Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T18:37:20.547Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The neuroscience of free will: implications for psychiatry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2013

J. M. Pierre*
Affiliation:
11301 Wilshire Boulevard, Building 210, Room 15, Los Angeles, CA 90073, USA
*
* Address for correspondence: J. M. Pierre, M.D., 11301 Wilshire Boulevard, Building 210, Room 15, Los Angeles, CA 90073, USA. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Belief in free will has been a mainstay in philosophy throughout history, grounded in large part in our intuitive sense that we consciously control our actions and could have done otherwise. However, psychology and psychiatry have long sought to uncover mechanistic explanations for human behavior that challenge the notion of free will. In recent years, neuroscientific discoveries have produced a model of volitional behavior that is at odds with the notion of contra-causal free will and our sense of conscious agency. Volitional behavior instead appears to have antecedents in unconscious brain activity that is localizable to specific neuroanatomical structures. Updating notions of free will in favor of a continuous model of volitional self-control provides a useful paradigm to conceptualize and study some forms of psychopathology such as addiction and impulse control disorders. Similarly, thinking of specific symptoms of schizophrenia as disorders of agency may help to elucidate mechanisms of psychosis. Beyond clinical understanding and etiological research, a neuroscientific model of volitional behavior has the potential to modernize forensic notions of responsibility and criminal punishment in order to inform public policy. Ultimately, moving away from the language of free will towards the language of volitional control may result in an enhanced understanding of the very nature of ourselves.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
This is a work of the U.S. Government and is not subject to copyright protection in the United States.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aharoni, E, Funk, C, Sinnott-Armstrong, W, Gazzaniga, M (2008). Can neurological evidence help courts assess criminal responsibility? Lessons from law and neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124, 11451160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ammon, K, Gandevia, SC (1990). Transcranial magnetic stimulation can influence the selection of motor programmes. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 53, 705707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baler, RD, Volkow, ND (2006). Drug addiction: the neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends in Molecular Medicine 12, 559566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bargh, JA, Chen, M, Burrows, L (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71, 230244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bargh, JA, Schwader, KL, Hailey, SE, Dyer, RL, Boothby, EJ (2012). Automaticity in social–cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Science 16, 593605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R, Masicampo, EJ, DeWall, CN (2009). Prosocial benefits of feeling free: disbelief in free will increases aggression and reduces helpfulness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 35, 260268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience 8, 14581463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S, Smith, J, Steel, R, Johnstone, EC, Frith, CD (2000). The perception of self-produced sensory stimuli in patients with auditory hallucinations and passivity experiences: evidence for a breakdown in self-monitoring. Psychological Medicine 30, 11311139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, SJ, Wolpert, DM, Frith, CD (2002). Abnormalities in awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Science 6, 237242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brasil-Neto, JP, Pascual-Leone, A, Valls-Sole, J, Cohen, LG, Hallett, M (1992). Focal transcranial magnetic stimulation and response bias in a forced-choice task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55, 964966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, RT (2010). Systematic review of the impact of adult drug-treatment courts. Translational Research 155, 263274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, K, Bechara, A (2007). Decision making and free will: a neuroscience perspective. Behavioral Science and the Law 25, 263280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright, T (2011). “To care for him who shall have borne the battle”: the recent development of veterans treatment courts in America. Stanford Law and Policy Review 22, 295316.Google Scholar
Cashmore, AR (2010). The Lucretian swerve: the biological basis of human behavior and the criminal justice system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107, 44994504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Sahakian, BJ (2007). The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 20, 255261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, CD, Garavan, H, Bellgrove, MA (2009). Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33, 631646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, RA, Taylor, JR, Potenza, MN (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 10411052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chappel, JN (1992). Effective use of Alcoholics Anonymous and Narcotics Anonymous in treating patients. Psychiatry Annals 22, 122131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colzato, LS, van den Wildenberg, WPM, Hommel, B (2007). Impaired inhibitory control in recreational cocaine users. PLoS ONE 2, e1143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Committee on Addictions of the Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry (2002). Responsibility and choice in addiction. Psychiatric Services 53, 717–713.Google Scholar
Danquah, AN, Farrell, MJ, O'Boyle, DJ (2008). Biases in the subjective timing of perceptual events: Libet et al. (1983) revisited. Consciousness and Cognition 17, 616627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daprati, E, Franck, N, Georgieff, N, Proust, J, Pacherier, E, Dalery, J, Jeannerod, M (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition 65, 7186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desmurget, M, Reilly, KT, Richard, N, Szathmari, A, Mottolese, C, Sirigu, A (2009). Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 324, 811814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijksterhuis, A, Aarts, H (2010). Goals, attention, and (un) consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology 61, 467490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijksterhuis, A, van Knippenberg, A (1998). The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74, 865877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyen, S, Klein, O, Pichon, C, Cleeremans, A (2012). Behavioral priming: it's all in the mind, but whose mind? PLOS ONE 7, e29081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, BD, Dalglieish, T, Lawrence, AD (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30, 239271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Easdon, C, Izenberg, A, Armilio, ML, Yu, H, Alain, C (2005). Alcohol consumption impairs stimulus- and error-related processing during a go/no-go task. Cognitive Brain Research 25, 873883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eimer, M, Schlaghecken, F (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24, 17371747.Google ScholarPubMed
Felthous, AR (2008). The will: from metaphysical freedom to normative functionalism. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 36, 1624.Google ScholarPubMed
Fillmore, MT, Rush, CR (2002). Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 66, 265273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillmore, MT, Rush, CR, Marczinski, CA (2003). Effects of d-amphetamine on behavioral control in stimulant abusers: the role of prepotent response tendencies. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 71, 143152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontenelle, LF, Mendlowicz, MV, Versiani, M (2009). Volitional disorders: a proposal for DSM-V. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 10, 10161029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, JM, Mathalon, DH (2005). Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: can it explain auditory hallucinations? International Journal of Psychophysiology 58, 179189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franck, N, Farrer, C, Gerogieff, N, Marie-Cardine, M, Dalery, J, d'Amato, T (2001). Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 454459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, I, Katz, A, McCarthy, G, Sass, KJ, Williamson, P, Spencer, SS, Spencer, DD (1991). Functional organization of human supplementary cortex studied by electrical stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience 11, 36563666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, I, Mukamel, R, Kreiman, G (2011). Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron 69, 548562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, CD, Blakemore, S, Wolpert, DM (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B Biological Sciences 355, 17111788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Verdejo, A (2009). A somatic marker theory of addiction. Neuropharmacology 56, 4862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomes, G (1998). The timing of conscious experience: a critical review and reinterpretation of Libet's research. Consciousness and Cognition 7, 559595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, MT (2007). Freedom and resistance: the phenomenal will in addiction. Nursing Philosophy 8, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J, Cohen, J (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B Biological Sciences 359, 17751785.Google ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P (2008). Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 9, 934946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P (2009). The sources of human volition. Science 324, 731733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P, Eimer, M (1999). On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Experimental Brain Research 126, 128133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallett, M (2007). Volitional control of movement: the physiology of free will. Clinical Neurophysiology 118, 11791192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M, Knoblich, G, Repp, BH, Lautenschlager, M, Gallinat, J, Heinz, A, Voss, M (2011). Altered sense of agency in schizophrenia and the putative psychosis prodrome. Psychiatry Research 186, 170176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haynes, J (2011). Decoding and predicting intentions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1224, 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinks-Maldonado, TH, Mathalon, DH, Houde, JF, Gray, M, Faustman, WO, Ford, J (2007). Relationship of imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia to auditory hallucinations. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 286296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, SE (2007). The neurobiology of addiction: implications for voluntary control of behavior. American Journal of Bioethics 7, 811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K, Sanislow, C, Wang, P (2010). Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 748750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanov, I, Schulz, KP, London, ED, Newcorn, JH (2008). Inhibitory control deficits in childhood and risk for substance abuse disorders: a review. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse 34, 239258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jardri, R, Pins, D, Lafargue, G, Very, E, Ameller, A, Delamire, C, Thomas, P (2011). Increased overlap between the brain areas involved in self–other distinction in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 6, e17500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juth, N, Lorentzon, F (2010). The concept of free will and forensic psychiatry. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 33, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klemm, WR (2010). Free will debates: simple experiments are not so simple. Advances in Cognitive Psychology 30, 4765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kranick, SM, Hallett, M (2013). Neurology of volition. Experimental Brain Research 229, 313327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau, HC, Rogers, RD, Haggard, P, Passingham, RE (2004). Attention to intention. Science 303, 12081210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X, Lu, Z, D'Argembeau, A, Ng, M, Bechara, A (2010). The Iowa Gambling Task in fMRI images. Human Brain Mapping 31, 410423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B (1999). Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 4757.Google Scholar
Libet, B (2000). Time factors in conscious processes: reply to Gilberto Gomes. Consciousness and Cognition 9, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B (2002). The timing of mental events: Libet's experimental findings and their implications. Consciousness and Cognition 11, 291299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B (2003). Timing of conscious experience: reply to the 2002 commentaries on Libet's findings. Consciousness and Cognition 12, 321331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B, Gleason, CA, Wright, EW, Pearl, DK (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106, 623642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyvers, M (2000). “Loss of control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 8, 225249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marczinski, CA, Fillmore, MT (2005). Compensating for alcohol-induced impairment of control: effects on inhibition and activation of behavior. Psychopharmacology 181, 337346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsuhashi, M, Hallett, M (2008). The timing of the conscious intention to move. European Journal of Neuroscience 28, 23442351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, JB (2013 a). ‘Irresponsible and a disservice’: the integrity of social psychology turns on the free will dilemma. British Journal of Social Psychology 52, 205218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, JB (2013 b). The integrity of social psychology turns on the free will dilemma: reply to Baumesiter, Vonasch, and Bargh. British Journal of Social Psychology 52, 231237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, FG, Barratt, ES, Dougherty, DM, Schmitz, JM, Swann, AC (2001). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 17831793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monterosso, JR, Aron, AR, Cordova, X, Xu, J, London, ED (2005). Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 79, 273277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, JW, Ruge, D, Wenke, D, Rothwell, J, Haggard, P (2010). Disrupting the experience of control in the human brain: pre-supplementary motor area contributes to the sense of agency. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 277, 25032509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morse, SJ (2007). The non-problem of free will in forensic psychiatry and psychology. Behavioral Science and the Law 25, 203220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nahmias, E (2006). Folk fears about freedom and responsibility: determinism vs. reductionism. Journal of Cognition and Culture 6, 215237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naqvi, NH, Bechara, A (2008). The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends in Neuroscience 32, 5667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, S (2004). The folk psychology of free will: fits and starts. Mind and Language 19, 473502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, S, Knobe, J (2007). Moral responsibility and determinism: the cognitive science of folk intuitions. Nous 41, 663685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, T (1963). Volition: a new experimental approach. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 4, 225230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obhi, SS, Haggard, P (2004). Free will and free won't. American Scientist 92, 358365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penfield, W (1958). Some mechanisms of consciousness discovered during electrical stimulation of the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 44, 5166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, JL, Carroll, ME (2008). The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology 200, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pockett, S (2002). On subjective back-referral and how long it takes to become conscious of a stimulus: a reinterpretation of Libet's data. Consciousness and Cognition 11, 144161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanislow, CA, Pine, DS, Quinn, KJ, Kozak, MJ, Garvey, MA, Heinssen, RK, Wang, PS, Cuthbert, BN (2010). Developing constructs for psychopathology research: research domain criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 199, 631639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sappington, AA (1990). Recent psychological approaches to the free will versus determinism issue. Psychological Bulletin 108, 1929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarkissian, H, Chattterjee, A, de Brigard, F, Knobe, J, Nichols, S, Sirker, S (2010). Is belief in free will a cultural universal? Mind and Language 25, 346358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlaghecken, F, Eimer, M (2004). Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 11, 463468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selimbeyoglu, A, Parvizi, J (2010). Electrical stimulation of the human brain: perceptual and behavioral phenomena reported in the old and new literature. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4, 46.Google ScholarPubMed
Shanks, DR, Newell, BR, Lee, EH, Balakrishnan, , Elelund, L, Cenac, Z, Kavvadia, F, Moore, C (2013). Priming intelligent behavior: an elusive phenomenon. PLOS ONE 8, e56515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shergill, SS, Samson, G, Bays, PM, Frith, CD, Wolpert, DM (2005). Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 23842386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohn, YH, Kaelin-Lang, A, Hallett, M (2003). The effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on movement selection. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 74, 985987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soon, CS, Brass, M, Heinze, H, Haynes, J (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the brain. Nature Neuroscience 11, 543545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sumner, P, Husain, M (2008). At the edge of consciousness: automatic motor activation and voluntary control. Neuroscientist 14, 474486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syznofzik, M, Their, P, Leube, DT, Schlotterbeck, P, Linder, A (2010). Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one's actions. Brain 133, 262271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syznofzik, M, Vosgerau, G, Newen, A (2008). Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Consciousness and Cognition 17, 219239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syznofzik, M, Vosgerau, G, Voss, M (2013). The experience of agency: an interplay between prediction and postdiction. Frontiers in Psychology 4, 127.Google Scholar
Taylor, JL, McCloskey, DI (1996). Selection of motor responses on the basis of unperceived stimuli. Experimental Brain Research 110, 6266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevena, J, Miller, J (2010). Brain preparation before a voluntary action: evidence against unconscious movement initiation. Consciousness and Cognition 19, 447456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevena, JA, Miller, J (2002). Cortical movement preparation before and after a conscious decision to move. Consciousness and Cognition 11, 162190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van de Grind, W (2002). Physical, neural, and mental timing. Consciousness and Cognition 11, 241264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K, Baumeister, RF (2009). Addiction and free will. Addiction Research and Theory 17, 231235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K, Schooler, JW (2008). The value of believing in free will: encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychological Science 19, 4954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voss, M, Moore, J, Hauser, M, Gallinat, J, Heinz, A, Haggard, P (2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: a specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain 133, 31043112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, BN (2004 a). Neglected psychological elements of free will. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 11, 111118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, BN (2004 b). Comparing psychoanalytic and cognitive–behavioral perspectives on control. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 11, 125128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegner, DM (2002). The Illusion of Conscious Will. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegner, DM, Wheatley, T (1999). Apparent mental causation: sources of the experience of will. American Psychology 54, 480492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed