Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T03:45:25.472Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in currently and formerly depressed subjects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 March 2014

K. D. Young*
Affiliation:
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA
P. S. F. Bellgowan
Affiliation:
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA The University of Tulsa, OK, USA
J. Bodurka
Affiliation:
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK, USA
W. C. Drevets
Affiliation:
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA Janssen Pharmaceuticals of Johnson & Johnson, Inc., Titusville, NJ, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: K. D. Young, Ph.D., Laureate Institute for Brain Research, 6655 South Yale Avenue, Tulsa, OK 74136, USA. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) tested in either the depressed (dMDD) or remitted phase (rMDD) recall fewer specific and more categorical autobiographical memories (AMs) compared to healthy controls (HCs). The current study aimed to replicate findings of AM overgenerality in dMDD or rMDD, and to elucidate differences in neurophysiological correlates of AM recall between these MDD samples and HCs.

Method

Unmedicated participants who met criteria for the dMDD, rMDD or HC groups (n = 16/group) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while recalling AMs in response to emotionally valenced cue words. Control tasks involved generating examples from an assigned semantic category and counting the number of risers in a letter string.

Results

The results showed fewer specific and more categorical AMs in both MDD samples versus HCs; dMDDs and rMDDs performed similarly on these measures. The neuroimaging results showed differences between groups in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior insula, inferior temporal gyrus and parahippocampus/hippocampus during specific AM recall versus example generation. During specific AM recall cued by positively valenced words, group differences were evident in the DMPFC, middle temporal gyrus, parahippocampus/hippocampus and occipital gyrus, whereas differences during specific AM recall cued by negatively valenced words were evident in the DMPFC, superior temporal gyrus and hippocampus.

Conclusions

AM deficits exist in rMDDs, suggesting that these impairments constitute trait-like abnormalities in MDD. We also found distinct patterns of hemodynamic activity for each group as they recalled specific AMs, raising the possibility that each group used a partly unique strategy for self-referential focus during successful retrieval of specific memories.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

APA (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. DSM-IV-TR. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Arnold, JF, Fitzgerald, DA, Fernandez, G, Rijpkema, M, Rinck, M, Eling, PA, Becker, ES, Speckens, A, Tendolkar, I (2011). Rose or black-coloured glasses? Altered neural processing of positive events during memory formation is a trait marker of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 131, 214223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnhofer, T, Crane, C, Spinhoven, P, Williams, JM (2007). Failures to retrieve specific memories in previously depressed individuals: random errors or content-related? Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 18591869.Google Scholar
Brittlebank, A, Scott, J, Williams, J, Ferrier, I (1993). Autobiographical memory in depression: state or trait marker? British Journal of Psychiatry 162, 118121.Google Scholar
Bush, G, Luu, P, Posner, MI (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 215222.Google Scholar
Cavanna, AE, Trimble, MR (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crane, C, Barnhofer, T, Mark, J, Williams, G (2007). Cue self-relevance affects autobiographical memory specificity in individuals with a history of major depression. Memory 15, 312323.Google Scholar
Duric, V, Banasr, M, Stockmeier, CA, Simen, AA, Newton, SS, Overholser, JC, Jurjus, GJ, Dieter, L, Duman, RS (2013). Altered expression of synapse and glutamate related genes in post-mortem hippocampus of depressed subjects. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 16, 6982.Google Scholar
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JBW (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York, NY.Google Scholar
Frank, E, Prien, RF, Jarrett, RB, Keller, MB, Kupfer, DJ, Lavori, PW, Rush, AJ, Weissman, MM (1991). Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder. Remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence. Archives of General Psychiatry 48, 851855.Google Scholar
Gibbs, B, Rude, S (2004). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as depression vulnerability. Cognitive Therapy and Research 28, 511526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, M (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 23, 5662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hynes, CA, Baird, AA, Grafton, ST (2006). Differential role of the orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking. Neuropsychologia 44, 374383.Google Scholar
Kalbfleisch, ML, Van Meter, JW, Zeffiro, TA (2007). The influences of task difficulty and response correctness on neural systems supporting fluid reasoning. Cognitive Neurodynamics 1, 7184.Google Scholar
Kanske, P, Heissler, J, Schonfelder, S, Wessa, M (2012). Neural correlates of emotion regulation deficits in remitted depression: the influence of regulation strategy, habitual regulation use, and emotional valence. NeuroImage 61, 686693.Google Scholar
Keedwell, P, Andrew, C, Williams, S, Brammer, M, Phillips, M (2005). A double dissociation of ventromedial prefrontal cortical responses to sad and happy stimuli in depressed and healthy individuals. Biological Psychiatry 58, 495503.Google Scholar
Kennedy, SH, Evans, KR, Kruger, S, Mayberg, HS, Meyer, JH, McCann, S, Arifuzzman, AI, Houle, S, Vaccarino, FJ (2001). Changes in regional brain glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography after paroxetine treatment of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 899905.Google Scholar
Mackinger, H, Pachinger, M, Leibetseder, M, Fartacek, R (2000). Autobiographical memories in women remitted from major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 331334.Google Scholar
McNair, D, Lorr, M, Dropplemen, L (1971). EdiTS Manual: Profile of Mood States. Educational and Industrial Testing Services: San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Milne, AM, MacQueen, GM, Hall, GB (2012). Abnormal hippocampal activation in patients with extensive history of major depression: an fMRI study. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience: JPN 37, 2836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, SA, Asberg, M (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry 134, 382389.Google Scholar
Nandrino, JL, Pezard, L, Poste, A, Reveillere, C, Beaune, D (2002). Autobiographical memory in major depression: a comparison between first-episode and recurrent patients. Psychopathology 35, 335340.Google Scholar
Norbury, R, Mannie, Z, Cowen, PJ (2011). Imaging vulnerability for depression. Molecular Psychiatry 16, 10671068.Google Scholar
Ottowitz, WE, Dougherty, DD, Savage, CR (2002). The neural network basis for abnormalities of attention and executive function in major depressive disorder: implications for application of the medical disease model to psychiatric disorders. Harvard Reviews: Psychiatry 10, 8699.Google Scholar
Peeters, F, Nicolson, NA, Berkhof, J, Delespaul, P, deVries, M (2003). Effects of daily events on mood states in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112, 203211.Google Scholar
Snaith, RP, Hamilton, M, Morley, S, Humayan, A, Hargreaves, D, Trigwell, P (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 167, 99103.Google Scholar
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, RE (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Spinhoven, P, Bockting, CL, Schene, AH, Koeter, MW, Wekking, EM, Williams, JM (2006). Autobiographical memory in the euthymic phase of recurrent depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 590600.Google Scholar
Svoboda, E, McKinnon, M, Levine, B (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 21892208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talairach, J, Tournoux, P (1988). Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System – An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Thieme Medical Publishers: New York, NY.Google Scholar
van der Heiden, L, Scherpiet, S, Konicar, L, Birbaumer, N, Veit, R (2013). Inter-individual differences in successful perspective taking during pain perception mediates emotional responsiveness in self and others: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 65, 387394.Google Scholar
van Tol, MJ, Demenescu, LR, van der Wee, NJ, Kortekaas, R, Marjan, MAN, Boer, JA, Renken, RJ, van Buchem, MA, Zitman, FG, Aleman, A, Veltman, DJ (2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of emotional word encoding and recognition in depression and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychiatry 71, 593602.Google Scholar
van Vreeswijk, M, de Wilde, E (2004). Autobiographical memory specificity, psychopathology, depressed mood and the use of the Autobiographical Memory Test: a meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 42, 731743.Google Scholar
Videbech, P, Ravnkilde, B (2004). Hippocampal volume and depression: a meta-analysis of MRI studies. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 19571966.Google Scholar
Videbech, P, Ravnkilde, B, Pedersen, AR, Egander, A, Landbo, B, Rasmussen, NA, Andersen, F, Stodkilde-Jorgensen, H, Gjedde, A, Rosenberg, R (2001). The Danish PET/depression project: PET findings in patients with major depression. Psychological Medicine 31, 11471158.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) . Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Werner, NS, Meindl, T, Materne, J, Engel, RR, Huber, D, Riedel, M, Reiser, M, Hennig-Fast, K (2009). Functional MRI study of memory-related brain regions in patients with depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 119, 124131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wessel, I, Meeren, M, Peeters, F, Arntz, A, Merckelbach, H (2001). Correlates of autobiographical memory specificity: the role of depression, anxiety and childhood trauma. Behaviour Research and Therapy 39, 409421.Google Scholar
Whalley, MG, Rugg, MD, Brewin, CR (2012). Autobiographical memory in depression: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Research 201, 98106.Google Scholar
Williams, J, Barnhofer, T, Crane, C, Hermans, D, Raes, F, Watkins, E, Dalgleish, T (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin 133, 122148.Google Scholar
Williams, JM, Conway, MA, Cohen, G (2008). Autobiographical Memory. Psychology Press: Hove, UK.Google Scholar
Williams, JM, Dritschel, BH (1988). Emotional disturbance and the specificity of autobiographical memory. Cognition and Emotion 2, 221234.Google Scholar
Williams, JM, Ellis, NC, Tyers, C, Healy, H, Rose, G, MacLeod, AK (1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory and Cognition 24, 116125.Google Scholar
Young, KD, Bellgowan, PS, Bodurka, J, Drevets, WC (2013 a). Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in patients with depression and individuals at high risk for depression. Journal of the American Medical Association: Psychiatry 70, 698708.Google Scholar
Young, KD, Bellgowan, PS, Bodurka, J, Drevets, WC (2013 b). Functional neuroimaging of sex differences in autobiographical memory recall. Human Brain Mapping 34, 33303332.Google Scholar
Young, KD, Erickson, K, Nugent, AC, Fromm, SJ, Mallinger, AG, Furey, ML, Drevets, WC (2012). Functional anatomy of autobiographical memory recall deficits in depression. Psychological Medicine 42, 345357.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Supplementary Material

Supplementary information supplied by authors.

Download Supplementary Material(File)
File 307.2 KB