatments. Prior research has highlighted lamotrigine’s positive influence on memory; however, the current study explores, for the first time, its influence on the emotional memory domain, which is often affected in BD.

However, it is important to consider these results together with the study limitations. This study primarily found a significant effect on emotional memory (EREC), without widespread effects on other components of emotional processing. We had a relatively small sample (n = 36) which gave rise to reduced statistical power and we also did not correct for the multiple statistical comparisons across the ETB tasks in the current study which increases the risk for type 1 errors.

Participants in the lamotrigine group also exhibited higher trait anxiety levels, possibly causing an imbalance between groups. This might have affected their self-assessment of side effects, as the groups differed on this measure even at baseline. Nonetheless, despite this potentially negative valence experience, the lamotrigine group still displayed a positive memory bias. Finally, the sample involved in this study was comprised of healthy volunteers, and while these may provide advantages in the exploration of lamotrigine’s effect on emotional cognition, it is important to consider that different underlying mechanisms may exist between healthy and patient populations. Future research should explore a similar paradigm in patients with BD to further clarify these results and assess whether they are related to the clinical effects of lamotrigine in BD.

Conclusions

This study evaluated the effects of a single oral dose of lamotrigine in healthy volunteers on a range of cognitive-emotional functions using a well-validated task battery. Lamotrigine significantly increased accurate recall of positive versus negative self-referential descriptors compared to placebo without wider effects on emotional processing. There results suggest that emotional memory may be a useful target to explore in future studies of lamotrigine’s clinical action in bipolar disorder.

Acknowledgments

This research study was supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre (OH BRC). The views expressed here are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health. Liliana Capitão currently works at the Psychology Research Centre (PSI/01662), School of Psychology, University of Minho, supported by the Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT) through the Portuguese State Budget (Ref.: UID/PSI/01662/2020). She is individually funded by a Research Fellowship also from the Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT) (Ref: 2021.00415.CEECIND).

Author contribution

TZ was involved in the study conception, data collection, data analysis, and manuscript preparation. PAH was involved in data analysis and data checks and prepared the first manuscript draft. LW and AK were involved in data collection and manuscript preparation. GG was involved in the study design, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. LPC analyzed data, oversaw the study, and was involved in data interpretation and manuscript preparation. CJH was involved in the study conception, oversaw the study, and was involved in data interpretation and manuscript preparation.

Competing interests

CJH has received consultancy fees from P1 vital Ltd., Janssen Pharmaceuticals, Sage Therapeutics, Pfizer, Zogenix, Compass Pathways, and Lundbeck. CJH holds grant income from Zogenix, UCB Pharma, Pfizer, and Janssen Pharmaceuticals. GMG is Chief Medical Officer at Compass Pathways, holds shares and share options in Compass Pathways PLC and has served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker in the last 3 years for Beckley Psytech, Boehringer Ingelheim, Clerkenwell Health, Compass Pathways, Evapharma, Janssen, Lundbeck, Medscape, Novartis, Ocean Neuroscience, P1Vital, Servier, and Takeda.

Ethical standard

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

L.P.C. and C.J.H. are joint senior authors.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Bipolar and related disorders. In Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed., text rev.). American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Bearden, C. E., Glahn, D. C., Monkul, E. S., Barrett, J., Najt, P., Villarreal, V., & Soares, J. C. (2006). Patterns of memory impairment in bipolar disorder and unipolar major depression. Psychiatry Research, 142(2–3), 139150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilderbeck, A. C., Reed, Z. E., McMahon, H. C., Atkinson, L. Z., Price, J., Geddes, J. R., … Harmer, C. J. (2016). Associations between mood instability and emotional processing in a large cohort of bipolar patients. Psychological Medicine, 46(15), 31513160. https://doi.org/10.1017/s003329171600180xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogie, B. J. M., Persaud, M. R., Smith, D., Kapczinski, F. P., & Frey, B. N. (2019). Explicit emotional memory biases in mood disorders: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research, 278, 162172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.06.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celikyurt, I. K., Ulak, G., Mutlu, O., Akar, F. Y., Erden, F., & Komsuoglu, S. S. (2012). Lamotrigine, a mood stabilizer, may have beneficial effects on memory acquisition and retrieval in mice. Life Sciences, 91(25–26), 12701274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lfs.2012.09.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckersbach, T., Savage, C. R., Reilly-Harrington, N., Clark, L., Sachs, G., & Rauch, S. L. (2004). Episodic memory impairment in bipolar disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: The role of memory strategies. Bipolar Disorders, 6(3), 233244. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00118.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck personality questionnaire (junior and adult). Kent: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
Fijtman, A., Bücker, J., Strange, B. A., Martins, D. S., Passos, I. C., Hasse-Sousa, M., … Kauer-Sant’Anna, M. (2020). Emotional memory in bipolar disorder: Impact of multiple episodes and childhood trauma. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 206213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Global Burden of Disease Study, C. (2015). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 301 acute and chronic diseases and injuries in 188 countries, 1990–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. The Lancet, 386(9995), 743800. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60692-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godlewska, B. R., & Harmer, C. J. (2021). Cognitive neuropsychological theory of antidepressant action: A modern-day approach to depression and its treatment. Psychopharmacology, 238(5), 12651278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-019-05448-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, D. R., Wagstaff, A. J., Ibbotson, T., & Perry, C. M. (2003). Lamotrigine: A review of its use in bipolar disorder. Drugs, 63(19), 20292050. https://doi.org/10.2165/00003495-200363190-00009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haldane, M., Jogia, J., Cobb, A., Kozuch, E., Kumari, V., & Frangou, S. (2008). Changes in brain activation during working memory and facial recognition tasks in patients with bipolar disorder with lamotrigine monotherapy. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 18(1), 4854. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, C. J. (2010). Antidepressant drug action: A neuropsychological perspective. Depression and Anxiety, 27(3), 266273. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmer, C. J., Cowen, P. J., & Goodwin, G. M. (2011). Efficacy markers in depression. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25(9), 11481158. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881110367722CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, C. J., Duman, R. S., & Cowen, P. J. (2017). How do antidepressants work? New perspectives for refining future treatment approaches. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(5), 409418. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(17)30015-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, C. J., O’Sullivan, U., Favaron, E., Massey-Chase, R., Ayres, R., Reinecke, A., … Cowen, P. J. (2009). Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(10), 11781184. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitchcock, C., Newby, J., Timm, E., Howard, R. M., Golden, A. M., Kuyken, W., & Dalgleish, T. (2020). Memory category fluency, memory specificity, and the fading affect bias for positive and negative autobiographical events: Performance on a good day-bad day task in healthy and depressed individuals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(1), 198206. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000617CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Judd, L. L., Akiskal, H. S., Schettler, P. J., Endicott, J., Maser, J., Solomon, D. A., … Keller, M. B. (2002). The long-term natural history of the weekly symptomatic status of bipolar I disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(6), 530537. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.6.530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ketter, T. A., Manji, H. K., & Post, R. M. (2003). Potential mechanisms of action of lamotrigine in the treatment of bipolar disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 23(5), 484495. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.jcp.0000088915.02635.e8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C.-Y., Fu, W.-M., Chen, C.-C., Su, M.-J., & Liou, H.-H. (2008). Lamotrigine inhibits postsynaptic AMPA receptor and glutamate release in the dentate gyrus. Epilepsia, 49(5), 888897. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01526.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, N., He, X., Zhang, Y., Qi, X., Li, H., Zhu, X., & He, S. (2011). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor signalling mediates antidepressant effects of lamotrigine. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 14(8), 10911102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malhi, G. S., Mitchell, P. B., & Salim, S. (2003). Bipolar depression: Management options. CNS Drugs, 17(1), 925. https://doi.org/10.2165/00023210-200317010-00002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H., & Willison, J. (1991). The revised national adult reading test: Test manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Pavuluri, M. N., Passarotti, A. M., Mohammed, T., Carbray, J. A., & Sweeney, J. A. (2010). Enhanced working and verbal memory after lamotrigine treatment in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 12(2), 213220. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00792.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paykel, E. S. (1987). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders: A. T. Beck. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150(6), 870871. https://doi.org/10.1192/S0007125000214918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prica, C., Hascoet, M., & Bourin, M. (2008). Antidepressant-like effect of lamotrigine is reversed by veratrine: A possible role of sodium channels in bipolar depression. Behavioural Brain Research, 191(1), 4954. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanacora, G., Zarate, C. A., Krystal, J. H., & Manji, H. K. (2008). Targeting the glutamatergic system to develop novel, improved therapeutics for mood disorders. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery, 7(5), 426437. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrd2462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, K., Cowen, P. J., Harmer, C. J., Tzortzis, G., Errington, S., & Burnet, P. W. (2015). Prebiotic intake reduces the waking cortisol response and alters emotional bias in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 232(10), 17931801. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-014-3810-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sills, G. J., & Rogawski, M. A. (2020). Mechanisms of action of currently used antiseizure drugs. Neuropharmacology, 168, 107966. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2020.107966CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Stuart, S. A., Butler, P., Munafò, M. R., Nutt, D. J., & Robinson, E. S. (2015). Distinct neuropsychological mechanisms may explain delayed- versus rapid-onset antidepressant efficacy. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(9), 21652174. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vajda, F. J., Dodd, S., & Horgan, D. (2013). Lamotrigine in epilepsy, pregnancy and psychiatry—a drug for all seasons? Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 20(1), 1316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2012.05.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Gucht, E., Morriss, R., Lancaster, G., Kinderman, P., & Bentall, R. P. (2009). Psychological processes in bipolar affective disorder: Negative cognitive style and reward processing. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 194(2), 146151. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.047894CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, M. B., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2019). Subchronic treatment with St John’s wort produces a positive shift in emotional processing in healthy volunteers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 33(2), 194201. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881118812101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. W., Rowland, D., Calder, A. J., Etcoff, N. L., Seth, A., & Perrett, D. I. (1997). Facial expression megamix: Tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition, 63(3), 271313. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00003-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, K. D., Bodurka, J., & Drevets, W. C. (2016). Differential neural correlates of autobiographical memory recall in bipolar and unipolar depression. Bipolar Disorders, 18(7), 571582. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12441CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Figure 1. Accuracy of positive vs negative recalled words between treatment groups. Comparison between the number of recalled words (mean) for positively vs negatively valenced words in participants given lamotrigine vs placebo. Bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent a statistically significant difference between positive and negative words.