Introduction
Acquisition and extinction of threat contingencies are key learning mechanisms implicated in the onset and maintenance of anxiety disorders (Mineka & Oehlberg, Reference Mineka and Oehlberg2008; Pittig, Treanor, LeBeau, & Craske, Reference Pittig, Treanor, LeBeau and Craske2018). Specifically, influential developmental theories of anxiety link heightened risk for its emergence during adolescence to age-dependent maturational changes in threat learning neural circuitry (Casey, Glatt, & Lee, Reference Casey, Glatt and Lee2015; Jovanovic, Nylocks, & Gamwell, Reference Jovanovic, Nylocks and Gamwell2013; Kitt, Odriozola, & Gee, Reference Kitt, Odriozola and Gee2023). Nevertheless, despite the potential utility of such neurobiological theories for clinical research on anxiety etiology and treatment, very limited empirical research examines them. To begin bridging this gap, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to test whether adolescents, relative to adults, indeed show distinct, age-dependent patterns of neural responses to learned threats.
Anxiety symptoms typically emerge during adolescence (Beesdo, Knappe, & Pine, Reference Beesdo, Knappe and Pine2009; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005) and reflect a persistent tendency toward excessive, impairing defensive responses to potential threats (American Psychiatric Association, 2022). If not treated early, anxiety often becomes chronic, incurs considerable personal and societal costs, and confers increased risk for additional psychopathology. Despite high prevalence and considerable negative outcomes, current first-line treatments for anxiety, like cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), often show limited efficacy particularly among youth (Rapee, Creswell, Kendall, Pine, & Waters, Reference Rapee, Creswell, Kendall, Pine and Waters2023). While these treatments are effective in reducing anxiety, compared to no-treatment or waitlist conditions (James, Reardon, Soler, James, & Creswell, Reference James, Reardon, Soler, James and Creswell2020), they do not consistently result in substantial improvements for many adolescents. Incomplete understanding of the mechanisms giving rise to anxiety symptoms impedes research on improving therapeutic approaches.
Prominent developmental theories of anxiety link the emergence of anxiety to age-dependent perturbations in threat learning processes, encompassing the acquisition and extinction of threat contingencies. These theories posit that adolescence is characterized by sensitivity to variations in threat learning, increasing the risk of developing anxiety disorders. Specifically, adolescents tend to overgeneralize learned threat to safety cues, as indicated by self-reports, physiological responses, and avoidance behaviors (Klein, Berger, Vervliet, & Shechner, Reference Klein, Berger, Vervliet and Shechner2021; Skversky-Blocq, Pine, & Shechner, Reference Skversky-Blocq, Pine and Shechner2021; Skversky-Blocq, Shmuel, Waters, & Shechner, Reference Skversky-Blocq, Shmuel, Waters and Shechner2022b). Less effective differentiation between learned threat and safety cues has also been demonstrated in attention allocation during direct (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Berger, Vervliet and Shechner2021) and observational threat learning paradigms (Skversky-Blocq, Shmuel, Cohen, & Shechner, Reference Skversky-Blocq, Shmuel, Cohen and Shechner2022a). More broadly, physiological arousal to both threat and safety-acquired cues is higher among adolescents than adults (Linton & Levita, Reference Linton and Levita2021). Extinction of threat contingencies likewise appears to be age-specific, with studies reporting attenuated extinction learning among adolescents compared to adults and children, indicated by persistent physiological responding (Pattwell et al., Reference Pattwell, Duhoux, Hartley, Johnson, Jing, Elliott and Lee2012) and negative valence attributed to conditioned threat cues (Waters, Theresiana, Neumann, & Craske, Reference Waters, Theresiana, Neumann and Craske2017). Moreover, in a study on anxiety patients and healthy controls, anxiety was associated with sustained physiological fear responses during extinction in youth but not adults (Abend et al., Reference Abend, Burk, Ruiz, Gold, Napoli, Britton and Averbeck2022). Together, the findings across multiple levels of measurement support theories that adolescence is associated with distinct threat learning patterns, which may contribute to heightened sensitivity to developing anxiety symptomatology.
Neurobiological theories further link the substantial maturation processes the neural circuitry involved in threat learning undergoes during early childhood and adolescence to anxiety vulnerability (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Glatt and Lee2015; Jovanovic et al., Reference Jovanovic, Nylocks and Gamwell2013; Kitt et al., Reference Kitt, Odriozola and Gee2023). Specifically, variations in maturation of key threat circuitry nodes, such as the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and pathways between them, are posited to influence threat acquisition and extinction processes, rendering adolescents more susceptible to maintenance of acquired fear memories. A few functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have examined developmental differences in neural responding during extinction processes (Ganella, Drummond, Ganella, Whittle, & Kim, Reference Ganella, Drummond, Ganella, Whittle and Kim2018; Morriss, Christakou, & van Reekum, Reference Morriss, Christakou and van Reekum2019; Pattwell et al., Reference Pattwell, Duhoux, Hartley, Johnson, Jing, Elliott and Lee2012). Findings reveal that compared to adults, adolescents show reduced extinction and impaired extinction recall, linked to altered vmPFC activation. Comparing anxiety patients and healthy controls in youth and adult samples, Gold et al. (Reference Gold, Abend, Britton, Behrens, Farber, Ronkin and Pine2020) found youth exhibited distinct, anxiety-dependent amygdala-vmPFC connectivity during extinction recall, and also differed from adults in neural activation patterns when rating fear of threat cues and assessing memory of threat contingencies. Moreover, structural MRI work finds that volume of the amygdala and additional subcortical structures moderates associations between extinction learning and anxiety severity in adults but not youths (Abend et al., Reference Abend, Burk, Ruiz, Gold, Napoli, Britton and Averbeck2022).
EEG and ERP research offers several important, complementary advantages to fMRI research. While the MRI environment itself might induce an increase in state anxiety (Mutschler et al., Reference Mutschler, Wieckhorst, Meyer, Schweizer, Klarhöfer, Wilhelm and Ball2014), measuring EEG is generally less fear-inducing. Moreover, ERP offers high temporal resolution which may uncover specific aspects of the conditioned threat response. One ERP component in which threat learning may manifest is the late positive potential (LPP), observed ~300–400 milliseconds following cue onset and lasting several hundred milliseconds, with a maximal parietal topography (Danon-Kraun et al., Reference Danon-Kraun, Horovitz, Frenkel, Richter-Levin, Pine and Shechner2021; Dennis & Hajcak, Reference Dennis and Hajcak2009; Weinberg & Hajcak, Reference Weinberg and Hajcak2011). LPP is believed to index the emotional significance of stimuli (Hajcak & Foti, Reference Hajcak and Foti2020; Schupp & Kirmse, Reference Schupp and Kirmse2021). Indeed, fMRI work on the processing of emotional stimuli links neural activation in ‘limbic’ circuitry nodes, such as the amygdala and insula, to LPP amplitude in youth (Bunford, Kujawa, Fitzgerald, Monk, & Phan, Reference Bunford, Kujawa, Fitzgerald, Monk and Phan2018) and adults (Liu, Huang, McGinnis-Deweese, Keil, & Ding, Reference Liu, Huang, McGinnis-Deweese, Keil and Ding2012; MacNamara, Rabinak, Kennedy, & Phan, Reference MacNamara, Rabinak, Kennedy and Phan2018). LPP could therefore provide a more direct way to track the brain's responses to threats as these change during acquisition and extinction of threat contingencies.
Previous research establishes the relevance of LPP for indexing threat learning in adults, with increased LPP amplitude in response to conditioned threat (CS+) relative to safety (CS−) cues during acquisition, and a gradual decrease in amplitude over extinction trials (Bacigalupo & Luck, Reference Bacigalupo and Luck2018; Cheng, Jackson, & MacNamara, Reference Cheng, Jackson and MacNamara2022; Ferreira de Sá, Michael, Wilhelm, & Peyk, Reference Ferreira de Sá, Michael, Wilhelm and Peyk2019; Paiva et al., Reference Paiva, Almeida, Coelho, Pasion, Barbosa, Ferreira-Santos and Marques-Teixeira2020; Panitz, Hermann, & Mueller, Reference Panitz, Hermann and Mueller2015; Seligowski et al., Reference Seligowski, Bondy, Singleton, Orcutt, Ressler and Auerbach2018, Reference Seligowski, Reffi, Phillips, Orcutt, Auerbach, Pizzagalli and Ressler2021; Sperl, Wroblewski, Mueller, Straube, & Mueller, Reference Sperl, Wroblewski, Mueller, Straube and Mueller2021). Importantly, LPP responses during extinction were shown to diminish more slowly than corresponding psychophysiological responses, suggesting LPP may be less susceptible to simple habituation effects (Bacigalupo & Luck, Reference Bacigalupo and Luck2018; Sperl et al., Reference Sperl, Wroblewski, Mueller, Straube and Mueller2021). Because LPP may show less habituation than psychophysiological readouts, it may serve as a more direct and accurate index of neural responses to threat contingencies (Bacigalupo & Luck, Reference Bacigalupo and Luck2018).
Only two studies to date have assessed LPP in youth during a classical learning task. One assessed a community sample of adolescents and found differential LPP responses towards the conditioned stimulus (CS) during an extinction recall task, a week following extinction, suggesting it as a promising index of return of fear (Danon-Kraun et al., Reference Danon-Kraun, Horovitz, Frenkel, Richter-Levin, Pine and Shechner2021). The second study revealed enhanced differential LPP among anxious compared to non-anxious adolescents, one week after extinction, underlining LPP's ability to reveal anxiety-related differences in extinction learning (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Shner-livne, Danon-kraun, Ginat-frolich, Pine and Shechner2023). However, no study to date has directly compared youth and adult samples in terms of LPP during threat acquisition and extinction, thus limiting the ability to make strong inferences on adolescence-specific effects.
Here, we examined threat learning processes in typically developing adolescents and adults, measured during both threat acquisition and extinction and indicated by LPP, skin conductance response (SCR), and self-reported fear. By extending the extinction phase, as well as including a 24-h interval between acquisition and extinction to allow consolidation of learning, our study is uniquely positioned to identify subtle differences in the slower process of threat extinction assessed in the lab (Treanor, Rosenberg, & Craske, Reference Treanor, Rosenberg and Craske2021). Identifying neural markers of developmental differences in threat learning and, critically, extinction processes implicated in the pathogenesis of anxiety disorders – could be pivotal in enhancing clinical approaches to anxiety, informing both the timing and method of intervention (Pittig et al., Reference Pittig, Treanor, LeBeau and Craske2018).
We had three a-priori preregistered hypotheses (https://osf.io/y79s2). First, we hypothesized that while both age groups (adolescents, adults) would exhibit successful differential threat acquisition and a reduction in these responses during extinction in all measures, adolescents would demonstrate a slower extinction than adults, as indicated by differential LPP. Second, we hypothesized that adolescents would display overall higher psychophysiological arousal than adults, evidenced by increased SCR during acquisition and extinction (Abend et al., Reference Abend, Gold, Britton, Michalska, Shechner, Sachs and Pine2020; Linton & Levita, Reference Linton and Levita2021). Third, we hypothesized that trait anxiety levels would be associated with increased differential threat acquisition and impaired extinction in both age groups (Klein, Abend, Shmuel, & Shechner, Reference Klein, Abend, Shmuel and Shechner2022; Klein, Shner, Ginat-Frolich, Vervliet, & Shechner, Reference Klein, Shner, Ginat-Frolich, Vervliet and Shechner2020). Of note, we preregistered six hypotheses; for clarity, hypotheses 1,2,3, and 5 in the preregistration were combined to form the first hypothesis presented here.
In addition to our a-priori hypotheses, we conducted several exploratory analyses. Building on previous research indicating extinction impairments among anxious youth (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Shner-livne, Danon-kraun, Ginat-frolich, Pine and Shechner2023), we explored potential interactions among age, anxiety levels, and extinction during the later stages of learning. We also investigated potential associations between differential threat learning manifesting in LPP and differential threat responses manifesting in psychophysiological and self-report indices.
Method
Preregistration
The study hypotheses, sample size design, procedure, and data analysis plan were preregistered on the Open Science Framework prior to data collection (https://osf.io/y79s2).
Participants
Participants included 65 adults (M age = 24.83 years, s.d. = 3.45, range:18.08–34.62 years; 55.4% females) and 63 adolescents (M = 15.03 years, s.d. = 1.65, range:12.19–17.91 years; 57.1% females). Of these, three participants (one adult and two adolescents) aborted during threat acquisition, and nine (five adults and four adolescents) did not return to the lab for a second visit. Overall, 59 adults and 57 adolescents completed both visits (for complete demographic and clinical characteristics, see online Supplementary Table S1). Participants were recruited through online advertisements and received modest compensation. Before the experiment, adult participants and legal guardians of the adolescent participants signed a consent form, and adolescents signed assent forms.
No significant differences emerged between adults and adolescents in gender, anxiety symptom severity (SCARED/SCARRED), or sleep duration or quality between visits, all ps > 0.075. Adolescents reported greater intolerance of uncertainty (Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale, IUS) than adults, t(124) = −2.288, p = 0.024 (see online Supplementary Table S1). Information on pubertal development of the adolescent sample is presented in the online Supplemental Materials.
Procedure
The study consisted of two experimental lab visits. On visit 1, participants completed the habituation and differential threat acquisition phases of the bell threat learning task (Shechner et al., Reference Shechner, Britton, Ronkin, Jarcho, Mash, Michalska and Pine2015). On the next day, participants returned for a second visit and underwent threat extinction followed by self-report questionnaires. All experimental phases included electroencephalogram (EEG) and SCR measurements. All procedures received IRB approval (316/21).
Threat learning task
The habituation phase consisted of eight trials presenting a grey-colored animated bell, with each trial lasting seven seconds; see online Supplementary Fig. S1. Next, participants underwent three blocks of threat acquisition, each comprised of 10 presentations of the CS+ and 10 presentations of the CS−, in pseudorandomized order. Blue and yellow bells served as the CSs, counterbalanced as CS+ and CS−. Each CS was presented for seven seconds in each trial. In 60% of the trials, immediately following the CS+ (i.e. at the end of the 7th second), a 1-second-long unconditioned stimulus (US) consisting of a picture of the same-colored bell ringing paired with a loud alarm sound at 95 dB was presented. Trials (across all experimental phases) were separated by an inter-trial interval (ITI), in which participants observed a white fixation cross on a black background, varying randomly in length between 10 and 12 s. Following the three threat acquisition blocks, participants rated their unpleasantness when encountering the US using a Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all unpleasant) to 10 (extremely unpleasant). The acquisition phase lasted ~20–25 min.
Because extinction learning tends to occur more slowly than threat acquisition, the extinction phase was designed to be twice as long as the acquisition phase (Treanor et al., Reference Treanor, Rosenberg and Craske2021). Immediately prior to the start of the extinction phase, we presented the CS+ paired with the US as a reminder of the previously learned association. Thereafter, participants completed six consecutive blocks of extinction, each consisting of 10 CS+ and 10 CS− presentations using a procedure similar to that of acquisition, but without US delivery at any point (online Supplementary Fig. S1a, S1b in Supplemental Materials). The extinction phase lasted ~40–45 min. Participants were not explicitly informed about the CS + -US contingency at any stage of the learning task.
Measures
Self-report questionnaires
Fear ratings. Fear levels (‘How afraid are you of this bell?’) were assessed for each CS separately, prior to acquisition (‘pre-acquisition’, i.e. before the presentation of any stimulus) and extinction (‘pre-extinction’, i.e. before the reminder trials preceding the extinction phase) and following each block, using a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all afraid) to 10 (extremely afraid). Threat expectancy levels (‘How likely is it for this bell to ring?’) were also assessed and are reported in online Supplemental Materials.
Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED). The SCARED questionnaire is a commonly used self-report measure assessing anxiety symptoms in youth (Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997). It consists of 41 items assessing anxiety symptoms, each rated on a 3-point Likert scale (0–2), with total anxiety scores ranging 0–82. Internal consistency in our study was α = 0.91. Total scores were converted into Z scores in the adolescent sample for comparability with the adult sample (Abend et al., Reference Abend, Gold, Britton, Michalska, Shechner, Sachs and Pine2020).
Screen for Adult Anxiety Related Disorders (SCAARED). The SCAARED questionnaire is an adaptation of the SCARED questionnaire to assess anxiety symptoms in adults (Angulo et al., Reference Angulo, Rooks, Gill, Goldstein, Sakolsky, Goldstein and Birmaher2017). It features 44 items rated similarly to the SCARED, with total scores ranging 0–88. Internal consistency in our study was α = 0.94. Total scores were likewise converted into Z scores in the adult sample.
Duration and Quality of Sleep between Visits. Given that acquisition and extinction were conducted on separate days, and night sleep is critical for consolidation of learning (Treanor et al., Reference Treanor, Rosenberg and Craske2021), participants reported sleep duration between visits 1 and 2 and ranked their subjective quality of sleep on a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (very bad) to 3 (very good).
Skin conductance response (SCR)
Skin conductance was recorded continuously at 500 Hz using an 8-slot Bionex system (Mindware Technologies Ltd., OH, USA) and two isotonic gel electrodes placed on participants' left palm (i.e. hypothenar and thenar muscles).
We used MindWare data analysis software (Version 3.0.25). Responses to the CSs were calculated on a trial-by-trial basis as the difference between trough-to-peak amplitude, between 500 ms and 7 s following CS onset. SCR threshold was set at 0.01; scores <0.01, or negative changes, were counted as zero. A square-root transformation was then applied to normalize SCR scores (Lonsdorf et al., Reference Lonsdorf, Klingelhöfer-Jens, Andreatta, Beckers, Chalkia, Gerlicher and Merz2019). Finally, SCR data were averaged per stimulus type (CS+, CS−), block, and phase (acquisition, extinction). Temperature in the experiment room was kept consistent across participants (20°–22° Celsius).
Electroencephalogram (EEG)
EEG was recorded using a g.Nautilus RESEARCH 32 g.SCARABEO (g.tec, medical engineering GmbH, Austria) wearable headset with 32 Ag/AgCl electrodes. Electrodes were placed according to the standard 10–20 international system. All EEG data processing and analysis were performed in MATLAB (v. R2022a) using the toolboxes for EEGLAB version 2022.1 (Delorme & Makeig, Reference Delorme and Makeig2004) and ERPLAB version 9.00 (Lopez-Calderon & Luck, Reference Lopez-Calderon and Luck2014). For more information on EEG preprocessing, see online Supplemental Materials.
Late positive potential (LPP). ERP segments were averaged separately per stimulus type (CS+, CS−), experimental block (1–6), and phase (acquisition/extinction). In line with prior work, LPP was extracted in a time window of [400, 1000 ms] following stimulus onset in a parietal site (Pz) (Danon-Kraun et al., Reference Danon-Kraun, Horovitz, Frenkel, Richter-Levin, Pine and Shechner2021; Dennis & Hajcak, Reference Dennis and Hajcak2009; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Shner-livne, Danon-kraun, Ginat-frolich, Pine and Shechner2023; Weinberg & Hajcak, Reference Weinberg and Hajcak2011). To ensure enough trials to calculate reliable LPP, we used the averaged LPP score in all three acquisition blocks and the averaged LPP score for blocks (1,2) (3,4) and (5,6) extinction in our statistical analyses.
Statistical analysis
All analyses were preregistered (https://osf.io/y79s2). Our primary analyses focused on developmental differences in differential threat acquisition and extinction in all measures. For the acquisition phase and the extinction phase, we used repeated-measures analyses of covariance (RM-ANCOVAs) of LPP, SCR, and self-reported fear. Stimulus (CS+, CS) and block were within-subject factors, age group (adolescents, adults) was a between-subjects factor, and anxiety level (Z scores of SCARED/SCAARED) was entered as a continuous covariate. In follow-up analyses, to facilitate interpretation, we divided participants into low- and high-anxiety groups based on a median split in each age group (adults, adolescents). Greenhouse–Geisser or Huynh-Feldt corrections were applied as needed.
In additional analyses, we examined the moderating effect of age on the association between anxiety levels (SCARED/SCAARED Z scores) and magnitude of extinction learning (differences between CS+ and CS− at final stages of extinction), using Model 1 in the SPSS Hayes macro-PROCESS (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2004).
Finally, we used Pearson's correlations to explore associations among LPP, SCR, self-reported fear, and threat expectancy, anxiety levels, and age.
Results
Threat acquisition
Self-reported fear
RM-ANCOVA of reported fear towards the conditioned cues with Stimulus (CS+, CS−) × Block (pre-acquisition, block 1, block 2, block 3) × Age (adolescents, adults) × Anxiety levels (continuous), yielded a three-way interaction of Stimulus × Block × Anxiety level, F (2.208, 262.806) = 5.986, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.048. Follow-up analysis indicated that a two-way interaction of Stimulus × Block emerged in both high- and low-anxiety groups, F (2.716, 179.229)= 94.071, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.588, and F (1.621, 87.539) = 36.114, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.401, respectively. To further examine self-reported fear of the CSs, we corrected for four multiple comparisons in each group (high- and low-anxiety levels) according to pre-acquisition and three acquisition blocks (Bonferroni-corrected for four tests; corrected alpha = 0.013). In line with our first hypothesis, both groups reported comparable fear of the CS+ and CS− in pre-acquisition, but differential fear (CS+ > CS−) was evident during the three subsequent blocks of threat acquisition (all ps < 0.001), indicating successful threat learning (see Fig. 1a). Further analyses showed that in line with our third hypothesis, individuals with high-anxiety symptoms reported more fear of the CS+ than those with low-anxiety symptoms following the first, second, and third block of acquisition, t(123) = −3.79, p < 0.001, t(123) = −4.36, p < 0.001, and t(120) = −2.77, p < 0.001, respectively, but not in pre-acquisition t(118.56) = −1.99, p = 0.049. No differences in self-reported fear of the CS− emerged during pre-acquisition or across acquisition; all ps ⩾ 0.173.
A main effect of anxiety also emerged, F (1,119) = 12.43, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.095, with follow-up analyses indicating that highly anxious participants reported overall higher levels of fear towards conditioned cues across the acquisition phase (M = 3.44, s.d. = 1.27) than those with low self-reported anxiety (M = 2.65, s.d. = 1.27). Additional lower-order effects are reported in online Supplementary Table S4 in the supplemental materials.
Skin conductance response (SCR)
RM-ANCOVA of SCR with Stimulus (CS+, CS−) × Block (block 1, block 2, block 3) × Age (adolescents, adults) × Anxiety (continuous) yielded a two-way interaction of Stimulus × Age, F (1,107) = 6.02, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.053. In line with our first hypothesis, both adults, F (1,62) = 48.62, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.440, and adolescents, F (1,57) = 105.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.648, displayed successful threat acquisition (main effect of CS, CS+ > CS−). The source of this two-way interaction is likely derived from the stronger differential acquisition effect among adolescents than adults (see Fig. 1b; for a detailed presentation of developmental difference in SCR across acquisition blocks, see online Supplementary Fig. S3 in Supplemental Materials). In line with our second hypothesis, a main effect of age emerged, F (1,107) = 6.76, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.059, whereby adolescents exhibited overall greater SCR across the acquisition phase (M = 0.34, s.d. = 0.20) than adults (M = 0.24, s.d. = 0.20).
We noted a two-way interaction of Stimulus × Block, F (1.83, 195.06) = 23.99, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.183, suggesting a gradual decrease in the difference between the CSs across acquisition due to habituation. However, follow-up analyses indicated that differential SCR remained significant during the first, t(122) = 12.57, p < 0.001, second, t(121) = 7.73, p < 0.001, and third, t(112) = 7.82, p < 0.001, blocks of acquisition. Additional lower-order effects are reported in online Supplementary Table S4 in the supplemental materials.
Additional analyses of SCR to the US indicated that adults exhibited greater habituation than adolescents across acquisition blocks (online Supplementary Fig. S4, Supplemental Materials).
Late positive potential (LPP)
LPP temporal dynamics and topographic activation across the scalp are presented in Fig. 2. The RM-ANCOVA of LPP mean amplitude testing the Stimulus × Age × Anxiety effect, yielded a main effect of Stimulus, F (1,95) = 27.30, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.223. In line with the first hypothesis, participants exhibited enhanced LPP response to the CS+ (M = 5.55, s.d. = 2.49) compared to the CS− (M = 4.41, s.d. = 2.63), suggesting threat acquisition manifests in this component. Further, a main effect of Age emerged, F (1,95) = 35.95, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.275, suggesting greater LPP amplitude across CSs in adolescents (M = 6.38, s.d. = 2.31) relative to adults (M = 3.57, s.d. = 2.32); see Fig. 2. Additional analyses on the effects of age are presented in the online Supplemental Materials. No other effects were observed.
To summarize, in line with our first hypothesis, the results indicated successful threat acquisition across self-reported fear, SCR, and LPP in adolescents and adults. Notably, developmental differences emerged at psychophysiological and neural levels. Supporting our second hypothesis, adolescents displayed heightened SCR responses to both threat and safety cues, but contrary to our hypothesis, they also demonstrated stronger threat differentiation in SCR. Additionally, adolescents showed heightened LPP responses to both threat and safety cues. Finally, supporting our third hypothesis, individuals with higher anxiety levels exhibited greater differential self-reported fear during acquisition irrespective of their age group. However, anxiety moderation was not observed in other threat-related indices, as initially expected. All significant results for threat acquisition are presented in online Supplementary Table S4 in Supplemental Materials.
Threat extinction
Self-reported fear
RM-ANCOVA of reported fear towards conditioned cues with Stimulus (CS+, CS−) × Block (pre-extinction, block 1, block 2, block 3, block 4, block 5, block 6) × Age (adolescents, adults) × Anxiety, yielded a significant Stimulus × Block × Anxiety interaction, F (2.14, 239.65) = 4.01, p = 0.017, ηp2 = 0.035. Participants with low- and high-anxiety levels exhibited a significant Stimulus × Block interaction, F (2.43, 126.22) = 10.21, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.164, and F (1.91, 116.07) = 43.16, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.414, respectively. Follow-up t tests comparing differential responses to CSs (CS+ v. CS−) in extinction blocks (Bonferroni-corrected for seven tests; corrected alpha = 0.007) indicated complete diminishing of differential self-reported fear (CS+ not different than CS−) earlier in the low-anxiety group (blocks 5–6; ps ⩾ 0.012) than in the high-anxiety group (block 6, p = 0.009); see Fig. 3a. A lower-order main effect of block was also observed (see online Supplemental Materials). Additional lower-order effects are reported in online Supplementary Table S5 in the supplemental materials.
Skin conductance response (SCR)
RM-ANCOVA of SCR testing the Stimulus × Block × Age × Anxiety effect yielded a significant Stimulus × Block interaction, F (2.93,255.14) = 19.77, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.185. Follow-up t tests examining differential SCR (CS+ v. CS−) per block (Bonferroni-corrected for six tests, alpha = 0.0083) indicated a gradual decrease in differential response across extinction but without complete extinction (all ps ⩽ 0.006), in contrast to our initial hypothesis; see Fig. 3b. A lower-order main effect of block was also observed (see online Supplemental Materials). Other lower-order effects are reported in online Supplementary Table S5 in the supplemental materials.
Late positive potential (LPP)
The RM-ANCOVA testing the Stimulus × Block × Age × Anxiety effect yielded a significant Stimulus × Block × Age interaction, F (2,172) = 3.16, p = 0.045, ηp2 = 0.035. To reveal the source of this three-way interaction, we ran follow-up analyses comparing differential LPP for adolescents and adults, in each of the three extinction blocks (blocks 1&2, blocks 3&4, and blocks 5&6).
Blocks 1&2. RM-ANOVA with Stimulus (CS+, CS−) × Age (adolescents, adults) revealed a main effect of stimulus, showing participants demonstrated greater LPP towards the CS+ (M = 3.91, s.d. = 2.84) than the CS− (M = 3.05, s.d. = 2.51), F (1,88) = 10.14, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.103. A main effect of age emerged, F (1,88) = 16.94, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.161, suggesting adolescents (M = 4.38, s.d. = 2.17) demonstrated overall greater LPP than adults (M = 2.50, s.d. = 2.16) during the first block of extinction. The Stimulus × Age interaction was not significant, F (1,88) = 0.18, p = 0.674, ηp2 = 0.002.
Block 3&4. RM-ANOVA with Stimulus (CS+, CS−) × Age (adolescents, adults) revealed a main effect of stimulus, suggesting participants demonstrated greater LPP towards the CS+ (M = 3.61, s.d. = 2.90) than the CS− (M = 2.51, s.d. = 2.51), F (1,89) = 17.44, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.164. A main effect of age emerged, F (1,89) = 5.06, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.054, suggesting adolescents (M = 3.59, s.d. = 2.36) demonstrated overall greater LPP than adults (M = 2.47, s.d. = 2.35) during the second block of extinction. The Stimulus × Age interaction was not significant, F (1,89) = 1.27, p = 0.262, ηp2 = 0.014.
Block 5&6. RM-ANOVA with Stimulus (CS+, CS−) × Age (adolescents, adults) revealed a two-way interaction, F (1,88) = 5.55, p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.059. Follow-up analysis for each age group revealed a main effect of stimulus only among adolescents, F (1,47) = 11.70, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.199, with greater LPP towards the CS+ (M = 4.11, s.d. = 3.06) than the CS− (M = 2.52, s.d. = 3.29), even during the last blocks of extinction, thus supporting our first hypothesis. In contrast, adults showed no main effect of stimulus, F (1,41) = 0.92, p = 0.344, ηp2 = 0.022, suggesting complete extinction of the threat contingencies. For waveform and topographical presentations of the LPP across extinction, see Fig. 4. Additional lower-order effects are reported in online Supplementary Table S5 in the supplemental materials.
To summarize, in line with our first hypothesis, adolescents and adults showed comparable extinction learning as indexed by self-reported fear and SCR. Contrary to our hypothesis, participants did not exhibit complete extinction as indexed by SCR. Confirming our second hypothesis, developmental differences emerged at the neural level, with persistent differential LPP responses in adolescents compared to adults. Finally, supporting our third hypothesis, individuals with higher anxiety levels demonstrated slower extinction, particularly in self-reported fear, but this anxiety effect was not observed in other threat-related measures. All significant results for threat extinction are presented in online Supplementary Table S5 in Supplemental Materials.
Moderating effect of age on the association between anxiety levels and extinction learning
In addition to the pre-registered analyses above, we examined exploratory moderation models to assess the possible moderating effect of age on the association between anxiety severity (measured by SCARED/SCAARED Z scores) and the extent of threat extinction learning. The latter was quantified for all dependent variables as the difference between responses to CS+ and CS− during the final block of extinction. To maximize the sensitivity of the moderation analyses, we used age as a continuous variable. For LPP, results revealed a significant age moderation effect, B = −0.208, F (1,84) = 11.71, p = 0.001. Post-hoc analyses showed more severe anxiety was associated with diminished extinction in the youngest group (one standard deviation below the mean age; 14.15 years), B = 2.34, t = 4.63, p < 0.001, and middle group (around the mean age; 19.55 years), B = 1.22, t = 3.43, p < 0.001, but not the older group (one standard deviation above the mean age; 24.94 years), B = 0.10, t = 0.21, p = 0.835; see Fig. 5.
No moderation effects emerged for self-reported fear or SCR, all ps ⩾ 0.476. These results suggest that age-dependent associations between anxiety severity and extinction learning manifest in LPP amplitude.
Association between self-reports, psychophysiological measures, and neural measures during threat acquisition and extinction
We conducted exploratory correlation analyses to assess associations among the dependent variables (self-reports, psychophysiological measures, neural measures) during acquisition and extinction. Notable among these were significant correlations among self-reported fear, SCR, and LPP elicited by threat (CS+) during extinction (ps ⩽ 0.036), suggesting convergence in responses across cognitive, psychophysiological, and neural levels during extinction (online Supplementary Tables S6 and S7, Supplemental Materials). We emphasize that these findings are strictly exploratory and meant to guide future research.
Discussion
This study examined differences between adolescents and adults in their responses to conditioned threat, assessed at cognitive, psychophysiological, and neural levels, focusing on extinction deficits as indicated by LPP. Supporting our first hypothesis, both adolescents and adults showed threat differentiation in LPP during acquisition indicated by enhanced responses towards the CS+ compared to the CS−, in line with prior work in adults (Bacigalupo & Luck, Reference Bacigalupo and Luck2018; Ferreira de Sá et al., Reference Ferreira de Sá, Michael, Wilhelm and Peyk2019; Panitz et al., Reference Panitz, Hermann and Mueller2015; Seligowski et al., Reference Seligowski, Bondy, Singleton, Orcutt, Ressler and Auerbach2018; Sperl et al., Reference Sperl, Wroblewski, Mueller, Straube and Mueller2021). This suggests differential threat acquisition is evident already in adolescents as indexed by LPP. Moreover, adolescents in our sample exhibited overall enhanced LPP compared to adults, possibly suggesting increased general context-dependent sensitivity to potential threat cues in this age group (Abend, Reference Abend2023). The overall heightened LPP responses that emerged during acquisition are in line with the findings of a recent magnetoencephalogram (MEG) study, showing overall enhanced activation among adolescents compared to adults during fear learning and generalization in time windows parallel to the LPP (Roesmann et al., Reference Roesmann, Wessing, Kraß, Leehr, Klucken, Straube and Junghöfer2022). A similar main effect of age was identified in a study examining developmental differences in early ERP components during a fear learning task (Linton & Levita, Reference Linton and Levita2021). These findings may reflect hyper-sensitivity to contexts of potential threat (e.g. probabilistic threat learning) among adolescents across different neural indices. At the same time, it is important to note that additional factors may have contributed to this age effect, such as a non-specific elevation in general arousal or attention levels among adolescents; alternatively, non-neural factors, such as skull thickness, could contribute to the observed higher neural amplitude in adolescents (Segalowitz, Santesso, & Jetha, Reference Segalowitz, Santesso and Jetha2010). Future work comparing age groups may wish to take such factors into consideration.
Alongside developmental differences in neural responses to threat cues, supporting our second hypothesis, adolescents exhibited a general elevated psychophysiological arousal to conditioned cues compared to adults, as well as greater threat-specific arousal, contrary to our initial expectations. Adolescents also demonstrated relatively slower habituation and sustained arousal to the US. Such findings are in line with previous results suggesting adolescents show overall higher SCR (i.e. to both threat and safety cues) than adults during threat acquisition (Linton & Levita, Reference Linton and Levita2021; Skversky-Blocq et al., Reference Skversky-Blocq, Shmuel, Waters and Shechner2022b) and increased differential SCR during acquisition (Abend et al., Reference Abend, Gold, Britton, Michalska, Shechner, Sachs and Pine2020). Slower habituation to the US among adolescents may reflect increased sensitivity and diminished ability to regulate psychophysiological arousal to aversive events in this age group, possibly contributing to stronger conditioning effects that are generalized to all conditioned cues. This, in turn, may contribute to the emergence of anxiety in adolescents. This explanation is supported by the extinction phase in which no US was presented, and age group differences in gradual decreases or differential SCR were no longer apparent. Age differences in LPP and elevated SCR may also be partially explained by a developmental difference in intolerance of uncertainty (IUC). While it was only a secondary measure in this study, we found adolescents reported higher IUC than adults (see online Supplemental Materials, Table S1). As IUC has been suggested to contribute to the development and maintenance of anxiety (Morriss, Zuj, & Mertens, Reference Morriss, Zuj and Mertens2021; Osmanağaoğlu, Creswell, & Dodd, Reference Osmanağaoğlu, Creswell and Dodd2018), future work could focus on the role of IUC in the context of an uninstructed threat learning paradigm which inherently involves some uncertainty as to the occurrence of the aversive outcome (US).
In line with our first hypothesis, developmental differences in LPP emerged during threat extinction. Unlike adults who showed a decrease in LPP differential responses until full extinction, adolescents showed persistent differential LPP responses, suggesting difficulty in extinguishing the learned threat contingency. Our finding for extinction in LPP is similar to previous studies reporting diminished LPP responses until complete extinction among adults (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Jackson and MacNamara2022; Ferreira de Sá et al., Reference Ferreira de Sá, Michael, Wilhelm and Peyk2019; Sperl et al., Reference Sperl, Wroblewski, Mueller, Straube and Mueller2021). Lack of complete extinction in adolescents may indicate difficulty adapting to changes in the emotional significance of the threatening cue when it no longer predicts any actual danger. A related developmental study reported that during extinction, adolescents did not show diminished early visual differential responses as measured by the N1 component, but adults did (Linton & Levita, Reference Linton and Levita2021). Moreover, an fMRI study focusing on developmental differences in extinction found adolescents, but not adults, showed increased amygdala activation towards the CS+ compared to the CS−, even during prolonged extinction learning (Morriss et al., Reference Morriss, Christakou and van Reekum2019).
From a clinical perspective, our study provides support to theories linking adolescence-specific perturbations in threat extinction and anxiety symptoms (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Glatt and Lee2015). Specifically, we found higher anxiety levels in adolescents were linked to less effective extinction, as revealed by sustained threat differentiation in LPP through the end of the extinction phase. Prior complementary imaging work found anxiety-related activations in key nodes of the fear circuitry differed in youth and adults (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Abend, Britton, Behrens, Farber, Ronkin and Pine2020). Stronger retention of conditioned threat responses in youth may therefore reflect an adaptive characteristic; it has been hypothesized to support survival at an age when the individual is biologically prepared to leave familial safety and attain reproductive success (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Glatt and Lee2015). However, individual variations in resistance to extinction may also lead to anxiety vulnerability, as many threat contingencies that were acquired incidentally and should have been normatively extinguished remain intact. Along these lines, in a recent study, clinically anxious adolescents exhibited sustained LPP differentiation compared to non-anxious counterparts during a delayed extinction task, and enhanced LPP differentiation was associated with poorer outcomes in exposure-based therapy (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Shner-livne, Danon-kraun, Ginat-frolich, Pine and Shechner2023). Thus, our findings underscore adolescence as a specific developmental period susceptible to variations in threat extinction which may impact both anxiety vulnerability and response to treatment. Treatment approaches that address this issue may therefore be warranted, although it is important to emphasize that additional research is needed to replicate and extend our findings to clinical populations.
Our findings suggest that while the decrease in psychophysiological response during extinction was comparable in adolescents and adults, LPP revealed distinct age-related extinction patterns. In line with prior research highlighting different facets of threat responses across various measures (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Abend, Shmuel and Shechner2022; Newsome, Ruiz, Gold, Pine, & Abend, Reference Newsome, Ruiz, Gold, Pine and Abend2023), our results emphasize that SCR, reflecting arousal in preparation for behavioral action, and LPP, a correlate of the emotional significance of a stimulus, quantify different aspects of the fear response mechanisms (Abend, Reference Abend2023; Bacigalupo & Luck, Reference Bacigalupo and Luck2018). The relatively sustained differential LPP we found for adolescents but not adults suggests a potentially more sensitive measure for exploring age-related differences during extinction learning.
The study had several limitations. The threat learning task was extended to accommodate EEG recording (requiring a larger number of trials) and SCR measures (necessitating a longer ITI). This extended timeframe might have made it challenging for some participants to maintain focus, resulting in increased movements and artifacts; indeed, approximately 20% of participants were excluded due to artifactual data and technical issues. This may have been related to the experimental design and may have limited our ability to detect certain statistical effects, particularly given our complex models and multiple variables. Furthermore, repeated exposure to the US could also lead to habituation, impacting learning (Abend et al., Reference Abend, Burk, Ruiz, Gold, Napoli, Britton and Averbeck2022). At the same time, the task design enabled the integration of multiple measures, allowed the consolidation of acquisition (over a 24-h period), and afforded a longer extinction period, consistent with recent recommendations (Treanor et al., Reference Treanor, Rosenberg and Craske2021). Finally, we averaged the LPP trials across acquisition blocks and every two blocks during extinction to enable a sufficient signal-to-noise ratio. However, doing so hindered direct comparisons with SCR and self-reports that were analyzed using block-by-block design. This is an inherent limitation when integrating different physiological measures, particularly EEG, which require different numbers of trials to achieve a valid measure.
In conclusion, our results underline the capacity of LPP to capture developmental differences in threat learning, with the potential to identify neural processes that render adolescents more susceptible to heightened fear responses and thus vulnerability to anxiety symptomatology. This is particularly relevant given the growing evidence of distinct LPP responses among anxious adolescents and their predictive value in exposure-based treatment outcomes (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Shner-livne, Danon-kraun, Ginat-frolich, Pine and Shechner2023). These findings encourage a continued focus on the LPP in the context of clinical research on pediatric anxiety and its treatment.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001314
Acknowledgements
The study was funded by the Israel Science Foundation (ISF-920/19, TS) and the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (Young Investigator award, RA).
Competing interests
The authors declare no conflict of interest.