Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T08:28:29.791Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pathological personality dimensions and neurobiological emotional reactivity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 October 2024

Sarah B. Barkley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Psychology B Building, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Jacob Feldman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Adina Levy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Psychology B Building, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Alex Grieshaber
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Psychology B Building, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Brady D. Nelson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Psychology B Building, Stony Brook, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Sarah B. Barkley; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP) offers a promising framework to identify the neurobiological mechanisms of psychopathology. Many forms of psychopathology are characterized by dysfunctional emotional reactivity. The late positive potential (LPP) is an event-related potential component that provides an index of neurobiological emotional reactivity. Several categorical disorders have demonstrated a similar association with the emotion-modulated LPP. It is possible that higher-order dimensional representations of psychopathology might explain the comparable results. The present study examined the association between HiTOP-consistent pathological personality dimensions across multiple levels of the hierarchy and neurobiological emotional reactivity.

Methods

The sample included 215 18–35-year-old adults (86% female) who were oversampled for psychopathology. Participants completed the emotional interrupt task while electroencephalography was recorded to examine the LPP. Participants also completed the Comprehensive Assessment of Traits relevant to Personality Disorders to assess pathological personality.

Results

At the spectra level, higher negative emotionality was associated with a larger emotion-modulated LPP, while higher detachment was associated with a smaller emotion-modulated LPP. There were no associations between higher-order psychopathology levels and the emotion-modulated LPP. Compared to categorical diagnoses, spectra-level personality pathology dimensions significantly improved the prediction of the emotion-modulated LPP.

Conclusions

The present study indicates that HiTOP spectra levels of negative emotionality and detachment demonstrate unique associations with neurobiological emotional reactivity. The study highlights the utility of examining dimensional and hierarchical, rather than categorical, representations of psychopathology in the attempt to identify the neurobiological origins of psychopathology.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP) is an empirically derived model that offers a promising framework for the classification of psychopathology (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017). The dimensional and hierarchical nature of the model accounts for both higher-order commonalities and lower-order distinctions between psychopathologies, thereby addressing the long-standing issues of heterogeneity within and comorbidity between disorders using traditional categorical classifications (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Wright2021). Importantly, these framework characteristics make HiTOP-derived dimensions ideal targets to determine how neurobiological mechanisms play a role in psychopathology (Latzman, DeYoung, & HITOP Neurobiological Foundations Workgroup, Reference Latzman and DeYoung2020; Perkins, Latzman, & Patrick, Reference Perkins, Latzman and Patrick2020).

It has been a challenge to directly measure HiTOP-derived dimensions that are largely based on factor analysis. Symptom-based scales often focus on a particular spectrum or form of psychopathology and do not capture the full range of psychopathology. Personality measures can be useful in comprehensively measuring most experiences and behaviors that constitute the wide range of psychopathology. Both the five-factor model (Costa & McCrae, Reference Costa and McCrae1992) and alternative model of personality disorders (Krueger, Derringer, Markon, Watson, & Skodol, Reference Krueger, Derringer, Markon, Watson and Skodol2012) have demonstrated that hierarchical personality trait systems can provide a general structural framework for psychopathology (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Thomas, Hopwood, Markon, Pincus and Krueger2012). Notably, both normative and pathological personality are directly represented in the HiTOP framework (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Wright2021). At the spectra level, HiTOP includes five domains (i.e. internalizing, detachment, thought disorder, disinhibited externalizing, and antagonistic externalizing) which have links to the five-factor model of personality (e.g. internalizing to neuroticism, detachment to introversion). Additionally, these five domains are closely aligned with the pathological personality dimensions suggested in the alternative model of personality disorders (i.e. negative affectivity, detachment, psychoticism, disinhibition, and antagonism). Therefore, personality provides an ideal foundation for measuring HiTOP dimensions (Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019). Recent research has suggested immense promise in using these HiTOP-inspired personality dimensions to better understand neurobiological mechanisms of psychopathology (Perkins et al., Reference Perkins, Latzman and Patrick2020).

Emotional reactivity is central to many forms of psychopathology (e.g. Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991; Rottenberg, Gross, & Gotlib, Reference Rottenberg, Gross and Gotlib2005), and there is growing research establishing the neurobiological indicators of emotional reactivity. Electroencephalography (EEG) is an ideal tool to measure neurobiological emotional reactivity due to its excellent temporal resolution, sensitivity to subtle characteristics of emotional stimuli, and ability to be measured across the lifespan. The late positive potential (LPP) is an EEG event-related potential (ERP) component hypothesized to index attentional and elaborative processing of motivationally salient information. The LPP begins 200 ms after stimulus onset (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, Reference Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer and Lang2000) and is maximal at centroparietal electrodes (Hajcak & Olvet, Reference Hajcak and Olvet2008). The LPP is larger in response to pleasant and unpleasant relative to neutral stimuli (Weinberg & Hajcak, Reference Weinberg and Hajcak2010) – and this increased response is known as the emotion-modulated LPP.

Several studies have utilized the emotion-modulated LPP to explore emotional dysregulation in psychopathology. Across internalizing disorders, a more blunted LPP to emotional (Weinberg, Perlman, Kotov, & Hajcak, Reference Weinberg, Perlman, Kotov and Hajcak2016) and unpleasant stimuli (Foti, Olvet, Klein, & Hajcak, Reference Foti, Olvet, Klein and Hajcak2010; MacNamara, Kotov, & Hajcak, Reference MacNamara, Kotov and Hajcak2016) has been associated with depression, while a more enhanced LPP to emotional (Weinberg & Sandre, Reference Weinberg and Sandre2018) and unpleasant stimuli (MacNamara et al., Reference MacNamara, Kotov and Hajcak2016; Moser, Huppert, Duval, & Simons, Reference Moser, Huppert, Duval and Simons2008) has been associated with generalized anxiety, panic, and social anxiety. The similar findings across both pleasant and unpleasant stimuli in depression are consistent with the emotion context insensitivity theory (Rottenberg et al., Reference Rottenberg, Gross and Gotlib2005), suggesting that depression may be related to overall attenuated emotional reactivity. Similar results across purportedly distinct disorders suggest there might be higher-order commonalities that better represent the association between neurobiological emotionality reactivity and psychopathology. Indeed, this is consistent with prominent models and theories of psychopathology. For example, a tripartite model suggests that high negative emotionality is characteristic of both depression and anxiety disorders, while low positive emotionality is more unique to depression (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991). Interestingly, one study found that a higher-order internalizing dimension was associated with a larger emotion-modulated LPP (Rozalski & Benning, Reference Rozalski and Benning2019), suggesting that the shared aspect between the disorders (i.e. negative emotionality) may be affiliated with enhanced emotional reactivity. In contrast, emotional dimensions related to detachment have been associated with decreased emotional reactivity. For example, low positive affect has been associated with a blunted LPP to both pleasant and unpleasant images (Weinberg & Sandre, Reference Weinberg and Sandre2018). The combination of a blunted LPP and heightened amygdala activity to unpleasant stimuli has also been shown to prospectively predict increased dysphoria (Bauer, Wilson, Phan, Shankman, & MacNamara, Reference Bauer, Wilson, Phan, Shankman and MacNamara2023). Moreover, a separate study found that extraversion was associated with an enhanced LPP to emotional images (Speed et al., Reference Speed, Nelson, Perlman, Klein, Kotov and Hajcak2015), suggesting that introversion (and its maladaptive form detachment) might be associated with a more blunted emotion-modulated LPP.

Across externalizing disorders, a more blunted LPP to unpleasant stimuli has been associated with psychopathic traits (Medina, Kirilko, & Grose-Fifer, Reference Medina, Kirilko and Grose-Fifer2016; Sadeh & Verona, Reference Sadeh and Verona2012) and binge drinking (Connell, Patton, & McKillop, Reference Connell, Patton and McKillop2015), while a larger LPP to unpleasant stimuli has been associated with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Shushakova, Ohrmann, & Pedersen, Reference Shushakova, Ohrmann and Pedersen2018). The opposite results, as well as one investigation showing that a higher-order externalizing factor was not associated with the LPP (Rozalski & Benning, Reference Rozalski and Benning2019), suggest that it is important to distinguish between disinhibition and antagonism when examining emotion dysregulation in externalizing psychopathology. It is possible that higher-order dimensional representations of psychopathology, such as pathological personality, might explain the overlapping as well as distinct results. However, to date no study has comprehensively examined pathological personality dimensions and the emotion-modulated LPP.

The present study examined the association between pathological personality dimensions and neurobiological emotional reactivity. Importantly, the study examined these relationships across different hierarchical levels of psychopathology, including psychopathology spectra, lower-order traits, and higher-order dimensions. The final sample included 215 18–35-year-old adults who were oversampled for lifetime psychopathology and completed the emotional interrupt task while EEG was recorded to measure the LPP. Participants also completed the self-report Comprehensive Assessment of Traits relevant to Personality Disorder (CAT-PD; Simms et al., Reference Simms, Goldberg, Roberts, Watson, Welte and Rotterman2011) to assess negative emotionality, detachment, psychoticism, disinhibition, antagonism, and anankastia spectra as well as affiliated lower-order maladaptive traits (Ringwald et al., Reference Ringwald, Emery, Khoo, Clark, Kotelnikova, Scalco and Simms2023a). The CAT-PD spectra can also be combined to estimate higher-order dimensions. To assess lifetime categorical diagnoses, participants completed the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-5 (SCID-5). Based on the reviewed literature, we hypothesized that relationship between personality pathology and the emotion-modulated LPP would exist at the spectra level but not the lower-order maladaptive trait or higher-order dimension (e.g. internalizing, externalizing) levels. Specifically, we hypothesized that negative emotionality and disinhibition would be associated with a larger emotion-modulated LPP, while detachment and antagonism would be associated with a smaller emotion-modulated LPP. We did not make specific hypotheses about the other pathological personality domains (psychoticism and anankastia) due to the more limited literature between these dimensions and the LPP. We also hypothesized that dimensional measures of personality pathology would significantly improve the prediction of the emotion-modulated LPP compared to the use of categorical diagnoses.

Method

Participants

The study consisted of 249 18–35-year-olds (M = 23.06, s.d. = 3.82; 86% assigned sex female). Participant racial identity included 64.0% White, 9.2% Asian, 8.3% more than one race, 7.8% Black or African American, and 0.4% Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander. Participant ethnicity included 20.6% Hispanic. Participant sexual orientation included 45.3% heterosexual, 24.9% bisexual, 9.8% pansexual, 8.4% queer, 5.8% homosexual, 4.0% questioning/unsure, 1.3% prefer not to say, and 0.4% asexual. Participant education level included 8.7% high school graduate, 60.5% partial college, 25.2% college graduate, and 5.8% graduate/professional training.

Participants were recruited from the Long Island (68.98%) and New York City (31.02%) areas via online advertisements (Instagram, Facebook, Reddit), fliers, and word of mouth. Twelve different advertisements were used that targeted the cardinal symptom of multiple forms of psychopathology (ADHD, alcohol and substance use disorders, bipolar disorders, depressive disorders, eating disorders, generalized anxiety disorder, intermittent explosive disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, schizophrenia spectrum and other psychotic disorders, and social anxiety disorder). Participants were screened using a two-step approach: first, interested participants were contacted via phone and administered the SCID-5 (First, Williams, Karg, & Spitzer, Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2015) screening questions; if at least one cardinal symptom was endorsed, participants completed an online self-report questionnaire containing subscales from the Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms–Expanded Version (Watson et al., Reference Watson, O'Hara, Naragon-Gainey, Koffel, Chmielewski, Kotov and Ruggero2012), the Externalizing Spectrum Inventory–Brief Form (Patrick, Kramer, Krueger, and Markon, Reference Patrick, Kramer, Krueger and Markon2013), and the CAT-PD (Simms et al., Reference Simms, Goldberg, Roberts, Watson, Welte and Rotterman2011) consistent with the endorsed form of psychopathology. Participants were enrolled in the study if they had a normed T score ⩾65. Exclusion criteria were active auditory or visual hallucinations, autism spectrum disorder, Down syndrome, history of select neurological conditions (cerebral palsy, epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson's disease, stroke, traumatic brain injury), head injury associated with a loss of consciousness of 60 s or more, or an inability to read or write in English. Participants were compensated $25/h. Study procedures were approved by the Stony Brook University Institutional Review Board and informed consent was obtained from all participants in the study. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Measures

Comprehensive Assessment of Traits relevant to Personality Disorder

The CAT-PD is a 216-item self-report measure of traits relevant to personality disorder. The CAT-PD consists of 33 facet traits of personality pathology that are consistent with the PSY-5 structure of personality traits (Wright & Simms, Reference Wright and Simms2014) and the alternative model of personality disorder in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The traits can further be combined to measure higher-order domains consistent with the HiTOP model spectra level. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Very Untrue of Me) to 5 (Very True of Me). The CAT-PD has demonstrated good internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and convergent validity (Long, Reinhard, Sellbom, & Anderson, Reference Long, Reinhard, Sellbom and Anderson2021). The present study examined six-factor analytically derived domains that were calculated by applying factor loadings to each trait and summing all values: negative emotionality, detachment, psychoticism, disinhibition, antagonism, and anankastia (Ringwald et al., Reference Ringwald, Emery, Khoo, Clark, Kotelnikova, Scalco and Simms2023a). Cronbach's α for the domains were calculated using items from traits that loaded highest on that domain and were as follows: negative emotionality α = 0.87, detachment α = 0.76, psychoticism α = 0.84, disinhibition α = 0.91, antagonism α = 0.92, and anankastia α = 0.58.

Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-5 – Research Version (SCID-5-RV)

The SCID-5-RV was administered to the participants to determine lifetime presence of categorical psychopathology. Interviews were administered by B.A./B.S., M.A., or Ph.D. level interviewers who were supervised by clinical psychologists. All interviewers were trained by watching the SCID-101 training videos, watching two–three video-recorded interviews, and co-administering two–three interviews. All diagnoses were discussed as a best-estimate consensus meeting involving clinical psychologists where a final diagnosis was determined.

Procedure

Emotional interrupt task

The LPP was examined using a modified version of the emotional interrupt task (Weinberg & Hajcak, Reference Weinberg and Hajcak2011), which required participants to respond to a target (left- or right-pointing arrow) presented in between the presentation of the same emotional picture. The emotional interrupt task provides advantages over passive picture-viewing, including confirmation of participant attention by only examining trials in which target response was correct. Each trial consisted of a fixation cross (800 ms), followed by a neutral, pleasant, or unpleasant picture (1000 ms), followed by either a left (<) or right (>) pointing arrow (i.e. the target; 150 ms), followed by the same picture that had preceded the target (400 ms). The intertrial interval consisted of a blank screen and ranged from 1500 to 2000 ms. The task included 120 trials (40 neutral, 40 pleasant, 40 unpleasant) presented in a random order. Pictures were selected from the International Affective Picture System (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, Reference Lang, Bradley and Cuthbert2008). Pleasant and unpleasant images were matched on normative arousal ratings (see online Supplemental materials). The final task included 20 neutral pictures displaying objects or scenes with people, 20 pleasant pictures displaying cute animals or babies and erotica, and 20 unpleasant pictures displaying animal or gun threat and mutilation. Each picture was presented twice during the task. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible to the target (left or right arrow) by clicking the corresponding left or right mouse button.

EEG recoding and data processing

Continuous EEG was collected using the ActiveTwo BioSemi system (BioSemi, the Netherlands). A total of 34 electrodes (standard 32 plus FCz and Iz) were used based on the international 10/20 system plus two electrodes placed on the left and right mastoids. Electro-oculogram activity was recorded using four facial electrodes: horizontal eye movements were measured via two electrodes located approximately 1 cm outside the outer canthus of the left and right eyes; vertical eye movements and blinks were measured via two electrodes placed approximately 1 cm above and below the right eye. The EEG signal was pre-amplified at the electrode to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. The data were digitized at a 24-bit resolution with a sampling rate of 1024 Hz using a low-pass fifth-order sinc filter with a half-power cut-off of 204.8 Hz. Active electrodes were measured online with respect to a common mode sense active electrode producing a monopolar channel. Data were re-referenced offline to the average of the left and right mastoids and band-pass filtered from 0.1 and 30 Hz. Eye blink and ocular corrections were conducted using established standards (Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, Reference Gratton, Coles and Donchin1983).

A semiautomatic procedure was employed to detect and reject artifacts. The criteria applied were a voltage step of more than 50.0 μV between sample points, a voltage difference of 300.0 μV within a trial, or a maximum voltage difference of less than 0.50 μV within 100 ms intervals. Visual data inspection was conducted to detect and reject remaining artifacts. Trials were excluded if there was an artifact, if a response was incorrect or not provided, or if target reaction time was less than 150 ms (M = 7.06%, s.d. = 9.80%). The average number of trials available for data analysis included 37.56 (s.d. = 4.11) for neutral images, 37.00 (s.d. = 4.03) for pleasant images, and 36.99 (s.d. = 4.19) for unpleasant images, which did not differ between conditions (p = 0.26).

The EEG was segmented for each trial beginning 200 ms before the pre-target picture and continuing for 1200 ms (i.e. the entire duration of the pre-target picture presentation). The baseline was 200 ms prior to picture onset. The LPP was extracted as the average activity between 300 and 1000 ms after picture onset and separate averages were computed for neutral, pleasant, and unpleasant pictures. Consistent with prior literature examining the association between the LPP and psychopathology (Bauer et al., Reference Bauer, Wilson, Phan, Shankman and MacNamara2023; MacNamara et al., Reference MacNamara, Kotov and Hajcak2016; Rozalski & Benning, Reference Rozalski and Benning2019; Speed et al., Reference Speed, Nelson, Perlman, Klein, Kotov and Hajcak2015), the current study focused on the emotion-modulated LPP via the difference between emotional and neutral stimuli (i.e. pleasant-neutral, unpleasant-neutral). We used a difference score approach, which, despite poorer reliability compared to each condition alone, can be useful to isolate neural activity of interest in ERPs (Clayson, Baldwin, & Larson, Reference Clayson, Baldwin and Larson2021). The neutral average was subtracted from each emotional average to create a difference score, producing two different averages for pleasant and unpleasant emotional stimuli. Consistent with previous studies (Foti, Hajcak, & Dien, Reference Foti, Hajcak and Dien2009; Moran, Jendrusina, & Moser, Reference Moran, Jendrusina and Moser2013; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Cuthbert, Bradley, Cacioppo, Ito and Lang2000), the LPP was extracted at a pooling of electrodes Cz, Pz, PO3, and PO4 where the relative difference between emotional and neutral stimuli was greatest across both pleasant and unpleasant stimuli. See online Supplementary materials for LPP psychometric properties and analyses involving neutral alone and all three levels of valence (neutral, pleasant, unpleasant).

Data analysis

Participants were excluded from data analyses if they did not complete the EEG recording (n = 28), had fewer than 50% usable trials (n = 4), or did not complete the CAT-PD (n = 2), resulting in a final sample of 215 participants. All analyses were completed using R Statistical Software (v4.2.1; R Core Team, 2021), R studio (v2022.07.1), and the afex package (Singmann, Bolker, Westfall, Aust, & Ben-Shachar, Reference Singmann, Bolker, Westfall, Aust and Ben-Shachar2023) including functions aov_car and aov_ez. Materials from external sources related to conducting and analyzing data can be found in the References section.

To examine pathological personality dimensions and the emotion-modulated LPP, we used a mixed-measure analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with valence (pleasant-neutral v. unpleasant-neutral) as a within-subject factor and CAT-PD scores as mean centered continuous covariates. CAT-PD scores were examined in three separate ways. First, to examine pathological personality domains consistent with the HiTOP model spectra level, negative emotionality, detachment, psychoticism, antagonism, disinhibition, and anankastia were included as covariates. Second, to examine lower-order maladaptive traits, we conducted a separate ANCOVA for each pathological personality domain in which the maladaptive traits that primarily loaded on the domain based on Ringwald et al. (Reference Ringwald, Emery, Khoo, Clark, Kotelnikova, Scalco and Simms2023a) were included as covariates. Finally, we examined higher-order dimensions that combined the different CAT-PD domains. Based on a recent meta-analysis of the structural evidence supporting the HiTOP model (Ringwald, Forbes, & Wright, Reference Ringwald, Forbes and Wright2023b), we tested multiple higher-order dimensions. The first model examined internalizing (sum of negative emotionality and detachment), psychosis (psychoticism), and externalizing (sum of antagonism, disinhibition, and anankastia) dimensions as continuous covariates. Ringwald et al. (Reference Ringwald, Forbes and Wright2023b) found that detachment also loaded on the psychosis factor, therefore, an analogous model was conducted that included detachment in the psychosis rather than internalizing dimension. The second model included internalizing (sum of negative emotionality, detachment, and psychoticism) and externalizing (sum of antagonism, disinhibition, and anankastia) dimensions as continuous covariates. All domains were z scored prior to creating the higher-order dimensions. For each model, we examined main effects of CAT-PD variables as well as CAT-PD × valence interactions. All continuous covariates were mean centered prior to their inclusion in the ANCOVA models. Finally, sequential regression was used to compare the change in R 2 when adding pathological personality variables to a model using SCID-5 categorical diagnoses to predict the emotion-modulated LPP.

Results

Pathological personality domains and the emotion-modulated LPP

As shown in online Supplementary materials, all CAT-PD domains were positively correlated ranging from weak (e.g. disinhibition and anankastia) to strong (e.g. negative emotionality and psychoticism). Figure 1 displays the emotion-modulated LPP waveforms and scalp distributions to pleasant and unpleasant images (see online Supplementary materials for task effects and waveforms and scalp distributions of the LPP to neutral, pleasant, and unpleasant stimuli). As shown in Table 1 and Fig. 2, analyses of CAT-PD domains and the emotion-modulated LPP indicated main effects of negative emotionality (F (1,208) = 4.02, p = 0.046, η 2 = 0.02) and detachment (F (1,208) = 5.29, p = 0.022, η 2 = 0.02), such that greater negative emotionality was associated with a larger emotion-modulated LPP, while greater detachment was associated with a smaller emotion-modulated LPP. Antagonism, disinhibition, psychoticism, and anankastia domains were not associated with the LPP. See online Supplementary materials for task behavior and picture content analyses.

Figure 1. Emotion-modulated LPP waveforms at a pooling of electrodes Cz, Pz, PO3, and PO4 and scalp distributions for pleasant (pleasant-neutral) and unpleasant (unpleasant-neutral) images. The shaded region of the waveforms shows the segment from 300 to 1000 ms that was used for the scalp distributions. ms, milliseconds.

Table 1. Pathological personality domains and the emotion-modulated late positive potential

ANA, anankastia; ANT, antagonism; DET, detachment; DIS, disinhibition; NEG, negative emotionality; PSY, psychoticism.

Note. This table displays F values, p values, and η 2 effect sizes for pathological personality domain main effects and interactions with valence.

*indicates significance at p <.05.

Figure 2. Scatterplots depict correlations between the emotion-modulated LPP and the negative emotionality residual (left) and detachment residual (right). Below depicts scalp distributions for participants with high (>1 standard deviation from the mean) and low (<1 standard deviation from the mean) negative emotionality and detachment residual scores. LPP, late positive potential.

Maladaptive personality traits and the emotion-modulated LPP

To examine whether the domain results for negative emotionality and detachment were due to lower-order maladaptive traits, similar ANCOVA models were conducted that included either the negative emotionality or detachment traits as continuous covariates. As shown in Table 2, there were no main effects or interactions involving any of the negative emotionality or detachment maladaptive traits (all ps > 0.05). See online Supplementary materials for analyses involving maladaptive traits within psychoticism, antagonism, disinhibition, and anankastia domains.

Table 2. Maladaptive personality traits and the emotion-modulated late positive potential

Note. F and p values for pathological personality traits and interactions with valence within negative emotionality and detachment domains.

Higher-order dimensions and the emotion-modulated LPP

In the first model that examined the higher-order dimensions internalizing (negative emotionality, detachment) and externalizing (antagonism, disinhibition, anankastia), there were no main effects (internalizing: (F (1,211) = 0.31, p = 0.58); externalizing: (F (1,211) = 0.54, p = 0.46)) or interactions with valence (internalizing × valence: F (1,211) = 0.50, p = 0.48; externalizing × valence: (F (1,211) = 1.03, p = 0.32)). When detachment was included with psychosis and not internalizing, there were no main effects (psychosis: (F (1,211) = 2.84, p = 0.09); externalizing (F (1,211) = 0.49, p = 0.48)) or interactions with valence (psychosis × valence: (F (1,211) = 0.01, p = 0.91; externalizing × valence: F (1, 211) = 1.78, p = 0.18)).

In the second model that examined the higher-order dimensions internalizing (negative emotionality, detachment, psychosis) and externalizing (antagonism, disinhibition, anankastia), there were no main effects (internalizing: (F (1,212) = 0.03, p = 0.85); externalizing: (F (1,212) = 1.33, p = 0.25)) or interactions with valence (internalizing × valence: F (1,212) = 0.31, p = 0.58; externalizing × valence: (F (1,212) = 1.60, p = 0.21)).

Pathological personality domains v. SCID-5 diagnoses

To compare the utility of discrete diagnoses of internalizing disorders v. continuous psychopathology dimensions in predicting the emotion-modulated LPP, two blocks of a linear regression model were compared using an analysis of variance. The model included the emotion-modulated LPP, averaged across pleasant and unpleasant images, as the outcome variable. The first block included SCID-5 internalizing diagnoses that have been related to the emotion-modulated LPP as predictors (major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder with agoraphobia, social anxiety disorder, specific phobia). The second block included the same SCID-5 diagnoses in addition to the negative emotionality and detachment pathological personality domains. Compared to the first block with only diagnoses (F (5,202) = 1.44, p = 0.21, adjusted R 2 = 0.01), the addition of pathological personality predictors (F (7,200) = 3.18, p = 0.003, adjusted R 2 = 0.07) significantly improved the prediction of the emotion-modulated LPP (F (2,200) = 7.33, p = 0.001). See online Supplementary materials for the results of each block and the distribution of SCID-5 lifetime diagnoses across the sample.

Discussion

The present study indicated that the pathological personality domains’ negative emotionality and detachment were associated with a larger and smaller, respectively, emotion-modulated LPP. These associations were present irrespective of the stimulus valence (pleasant or unpleasant). Associations between personality pathology and the emotion-modulated LPP were not present at lower- or higher-order levels. Overall, the present study suggests that pathological personality dimensions consistent with the HiTOP model spectra level demonstrate unique associations with neurobiological emotional reactivity. These personality pathology domains improved the prediction of the emotion-modulated LPP compared to the use of categorical diagnoses, further emphasizing the utility of dimensional measures in clinical neuroscience.

The present study provides further clarity on the neurobiological emotional dysfunction that has been observed in purportedly distinct categorical disorders, such as anxiety disorders and depression, which at times show contrasting relationships with the LPP (MacNamara et al., Reference MacNamara, Kotov and Hajcak2016). The present study suggests that the association between many anxiety disorders and a larger emotion-modulated LPP could be due to elevated levels of negative emotionality. As negative emotionality has also been theorized to be a shared factor between anxiety and depression (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991), these findings support the notion found in previous literature (Rozalski & Benning, Reference Rozalski and Benning2019) that a higher-order internalizing factor is associated with an elevated emotion-modulated LPP. In addition to capturing commonalities between disorders, the use of HiTOP-consistent personality domains can shed light on unique aspects of psychopathology within disorders. For example, depression is characterized by both high negative emotionality and low positive emotionality/high detachment (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1991; Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2014). The present study suggests that the blunted emotion-modulated LPP often observed in depression could be the result of high detachment. Overall, these results indicate that the higher-order negative emotionality dimension might explain common relationships in the literature between some internalizing disorders and neurobiological emotional reactivity. At the same time, the results also suggest that the correlated negative emotionality and detachment dimensions can demonstrate discriminant relationships with neurobiological emotional reactivity – which likely contributes to the pervasive presence of heterogeneity within categorical disorders that involve multiple dimensions.

Overall, the CAT-PD domain findings suggest that the emotion-modulated LPP shows distinct relationships with psychopathology at the spectra level of the HiTOP model, as these domains are most consistent with the pathological personality domains used in the current study (Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019). The lack of finding at the trait level suggests that differences in neurobiological emotional reactivity are not driven by one specific trait, but rather, a combination of maladaptive traits that contribute to domain-level pathology. At the same time, the lack of findings at higher-order levels suggests that it may be important to consider distinct aspects of internalizing psychopathology when examining differences in emotional reactivity. As each of the pathological personality domains are implicated in several categorical disorders, their unique relationships with emotional reactivity support the notion that a dimensional framework may be better suited to map psychopathology to neurobiology (Tiego et al., Reference Tiego, Martin, DeYoung, Hagan, Cooper and Pasion2023). This notion is supported by our findings that the use of dimensional psychopathology variables resulted in a significant improvement in the prediction of the emotion-modulated LPP compared to the use of categorical diagnoses. Notably, categorical diagnoses did not show any associations with the emotion-modulated LPP, likely due to their heterogeneity and the high presence of comorbidity in the sample. By utilizing empirically derived, dimensional representations of psychopathology as the foundation for clinical neuroscience studies, researchers can better identify the pattern of dysregulated neurobiological reactivity among individuals with heterogeneous symptoms and comorbid disorders.

The current study did not find psychoticism or externalizing psychopathology to be associated with the emotion-modulated LPP. It should be noted that the current sample was largely characterized by females experiencing internalizing psychopathology, which might contribute to the lack of results for these types of psychopathologies. In addition, these domains might demonstrate associations with the LPP in response to disorder-specific stimuli (e.g. substance-related cues; Kroczek et al., Reference Kroczek, Haeussinger, Hudak, Vanes, Fallgatter and Ehlis2018). Additional research is needed in samples with greater variability in psychoticism and externalizing psychopathology and a wider range of emotional stimuli.

The present study contained multiple strengths. Most studies examining the emotion-modulated LPP did not match pleasant and unpleasant stimuli on arousal levels. Indeed, previous studies often found a larger LPP to unpleasant relative to pleasant images, but unpleasant images were also higher in arousal levels (Weinberg & Hajcak, Reference Weinberg and Hajcak2010). As the current study found associations between psychopathology and the LPP irrespective of valence, it may be important to control for arousal when examining the emotion-modulated LPP. Additionally, the present study oversampled for lifetime psychopathology, providing a more comprehensive assessment of related pathological personality dimensions. The current study also had several limitations; most notably, the gender composition of the sample was majority female (86%), which limits generalizability and investigation of sex-based differences. The sample was also mostly recruited from an area in which 82.8% of people identify as White (U.S. Census Bureau n.d.). While the current study expanded recruitment efforts and successfully increased racial diversity in our sample, certain minority groups are still underrepresented compared to the greater New York City area. In addition, the reliability of anankastia as indicated by the traits primarily loaded on the dimension was low (α = 0.58); however, the true reliability of the dimension was likely higher as domain scores were based on a weighting of all traits. Lastly, the cross-sectional design limits interpretation of the directionality of the findings. Future literature examining longitudinal associations between the emotion-modulated LPP and the development of pathological personality may further elucidate temporal relationships.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001946

Funding statement

This study was funded by Stony Brook University College of Arts & Sciences.

Competing interests

None.

References

American Psychiatric Publishing. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596Google Scholar
Bauer, E. A., Wilson, K. A., Phan, K. L., Shankman, S. A., & MacNamara, A. (2023). A neurobiological profile underlying comorbidity load and prospective increases in dysphoria in a focal fear sample. Biological Psychiatry, 93, 352361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.08.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316336. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayson, P. E., Baldwin, S. A., & Larson, M. J. (2021). Evaluating the internal consistency of subtraction-based and residualized difference scores: Considerations for psychometric reliability analyses of event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 58(4), e13762. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13762CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connell, A. M., Patton, E., & McKillop, H. (2015). Binge drinking, depression, and electrocortical responses to emotional images. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29, 673682. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The five-factor model of personality and its relevance to personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 6, 343359. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1992.6.4.343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology, 52, 95111. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00044-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2015). Structured clinical interview for DSM-5 – research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, Research Version; SCID-5-RV). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Foti, D., Hajcak, G., & Dien, J. (2009). Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: Evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. Psychophysiology, 46, 521530. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00796.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D., Olvet, D. M., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Reduced electrocortical response to threatening faces in major depressive disorder. Depression & Anxiety, 27, 813820. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20712CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468484. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G., & Olvet, D. M. (2008). The persistence of attention to emotion: Brain potentials during and after picture presentation. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 8, 250255. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Zimmerman, M. (2017). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 454477. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Cicero, D. C., Conway, C. C., DeYoung, C. G., … Wright, A. G. C. (2021). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A quantitative nosology based on consensus of evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17, 83108. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-093304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroczek, A. M., Haeussinger, F. B., Hudak, J., Vanes, L. D., Fallgatter, A. J., & Ehlis, A. C. (2018). Cue reactivity essentials: Event-related potentials during identification of visual alcoholic stimuli in social drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 79, 137147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2014). The role of the DSM-5 personality trait model in moving toward a quantitative and empirically based approach to classifying personality and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 477501. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krueger, R. F., Derringer, J., Markon, K. E., Watson, D., & Skodol, A. E. (2012). Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychological Medicine, 42, 18791890. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002674CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8, Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Latzman, R. D., & DeYoung, C. G., & HiTOP Neurobiological Foundations Workgroup (2020). Using empirically-derived dimensional phenotypes to accelerate clinical neuroscience: The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP) framework. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45, 10831085. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0639-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, T. A., Reinhard, E., Sellbom, M., & Anderson, J. L. (2021). An examination of the reliability and validity of the Comprehensive Assessment of Traits Relevant to Personality Disorder-Static Form (CAT-PD-SF). Assessment, 28, 13451357. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191120907957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNamara, A., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2016). Diagnostic and symptom-based predictors of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder: An event-related potential study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 275289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9717-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medina, A. L., Kirilko, E., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2016). Emotional processing and psychopathic traits in male college students: An event-related potential study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 106, 3949. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, T. P., Jendrusina, A. A., & Moser, J. S. (2013). The psychometric properties of the late positive potential during emotion processing and regulation. Brain Research, 1516, 6675. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.04.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, J. S., Huppert, J. D., Duval, E., & Simons, R. F. (2008). Face processing biases in social anxiety: An electrophysiological study. Biological Psychology, 78, 93103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2013). Optimizing efficiency of psychopathology assessment through quantitative modeling: Development of a brief form of the externalizing spectrum inventory. Psychological Assessment, 25, 13321348. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034864CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perkins, E. R., Latzman, R. D., & Patrick, C. J. (2020). Interfacing neural constructs with the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology: ‘Why’ and ‘how’. Personality and Mental Health, 14, 106122. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Ringwald, W. R., Emery, L., Khoo, S., Clark, L. A., Kotelnikova, Y., Scalco, M. D., … Simms, L. J. (2023a). Structure of pathological personality traits through the lens of the CAT-PD model. Assessment, 30, 2296–2295. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911221143343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringwald, W. R., Forbes, M. K., & Wright, A. G. C. (2023b). Meta-analysis of structural evidence for the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP) model. Psychological Medicine, 53, 533546. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001902Google ScholarPubMed
Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 627639. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozalski, V., & Benning, S. D. (2019). Divergences among three higher-order self-report psychopathology factors in normal-range personality and emotional late positive potential reactivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 82, 103861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.103861CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, N., & Verona, E. (2012). Visual complexity attenuates emotional processing in psychopathy: Implications for fear-potentiated startle deficits. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 346360. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-011-0079-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shushakova, A., Ohrmann, P., & Pedersen, A. (2018). Exploring deficient emotion regulation in adult ADHD: Electrophysiological evidence. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 268, 359371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-017-0826-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simms, L. J., Goldberg, L. R., Roberts, J. E., Watson, D., Welte, J., & Rotterman, J. H. (2011). Computerized adaptive assessment of personality disorder: Introducing the CAT-PD project. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 380389. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.577475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., & Ben-Shachar, M. (2023). afex: Analysis of factorial experiments. R package version 1.2-1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afexGoogle Scholar
Speed, B. C., Nelson, B. D., Perlman, G., Klein, D. N., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2015). Personality and emotional processing: A relationship between extraversion and the late positive potential in adolescence. Psychophysiology, 52, 10391047. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiego, J., Martin, E. A., DeYoung, C. G., Hagan, K., Cooper, S. E., Pasion, R., … HiTOP Neurobiological Foundations Work Group (2023). Precision behavioral phenotyping as a strategy for uncovering the biological correlates of psychopathology. Nature Mental Health, 1, 304315. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44220-023-00057-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Census Bureau. (n.d.). Quick Facts. U.S. Department of Commerce. Retrieved April 4, 2024, from https://data.census.gov/Google Scholar
Watson, D., O'Hara, M. W., Naragon-Gainey, K., Koffel, E., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., … Ruggero, C. J. (2012). Development and validation of new anxiety and bipolar symptom scales for an expanded version of the IDAS (the IDAS-II). Assessment, 19, 399420. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191112449857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Beyond good and evil: The time-course of neural activity elicited by specific picture content. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10, 767782. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2011). The late positive potential predicts subsequent interference with target processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 29943007. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2011.21630CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., & Sandre, A. (2018). Distinct associations between low positive affect, panic, and neural responses to reward and threat during late stages of affective picture processing. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3, 5968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.09.013Google ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Perlman, G., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2016). Depression and reduced neural response to emotional images: Distinction from anxiety, and importance of symptom dimensions and age of onset. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 2639. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widiger, T. A., Sellbom, M., Chmielewski, M., Clark, L. A., DeYoung, C. G., Kotov, R., … Wright, A. G. C. (2019). Personality in a hierarchical model of psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 7792. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618797105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, A. G. C., & Simms, L. J. (2014). On the structure of personality disorder traits: Conjoint analyses of the CAT-PD, PID-5, and NEO-PI-3 trait models. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 5, 4354. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, A. G., Thomas, K. M., Hopwood, C. J., Markon, K. E., Pincus, A. L., & Krueger, R. F. (2012). The hierarchical structure of DSM-5 pathological personality traits. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 951957. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027669CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Emotion-modulated LPP waveforms at a pooling of electrodes Cz, Pz, PO3, and PO4 and scalp distributions for pleasant (pleasant-neutral) and unpleasant (unpleasant-neutral) images. The shaded region of the waveforms shows the segment from 300 to 1000 ms that was used for the scalp distributions. ms, milliseconds.

Figure 1

Table 1. Pathological personality domains and the emotion-modulated late positive potential

Figure 2

Figure 2. Scatterplots depict correlations between the emotion-modulated LPP and the negative emotionality residual (left) and detachment residual (right). Below depicts scalp distributions for participants with high (>1 standard deviation from the mean) and low (<1 standard deviation from the mean) negative emotionality and detachment residual scores. LPP, late positive potential.

Figure 3

Table 2. Maladaptive personality traits and the emotion-modulated late positive potential

Supplementary material: File

Barkley et al. supplementary material

Barkley et al. supplementary material
Download Barkley et al. supplementary material(File)
File 153.5 KB