Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T20:33:45.008Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The nature of anhedonia and avolition in patients with first-episode schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2015

S. S. Y. Lui
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China Castle Peak Hospital, Hong Kong Special Administration Region, People's Republic of China
A. C. Y. Liu
Affiliation:
Castle Peak Hospital, Hong Kong Special Administration Region, People's Republic of China
W. W. H. Chui
Affiliation:
Castle Peak Hospital, Hong Kong Special Administration Region, People's Republic of China
Z. Li
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
F. Geng
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Y. Wang
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
E. A. Heerey
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
E. F. C. Cheung
Affiliation:
Castle Peak Hospital, Hong Kong Special Administration Region, People's Republic of China
R. C. K. Chan*
Affiliation:
Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, People's Republic of China
*
* Address for correspondence: R. C. K. Chan, Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 16 Lincui Road, Beijing 100101, People's Republic of China. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Patients with schizophrenia have intact ability to experience emotion, but empirical evidence suggests that they fail to translate emotional salience into effortful behaviour. Previous research in patients with chronic schizophrenia suggests that working memory is important in integrating emotion and behaviour. This study aimed to examine avolition and anhedonia in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and clarify the role of working memory in emotion–behaviour coupling.

Method

We recruited 72 participants with first-episode schizophrenia and 61 healthy controls, and used a validated emotion-inducing behavioural paradigm to measure participants' affective experiences and how experienced emotion coupled with behaviour. Participants were given the opportunity to expend effort to increase or decrease their exposure to emotion-inducing photographs. Participants with schizophrenia having poor working memory were compared with those with intact working memory in their liking and emotion–behaviour coupling.

Results

Patients with first-episode schizophrenia experienced intact ‘in-the-moment’ emotion, but their emotion was less predictive of the effort expended, compared with controls. The emotion–behaviour coupling was significantly weaker in patients with schizophrenia with poor working memory than in those with intact working memory. However, compared with controls, patients with intact working also showed substantial emotion–behaviour decoupling.

Conclusions

Our findings provide strong evidence for emotion–behaviour decoupling in first-episode schizophrenia. Although working memory deficits contribute to defective translation of liking into effortful behaviour, schizophrenia alone affects emotion–behaviour coupling.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreasen, NC (1989). The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): conceptual and theoretical foundations. British Journal of Psychiatry 155, 4958.Google Scholar
Baddeley, AD (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 829839.Google Scholar
Berridge, KC (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain Cognition 52, 106128.Google Scholar
Berridge, KC (2007). The debate over dopamine's role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology 191, 391431.Google Scholar
Cohen, AS, Minor, KS (2010). Emotional experience in patients with schizophrenia revisited: meta-analysis of laboratory studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 143150.Google Scholar
Fervaha, G, Foussias, G, Agid, O, Remington, G (2013). Neural substrates underlying effort computation in schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37, 26492665.Google Scholar
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JBW (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I) (User's Guide and Interview) Research Version. Biometrics Research Institute, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Frank, MJ, Claus, ED (2006). Anatomy of a decision: striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychological Review 113, 300326.Google Scholar
Gard, DE, Sanchez, AH, Cooper, K, Fisher, M, Garrett, C, Vinogradov, S (2014). Do patients with schizophrenia have difficulty anticipating pleasure, engaging in effortful behavior, or both? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 123, 771782.Google Scholar
Gold, JM, Carpenter, C, Randolph, C, Goldberg, TE, Weinberger, DR (1997). Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Archives in General Psychiatry 54, 159165.Google Scholar
Gold, JM, Hanh, B, Zhang, W, Robinson, BM, Kappenman, ES, Beck, VM, Luck, SL (2010). Reduced capacity but spared precision and maintenance of working memory representations in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 570577.Google Scholar
Gold, JM, Strauss, GP, Waltz, JA, Robinson, BM, Brown, JK, Frank, MJ (2013). Negative symptoms of schizophrenia are associated with abnormal effort–cost computations. Biological Psychiatry 74, 130136.Google Scholar
Gong, YX (1992). Manual of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Chinese Version. Chinese Map Press: Changsha.Google Scholar
Heerey, EA, Bell-Warren, KR, Gold, JM (2008). Decision-making impairments in the context of intact reward sensitivity in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 64, 6269.Google Scholar
Heerey, EA, Gold, JM (2007). Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate dissociation between affective experience and motivated behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 268278.Google Scholar
Kapur, S, Mizrahi, R, Li, M (2005). From dopamine to salience to psychosis – linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 79, 5968.Google Scholar
Kay, SR, Fiszbein, A, Opler, LA (1987). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 261276.Google Scholar
Kring, AM, Barch, MB (2014). The motivation and pleasure dimension of negative symptoms: neural substrates and behavioral outputs. European Neuropsychopharmacology 24, 725736.Google Scholar
Lang, PJ, Bradley, MM, Cuthbert, BN (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Digitized Photographs, Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. University of Florida: Gainesville, FL.Google Scholar
Lee, J, Park, S (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 599–611.Google Scholar
Llerena, K, Strauss, GP, Cohen, AS (2012). Looking at the other side of the coin: a meta-analysis of self-reported emotional arousal in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 142, 6570.Google Scholar
Lui, SSY, Sham, PC, Chan, RCK, Cheung, EFC (2011). A family study of endophenotypes for psychosis within an early intervention programme in Hong Kong: rationale and preliminary findings. Chinese Science Bulletin 56, 33943397.Google Scholar
Strauss, GP, Frank, MJ, Waltz, JA, Kasanova, Z, Herbener, ES, Gold, JM (2011 a). Deficits in positive reinforcement learning and uncertainty-driven exploration are associated with distinct aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 69, 424431.Google Scholar
Strauss, GP, Robinson, BM, Waltz, JA, Frank, MJ, Kasanova, Z, Herbener, ES, Gold, JM (2011 b). Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate inconsistent preference judgments for affective and nonaffective stimuli. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 12951304.Google Scholar
Trémeau, F, Antonius, D, Cacioppo, JT, Ziwich, R, Butler, P, Malaspina, D, Javitt, DC (2010). Anticipated, on-line and remembered positive experience in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 122, 199205.Google Scholar
Wallis, JD (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30, 3156.Google Scholar