Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T20:24:19.634Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychotic traits in musicians

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2018

Oliver Mason*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Stag Hill, Guildford, Surrey
Heidi Daniels
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Stag Hill, Guildford, Surrey
*
Author for correspondence: Oliver Mason, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The association of music with madness is very longstanding. But is it more than myth, and if so what is the nature of this relationship? We tested the hypotheses that musicians possess greater schizotypy and symptoms of bipolar disorder. A total of 102 musicians were found to have greater positive and negative schizotypal traits when compared to matched norms on the shortened Oxford–Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences. Based on the Mood Disorder Questionnaire, 10.8% of musicians also met criteria for lifetime bipolar disorder. Rock musicians appeared to have greater symptoms than those performing in other musical genres.

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Introduction

From antiquity, music and madness have been seen as intertwined in the popular imagination. In past centuries, this common cultural trope has alighted on several notable musicians exemplified by romantics such as Franz Schubert (Deaville, Reference Deaville, Howe, Jensen-Moulton, Lerner and Straus2006). In a contemporary context, this fascination has twinned with countercultural ideas in the more ‘alternative’ corners of music notably ‘heavy metal’ (Walser, Reference Walser1993), jazz and rock genres (Farrington, Reference Farrington2015). Affective and psychotic illnesses have often been ‘identified’ in the biographies of musically creative individuals; but few empirical studies have tested the association of musicianship with psychopathology or related personality traits. Preti & Vellante (Reference Preti and Vellante2007) identified a higher level of sub-clinical delusional beliefs in thirty musicians (amongst a group of 25 painters and 25 writers) when compared with non-creative controls. Rankin (Reference Rankin2006) identified a distinctive pattern of personality traits in a group of popular/rock musicians suggesting that they possessed high levels of neuroticism, openness to experience and extraversion, as well as responses reflecting depressive, obsessive–compulsive and psychotic tendencies.

Associations of psychopathology with creativity are not limited to musicianship, with a plethora of claims that include schizophrenia (Sass, Reference Sass2000) and bipolar disorder (Jamison, Reference Jamison1993). However, probably the best empirical evidence to date links creativity to affective instability, and bipolar disorder in particular. Nowakowska et al. (Reference Nowakowska, Strong, Santosa, Wang and Ketter2005) documented greater cyclothymic, dysthymic and irritability traits in both bipolar and creative groups. Santosa et al. (Reference Santosa, Strong, Nowakowska, Wang, Rennicke and Ketter2007) also found superior creativity in a bipolar disorder group; other evidence suggests that this may especially be the case during manic phases (Soeiro-de Souza et al. Reference Soeiro-de-Souza, Dias, Bio, Post and Moreno2011). The link is probably not confined to bipolarity: in a Swedish population-wide study (Kyaga et al. Reference Kyaga, Lichtenstein, Boman, Hultman, Långström and Landén2011) those in a creative profession possessed a greater likelihood of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression, suicide and substance abuse.

Elevated schizotypal traits have also been found in a range of creative groups (see Holt, Reference Holt, Mason and Claridge2015, for review), though musicians have not received specific attention. We used here the same methodology as recent studies of both comedians (Ando et al. Reference Ando, Claridge and Clark2014) and poets (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Mort and Woo2015). We predicted that musicians would possess both greater self-reported schizotypy and bipolar symptoms. We were also interested to explore the relationships between musical role/genre and traits/symptoms.

Method

Musicians were recruited by emails to online music networks and places of musical study. Participants completed an online survey based on an earlier study of poets (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Mort and Woo2015), but adapted to suit musicians. In addition to the Oxford–Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE: Mason et al. Reference Mason, Linney and Claridge2005) and Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ) (Hirschfeld, Reference Hirschfeld2000) the survey covered demographics and musical experience/expertise. The normative sample was also taken from Mason et al. (Reference Mason, Mort and Woo2015). The study was approved by the University of Surrey. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Results

A total of 102 musicians (24 females, 78 males) were aged from 16 to 81 years (M = 38.51, s.d. = 15.74), had a mean number of years involvement with the music of 22.06 (M = 15.00), and comprised both amateur (48) and professional/semi-professional (52). Most performed on an instrument (94); around half were vocalists (49) and/or composers (48). Many composed lyrics (44), while a minority chose ‘conductor’ (9) or turntablist/DJ (3). In terms of genre, 58 chose rock music, 31 classical, 35 blues, 34 jazz, 31 blues and 24 folk/world.

O-LIFE and MDQ scores

Table 1 shows mean scores for the groups on the four O-LIFE scales and the results of MANOVA. There were highly significant differences favouring higher scores in musicians for all schizotypy subscale scores. According to the MDQ criteria, 14 musicians (14%) met diagnostic criteria for bipolar disorder at some point in the past. This can be compared to the 3.7% base rate identified the US general population on the same measure (Hirschfeld et al. Reference Hirschfeld, Calabrese, Weissman, Reed, Davies and Frye2003).

Table 1. Scores on the O-LIFE for Musician and Normative groups

** p <.01

In terms of roles, many categories were too numerous or rare for formal analysis. However, composers, lyricists and vocalists were all found to be higher in the Cognitive Disorganisation sub-scale than those not identifying in these roles [composer M = 6.08 (3.01) v. non-composer M = 4.68 (2.74), t = 2.46, p < 0.05; lyricist M = 6.43 (2.69) v. non-lyricist M = 4.53 (2.89), t = 3.39, p < 0.01; vocalist M = 6.10 (2.73), non-vocalist M = 4.66 (2.99), t = 2.54, p < 0.05]. However, these categories of musicianship were highly overlapping. Of genres, only rock musicians reported a higher number of bipolar symptoms than those not so self-identifying [7.38 (3.56) v. 5.58 (4.24), t = 2.312, p < 0.05].

Discussion

In line with our hypothesis, musicians showed a high level of psychosis-prone personality traits consistent with several other studies of creative groups that included musicians (Nowakowska et al. Reference Nowakowska, Strong, Santosa, Wang and Ketter2005; Rankin, Reference Rankin2006; Preti & Vellante, Reference Preti and Vellante2007). They did so across all measures of schizotypy, as well as on self-reported symptoms of bipolar disorder. Self-reported bipolar symptoms were seen at a similar level (14%) to a comparable sample of poets (18%: Mason et al. Reference Mason, Mort and Woo2015), and exceeded the general population by a factor of four. The elevated levels of positive schizotypy were also broadly similar to those seen in both poets and comedians (Ando et al. Reference Ando, Claridge and Clark2014). In common with the comedians but not the poets, negative schizotypy – social withdrawal and anhedonia – was also more commonly seen in musicians. Though not always seen in creativity studies, there is evidence of its relationship to divergent thinking (Cox & Leon, Reference Cox and Leon1999) suggesting it may have a relationship to cognitive processes contributing to creativity. Perhaps the most intriguing finding relates to ‘rock’ musicianship and bipolar symptoms, though again this was not specifically hypothesised and many other music genres were too poorly represented to be appropriately tested. Given the cultural construction of the ‘rock rebel’ with an expression of anger and even mental instability (Walser, Reference Walser1993; Deaville, Reference Deaville, Howe, Jensen-Moulton, Lerner and Straus2006; Farrington, Reference Farrington2015), this area is definitely deserving of further investigation.

Limitations

The methodology does not allow estimation of the response rate or representativeness of the sample and may have attracted respondents with greater distress. Musical experience, traits and symptoms are self-reported and not corroborated by independent assessment.

Acknowledgements

Thanks are due to the many anonymous musicians who helped complete the study. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

The authors have no personal, professional or financial competing interests to declare.

References

Ando, V, Claridge, G and Clark, K (2014) Psychotic traits in comedians. The British Journal of Psychiatry 204, 341345.Google Scholar
Cox, AJ and Leon, JL (1999) Negative schizotypal traits in the relation of creativity to psychopathology. Creativity Research Journal 12, 2536.Google Scholar
Deaville, D (2006) Sounds of mind: music and madness in the popular imagination. In Howe, B, Jensen-Moulton, S, Lerner, N and Straus, J (eds). The Oxford Handbook of Music and Disability Studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 640652.Google Scholar
Farrington, C (2015) Music and madness: from kontakte to the cure. The Lancet Psychiatry 2, 388390.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, RM, Calabrese, JR, Weissman, MM, Reed, M, Davies, MA, Frye, M et al. (2003) Screening for bipolar disorder in the community. Journal of Clinical Psychology 64, 5359.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, RMA (2000) Development and validation of a screening instrument for bipolar spectrum disorder: the mood disorder questionnaire. The American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 18731875.Google Scholar
Holt, N (2015) Schizotypy: a creative advantage? In Mason, OJ and Claridge, G (eds). Schizotypy: New Dimensions. Oxford: Routledge, pp. 231243.Google Scholar
Jamison, KR (1993) Touched with Fire: Manic-Depressive Illness and the Artistic Temperament. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Kyaga, S, Lichtenstein, P, Boman, M, Hultman, C, Långström, N and Landén, M (2011) Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300000 people with severe mental disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry 199, 373379.Google Scholar
Mason, O, Linney, Y and Claridge, G (2005) Short scales for measuring schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research 78, 293296.Google Scholar
Mason, OJ, Mort, H and Woo, J (2015) Investigating psychotic traits in poets. Psychological Medicine 45, 667.Google Scholar
Nowakowska, C, Strong, CM, Santosa, CM, Wang, PW and Ketter, TA (2005) Temperamental commonalities and differences in euthymic mood disorder patients, creative controls, and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders 85, 207215.Google Scholar
Preti, A and Vellante, M. (2007) Creativity and psychopathology: higher rates of psychosis proneness and non right-handedness among creative artists compared to same age and gender peers. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 195, 837845.Google Scholar
Rankin, C (2006) The popular musician: personality traits and their relationship to creativity, self-esteem and clinical symptomatology. Unpublished poster at 114th conference of the American Psychological Association, August 10-13 2006, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Santosa, CM, Strong, CM, Nowakowska, C, Wang, PW, Rennicke, CM and Ketter, TA (2007) Enhanced creativity in bipolar disorder patients: a controlled study. Journal of Affective Disorders 100, 3139.Google Scholar
Sass, LA (2000) Schizophrenia, modernism, and the “creative imagination”: on creativity and psychopathology. Creativity Research Journal 13, 5574.Google Scholar
Soeiro-de-Souza, MG, Dias, VV, Bio, DS, Post, RM and Moreno, RA (2011) Creativity and executive function across manic, mixed and depressive episodes in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 135, 292297.Google Scholar
Walser, R (1993) Running with the Devil: Power, Gender and Madness in Heavy Metal Music. Connecticut: Weslyan University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Scores on the O-LIFE for Musician and Normative groups