Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T01:32:47.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is association of preterm birth with cognitive-neurophysiological impairments and ADHD symptoms consistent with a causal inference or due to familial confounds?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2019

Sarah-Naomi James*
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK MRC Lifelong Health and Ageing Unit at UCL, University College London, London, UK
Anna-Sophie Rommel
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
Fruhling Rijsdijk
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
Giorgia Michelini
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
Gráinne McLoughlin
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
Daniel Brandeis
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland Center for Integrative Human Physiology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Tobias Banaschewski
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Philip Asherson
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
Jonna Kuntsi
Affiliation:
King's College London, Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Sarah-Naomi James, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Preterm birth is associated with an increased risk for cognitive-neurophysiological impairments and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Whether the associations are due to the preterm birth insult per se, or due to other risk factors that characterise families with preterm-born children, is largely unknown.

Methods

We employed a within-sibling comparison design, using cognitive-performance and event-related potential (ERP) measures from 104 preterm-born adolescents and 104 of their term-born siblings. Analyses focused on ADHD symptoms and cognitive and ERP measures from a cued continuous performance test, an arrow flanker task and a reaction time task.

Results

Within-sibling analyses showed that preterm birth was significantly associated with increased ADHD symptoms (β = 0.32, p = 0.01, 95% CI 0.05 to 0.58) and specific cognitive-ERP impairments, such as IQ (β = −0.20, p = 0.02, 95% CI −0.40 to −0.01), preparation-vigilance measures and measures of error processing (ranging from β = 0.71, −0.35). There was a negligible within-sibling association between preterm birth with executive control measures of inhibition (NoGo-P3, β = −0.07, p = 0.45, 95% CI −0.33 to 0.15) or verbal working memory (digit span backward, β = −0.05, p = 0.63, 95% CI −0.30 to 0.18).

Conclusions

Our results suggest that the relationship between preterm birth with ADHD symptoms and specific cognitive-neurophysiological impairments (IQ, preparation-vigilance and error processing) is independent of family-level risk and consistent with a causal inference. In contrast, our results suggest that previously observed associations between preterm birth with executive control processes of inhibition and working memory are instead linked to background characteristics of families with a preterm-born child rather than preterm birth insult per se. These findings suggest that interventions need to target both preterm-birth specific and family-level risk factors.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Preterm birth occurs in 8.6% of births in developed countries (Blencowe et al., Reference Blencowe, Cousens, Oestergaard, Chou, Moller, Narwal, Adler, Vera Garcia, Rohde, Say and Lawn2012), and has many known risk factors such as low socio-economic status, low maternal educational status, maternal pre-existing health problems and maternal genetic risk (Goldenberg et al., Reference Goldenberg, Cliver, Mulvihill, Hickey, Hoffman, Klerman and Johnson1996, Reference Goldenberg, Culhane, Iams and Romero2008; Plunkett and Muglia, Reference Plunkett and Muglia2008; Blencowe et al., Reference Blencowe, Cousens, Oestergaard, Chou, Moller, Narwal, Adler, Vera Garcia, Rohde, Say and Lawn2012). Whilst survival rates of preterm-born children are improving, many negative long-term outcomes have been implicated, including academic difficulties (Moster et al., Reference Moster, Lie and Markestad2008), cognitive-neurophysiological impairments (Potgieter et al., Reference Potgieter, Vervisch and Lagae2003; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Wolke, Hennessy and Marlow2011; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Yeatman, Luna and Feldman2011; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017) and an increased risk for neurodevelopmental disorders (D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013), in particular attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Bhutta et al., Reference Bhutta, Cleves, Casey, Cradock and Anand2002).

Whether the association between preterm birth and the negative outcomes is due to the preterm birth insult per se, or due to other environmental or genetic risk factors that characterise families with preterm-born children (including individuals born at term), is difficult to disentangle as preterm-born children have often been compared to unrelated controls who may have differed on unmeasured risk factors (Thapar and Rutter, Reference Thapar and Rutter2009). While twin study designs are not well suited to address this issue given that twins are typically exposed to the same birth event, using non-twin siblings in a sibling-comparison design allows the comparison of preterm birth outcomes whilst controlling for all familial risk factors shared by siblings, such as socio-economic status and maternal genetic risk (Donovan and Susser, Reference Donovan and Susser2011; D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013, Reference D'onofrio, Class, Lahey and Larsson2014; Skoglund et al., Reference Skoglund, Chen, D'Onofrio, Lichtenstein and Larsson2014). If there are within-sibling associations between preterm birth and specific outcomes, which controls for unmeasured familial confounding factors, this is consistent with a causal effect of preterm birth. Using this approach, a large Swedish epidemiological study found a dose-response relationship between earlier gestational age (GA) and increased likelihood of an ADHD diagnosis, independent of familial confounds, in line with a causal inference (D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013). However, no study, to our knowledge, has applied the sibling-comparison approach to investigate cognitive and neurophysiological impairments associated with preterm birth.

We recently performed detailed investigations of the cognitive-neurophysiological impairments in preterm-born adolescents, when compared to unrelated term-born control adolescents, and, in order to help understand the increased risk of ADHD in preterm-born individuals, to term-born adolescents with ADHD (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019). Compared to the unrelated control group, the preterm group showed impairments that were similar to those observed in the ADHD group in decreased working memory, IQ, cognitive measures of preparation-vigilance [mean reaction time (MRT) and reaction time variability (RTV)], event-related potential (ERP) activity of response preparation, response inhibition, conflict monitoring and error processing (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019). The preterm group was further uniquely impaired on ERP activity of executive response control, compared to both ADHD and control groups (Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017). In addition, in a simple reaction time task, the introduction of rewards and a faster event rate produced greater improvements from baseline (slow, unrewarded) conditions in both ADHD and control groups in ERP measures of attention allocation and response preparation, but not in the preterm group (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017). Overall, preterm-born adolescents showed both ADHD-like and unique impairments, specific to preterm-born individuals, in cognitive-neurophysiological processes.

We have available new data from term-born siblings of the same preterm-born adolescents whom we previously compared to unrelated controls (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019). By comparing the preterm-born adolescents to their term-born siblings, we now aim to test the within-sibling associations of preterm birth with increased ADHD symptoms and the specific cognitive-neurophysiological impairments, to investigate if the relationships are independent of unmeasured familial factors shared by siblings.

Method

Sample

The preterm group was recruited from secondary schools in South East England, and was mainly Caucasian (84.6%) (Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017). Preterm participants had one full sibling available for ascertainment and were born before 37 weeks' gestation. Exclusion criteria for all groups were IQ < 70, cerebral palsy or any other medical conditions that affect motor co-ordination, as well as brain disorders and any genetic or medical disorder that might mimic ADHD. Seven individuals from the preterm sample were excluded because medical birth records could not corroborate preterm status (GA ⩾ 37 weeks). One individual was excluded due to IQ < 70. The total eligible sample consisted of 145 sibling pairs (n = 290 participants).

For descriptive purposes, in line with previous studies (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Rijsdijk, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2016; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017), a research diagnosis of ADHD was made if participants scored six or more on either the inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity parent-rated subscales of The Diagnostic Interview for ADHD in Adults (Kooij and Francken, Reference Kooij and Francken2007), and if they received two or more positive scores on two or more areas of impairment on the parent-rated Barkley Functional Impairment Scale (Barkley and Murphy, Reference Barkley and Murphy2006). Seventeen preterm-born participants and seven term-born siblings met our criteria for a research ADHD diagnosis. Two of the preterm-born participants were taking ADHD medication, but a 48-h ADHD medication-free period was required prior to assessments.

The present analysis only uses siblings discordant for preterm birth. Therefore, the final sample in this analysis consisted of 208 participants: 104 preterm-born participants (GA ranged from 23 weeks to 36) and their 104 term-born siblings (GA ranged from 37 to 42 weeks). On average, the siblings in a pair were discordant by 5.97 (s.d. = 3.00) weeks of gestation. Of the sibling pairs, 45% (n = 47) were same-sex siblings. In total, 55% of the preterm-born siblings were older than their term-born sibling. A 48-h ADHD medication-free period was required prior to assessments. Written informed consent was obtained following procedures approved by the National Research Ethics Service Committee London – Bromley (13/LO/0068).

Procedure

Participants attended a single 4.5 h research session, which included an electroencephalogram (EEG) assessment with cognitive tests and clinical interviews. As part of the EEG assessment, participants completed a cued continuous performance test (CPT) with flankers (CPT-OX) (Doehnert et al., Reference Doehnert, Brandeis, Imhof, Drechsler and Steinhausen2010), an arrow flanker task with congruent (low-conflict) and incongruent (high-conflict) conditions (Albrecht et al., Reference Albrecht, Brandeis, Uebel, Heinrich, Mueller, Hasselhorn, Steinhausen, Rothenberger and Banaschewski2008; McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Albrecht, Banaschewski, Rothenberger, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2009, Reference McLoughlin, Palmer, Rijsdijk and Makeig2014) and a four-choice reaction time task with two conditions, the fast task (Kuntsi et al., Reference Kuntsi, Andreou, Ma, Börger and van der Meere2005; Andreou et al., Reference Andreou, Neale, Chen, Christiansen, Gabriels, Heise, Meidad, Muller, Uebel, Banaschewski, Manor, Oades, Roeyers, Rothenberger, Sham, Steinhausen, Asherson and Kuntsi2007). Face-to-face clinical interviews were administered to the parent of each participant shortly before or after the participant's assessment.

Measures

Gestational age

GA was obtained from Personal Child Health Records (also known in the UK as the ‘red book’), the national standard development record given to parents. Preterm birth was considered as <37 gestational weeks and term birth was considered ⩾37 weeks.

IQ and digit span

The vocabulary and block design subtests of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) were administered to all participants to derive estimates of IQ. The digit span subtest from the WISC-III was administered to obtain digit span forward (DSF) and backwards (DSB) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1991). DSB was used in this analysis and required the participant to verbally repeat a sequence of digits in the backwards order, and is a measure of verbal working memory.

ADHD symptoms

Parents were asked to rate the behaviour of each sibling using the Revised Conners' Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R) (Conners et al., Reference Conners, Sitarenios, Parker and Epstein1998). The CPRS-R has two DSM-IV symptom sub-scales (inattentiveness and hyperactivity-impulsivity), each consisting of nine items that map onto DSM-IV criteria. The sum of all 18 items calculates a total DSM-IV ADHD symptom score with a greater score indicating more ADHD symptoms.

Cued continuous performance test (CPT-OX)

The CPT-OX is a cued Go/NoGo task that probes attention, preparation and response inhibition (Doehnert et al., Reference Doehnert, Brandeis, Imhof, Drechsler and Steinhausen2010). The task consisted of 400 black letter arrays, made up of a centre letter and incompatible flankers on each side to increase difficulty. Cue and target letters (‘O’ and ‘X’ respectively) were flanked by incompatible letters (‘XOX’ and ‘OXO’ respectively). Participants were instructed to ignore the flanking letters and respond as quickly as possible to only cue-target sequences (‘O’-‘X’). Eighty cues (‘XOX’) were followed by the target (‘OXO’) in 40 trials (Go condition), and by neutral distractors in the remainder of trials (NoGo condition).

The arrow flanker task

The task was an adaptation of the Eriksen flanker paradigm designed to increase cognitive load as used in previous studies (Albrecht et al., Reference Albrecht, Brandeis, Uebel, Heinrich, Mueller, Hasselhorn, Steinhausen, Rothenberger and Banaschewski2008; McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Albrecht, Banaschewski, Rothenberger, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2009, Reference McLoughlin, Palmer, Rijsdijk and Makeig2014). In each trial, a central black fixation mark was replaced by a target arrow (a black 18 mm equilateral triangle). Participants had to indicate whether this arrow pointed towards the left or right by pressing corresponding response buttons with their left or right index fingers. Both flankers either pointed in the same (congruent) or opposite (incongruent) direction to the target. As such, conflict monitoring is maximal during the incongruent condition.

The fast task

Participants performed a four-choice RT task with a baseline condition (72 trials) with four empty circles (warning signals, arranged horizontally) first appearing for 8000 ms, after which one of them (the target) was coloured in (Andreou et al., Reference Andreou, Neale, Chen, Christiansen, Gabriels, Heise, Meidad, Muller, Uebel, Banaschewski, Manor, Oades, Roeyers, Rothenberger, Sham, Steinhausen, Asherson and Kuntsi2007). Participants were asked to press the response key that directly corresponded to the position of the target. Following a response, the stimuli disappeared from the screen and a fixed inter-trial interval of 2.5 s followed. A comparison condition with a fast event rate (1 s) and incentives followed the baseline condition (Andreou et al., Reference Andreou, Neale, Chen, Christiansen, Gabriels, Heise, Meidad, Muller, Uebel, Banaschewski, Manor, Oades, Roeyers, Rothenberger, Sham, Steinhausen, Asherson and Kuntsi2007). Cognitive-performance measures of MRT, and RTV (s.d. of RTs) were calculated for each condition.

EEG recording

The EEG was recorded from 62 channels DC-coupled recording system (extended 10–20 montage), with a 500 Hz sampling rate, impedances kept under 10 kΩ, and FCz as the recording reference electrode. The electrooculograms were recorded from electrodes above and below the left eye and at the outer canthi. The EEG data were analysed using a Brain Vision Analyzer (2.0) (Brain Products, Germany). After down-sampling the data to 256 Hz, the EEG data were re-referenced to the average (turning FCz into an active electrode) and filtered offline with digital band-pass (0.1 to 30 Hz, 24 dB/oct) Butterworth filters. Ocular artefacts were identified and removed from the data using Independent Component Analysis [ICA (Jung et al., Reference Jung, Makeig, Humphries, Lee, McKeown, Iragui and Sejnowski2000)]. All ERP averages contained at least 20 accepted sweeps. ERPs were extracted from the CPT-OX (CNV, Go-P3, NoGo-P3), flanker task (N2, Pe, ERN, incongruent condition only) and fast task (CNV amplitude and P3 amplitude) following procedures used on previous analyses of this sample (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019); see online Supplementary Material I for further details.

Statistical analyses

ADHD symptoms and cognitive performance and event-related potential (ERP) measures which we have previously shown to be impaired in the preterm group (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019), compared to unrelated term-born controls, are included in this analysis (partly replicated in Table 2; see online Supplementary Material V for details about the unrelated term-born control group). We additionally investigated within-sibling associations for ADHD symptom sub-scales (inattentiveness and hyperactivity-impulsivity).

In order to reduce the number of statistical comparisons, in this analysis we only chose one electrode where the previous preterm-control differences were maximal (instead of electrodes at multiple sites), and consequently analysed Go-P3 from Pz only, CNV at CPz only in the CPT-OX, and N2 at Fz only. In addition, in the fast task, cognitive performance measures of MRT and RTV from the baseline condition only are included in this analysis, as the baseline condition is more sensitive to preterm-control differences in cognitive performance (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017). Similarly, change measures in P3 and CNV from the fast task, from a baseline (slow, unrewarded) condition to a faster condition with rewards, were also omitted due to their high correlation with P3 and CNV in the fast-incentive condition. According to these criteria the following measures were retained for inclusion: IQ, verbal working memory (DSB); response preparation (CNV at CPz), executive response control (Go-P3 at Pz), response inhibition (NoGo-P3 at Cz) from the CPT-OX; conflict monitoring (N2 at Fz), conscious error processing (Pe at CPz), automatic error processing (ERN at FCz), from the flanker task; MRT and RTV (baseline condition), response preparation (CNV at CPz in the fast-incentive condition) and attention allocation (P3 at Pz in the fast-incentive condition) from the fast task.

The relationship of preterm birth with cognitive and ERP measures was investigated using a within-sibling fixed-effect design (Neuhaus and McCulloch, Reference Neuhaus and McCulloch2006; Lahey and D'Onofrio, Reference Lahey and D'Onofrio2010; Donovan and Susser, Reference Donovan and Susser2011; D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013), which models within-sibling pair differences in cognitive and neurophysiological measures as a function of within-pair differences of preterm birth, allowing the effect of preterm birth on cognitive and neurophysiological measures to be estimated while accounting for unmeasured confounding factors (i.e. all genetic and environmental factors that make siblings alike). Preterm birth was first studied as a dichotomous variable (preterm birth: born before <37 weeks gestation). See Table 1 for the overall sample mean and standard deviations (s.d.), and see online Supplementary Material II for group-level (term and preterm) means, but note this is not a group analysis. Models were fitted to standardised (z) cognitive and neurophysiological measures so that beta coefficients represent a standardised effect size measure. Therefore, one-unit change for the measure of preterm birth leads to a beta change in standard deviation in cognitive-neurophysiological measures. The effect size for the dichotomous analysis is comparable to Cohen's d. All analyses were conducted in Stata 13 software. Age and sex effects were regressed out from all analyses as is standard practice for quantitative family studies (Bouchard et al., Reference Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal and Tellegen1990). IQ and birth order were used as additional covariates (online Supplementary Material III–IV).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics: means and standard deviations () for the overall sample

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; DSB, digit span backwards; MRT, mean reaction time in the baseline (slow, unrewarded) condition of the fast task; RTV, reaction time variability in the baseline (slow, unrewarded) condition of the fast task; CNV, contingent negative variation; Go-P3 = P3 amplitude; NoGo-P3 = P3 amplitude; N2 = N2 amplitude; Pe, positive related negativity in the incongruent condition; ERN, error-related negativity in the incongruent condition; CNV, contingent negative variation amplitude in the fast-incentive condition; P3 = P3 amplitude in the fast-incentive condition; ms, milliseconds, μV, microvolts.

Role of the funding source

The funder of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation or writing of the report. The corresponding author confirms that she has full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

Results

The within-sibling comparisons, which compare the preterm group to their term-born siblings, are new and the focus here but, for ease of comparison and completeness, we also report the statistics from comparisons between the preterm group and an unrelated control group (Table 2) [partly previously reported in James et al. (Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017) and Rommel et al. (Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019)].

Table 2. Standardised regression coefficients of the association between preterm birth, ADHD symptoms and standardised cognitive-event-related potential measures (controlling for age and sex). Preterm-probands are compared to (A) term-born siblings in a within-sibling fixed effect model and (B) unrelated controls in a linear regression model [originally partly reported in James et al. (Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017) and Rommel et al. (Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019)].

Note: p < 0.05 indicated in bold.

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; DSB, digit span backwards; MRT, mean reaction time in the baseline (slow, unrewarded) condition of the fast task; RTV, reaction time variability in the baseline (slow, unrewarded) condition of the fast task; CNV, contingent negative variation; Go-P3 = P3 amplitude; NoGo-P3 = P3 amplitude; N2 = N2 amplitude; Pe, positive related negativity in the incongruent condition; ERN, error-related negativity in the incongruent condition; CNV, contingent negative variation amplitude in the fast-incentive condition; P3 = P3 amplitude in the fast-incentive condition; ms, milliseconds, μV, microvolts.

Models were fitted to standardised (z) cognitive and neurophysiological measures so that beta coefficients presented represent a standardised effect size measure.

Within-sibling comparisons showed that those who were born preterm were more likely to have increased parent-rated total ADHD symptoms [and on both inattentiveness (β = 0.34, 95% CI 0.07 to 0.60) and hyperactivity-impulsivity (β = 0.24, 95% CI 0.04 to 0.51) ADHD symptom sub-scales]; lower IQ, as well as decreased CNV amplitude and decreased Go-P3 amplitude on the CPT-OX task, decreased N2 amplitude and decreased Pe and ERN on the flanker task, increased MRT and RTV in the baseline condition of fast task, and decreased CNV amplitude and P3 amplitude on the fast-incentive condition of fast task, independent of familial factors (Table 2). Unlike the unrelated-comparisons, within-sibling comparisons demonstrated that those who were born preterm were not more likely to be impaired on DSB (β = −0.05, 95% CI −0.30 to 0.18) or NoGo-P3 amplitude (β = −0.07, 95% CI −0.33 to 0.15) on the CPT-OX, compared to their term-born siblings; these within-sibling associations were therefore largely attenuated compared to the unrelated comparisons (Table 2). Post-hoc comparisons between term-born siblings of the preterm-probands and unrelated controls further demonstrated that, after adjusting for sex and age, the term-born siblings had lower DSB (β = −0.48, 95% CI −0.74 to −0.21) and NoGo-P3 amplitude (β = −0.55, 95% CI −0.83 to −0.26), compared to the unrelated control group; the results remained similar when IQ was additionally adjusted (β = −0.56, 95% CI −0.85 to −0.28 and β = −0.53, 95% CI −0.83 to −0.24, respectively).

The pattern of results remained similar when IQ and birth order were included as additional covariates (online Supplementary Material III–IV).

Discussion

In this sibling-comparison study of adolescents, we find evidence for significant associations of preterm birth with increased ADHD symptoms, including both inattentiveness and hyperactivity-impulsivity sub-scales, and with specific cognitive-neurophysiological impairments, including IQ, preparation-vigilance processes (RTV, MRT, CNV) and error and conflict processing (N2, Pe, ERN), which are independent of familial factors shared by siblings, indicating that preterm birth is likely in the causal pathway leading to these identified impairments. In contrast, the association of preterm birth with executive control measures of working memory (DSB) and inhibition (NoGo-P3) was not independent of familial factors, indicating that these previously obtained associations (James et al., Reference James, Rommel, Cheung, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017; Rommel et al., Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Brandeis, Banaschewski, Asherson and Kuntsi2017, Reference Rommel, James, McLoughlin, Michelini, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi2019) are more likely to be due to other characteristics that differentiate families with a preterm-born child from other families, rather than preterm birth per se.

The robust association between preterm birth and increased ADHD symptoms, when controlling for unmeasured familial confounding factors, is in line with a previous finding using this sibling-comparison approach (D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013). Whilst multiple studies have indicated that preterm birth is associated with decreased IQ (Kerr-Wilson et al., Reference Kerr-Wilson, Mackay, Smith and Pell2012), we show that this relationship is independent of potential shared familial confounds such as socio-economic status (Goldenberg et al., Reference Goldenberg, Culhane, Iams and Romero2008), consistent with a causal inference. We also obtained evidence of independent associations of preterm birth with cognitive and neurophysiological measures indexing preparation vigilance processes (MRT, RTV, CNV), response execution and attention allocation (Go-P3, P3), conflict monitoring (N2) and error processing (Pe, ERN), when controlling for unmeasured familial confounding factors. Future research should explore the specific mechanisms whereby preterm birth leads to these cognitive-neurophysiological impairments and increased ADHD symptoms. For example, as brain connections strengthen throughout the third trimester (29 to 40 weeks gestation) (Ball et al., Reference Ball, Aljabar, Zebari, Tusor, Arichi, Merchant, Robinson, Ogundipe, Rueckert, Edwards and Counsell2014; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Kersbergen, de Reus, Keunen, Kahn, Groenendaal, de Vries and Benders2014), giving birth prematurely could feasibly result in disruption of developing brain networks associated with ADHD, as well as disruption of other networks associated with additional impairments. In order to help identify and provide support for those at risk, our findings support the notion that there should be greater awareness amongst medical and educational professionals about the increased risk of cognitive-neurophysiological impairments in people born preterm (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Beer, Wolke and Johnson2012; Brogan et al., Reference Brogan, Cragg, Gilmore, Marlow, Simms and Johnson2014; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Hollis, Marlow, Simms and Wolke2014). It is striking that even within our sample, who were a relatively well-functioning sample recruited from mainstream schools, impairments are still observed in preterm-born individuals at least a decade after the preterm birth event.

In contrast, we showed no significant associations between preterm birth and decreased verbal working memory (DSB) and attenuated response inhibition (NoGo-P3) when controlling for unmeasured familial confounding factors. This suggests that familial factors shared by siblings, which include factors correlated with preterm birth (i.e. maternal genetic risk for giving birth preterm, socio-economic status, family upbringing, and other genetic and environmental factors that are shared by members of the same family), may account for these deviations with preterm birth previously observed in preterm individuals when compared to unrelated controls. The finding that working memory is not on the causal pathway from preterm birth is in line with results from a study which demonstrated that a combined measure of short-term auditory memory and verbal working memory (combining DSF and DSB) were not on the causal pathway between birth weight and ADHD symptoms in adolescents (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Loo and Lee2016). Future research should aim to identify the background risk factors that characterise families with a preterm-born child and account for the impairments that distinguish them from families without preterm-born children.

The present study is, to our knowledge, the first study investigating the effects of preterm birth on cognitive-neurophysiological measures in adolescents in a quasi-experimental sibling design, which is essential for drawing stronger causal inferences. However, as is the case for all such non-randomised quasi-experimental studies, we cannot rule out all confounding factors (D'Onofrio et al., Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013). Whilst we control for shared maternal risk factors for preterm birth, we were unable to investigate whether specific maternal risk factors (e.g. trauma, smoking) differed between pregnancies. As siblings only share ~50% of DNA, the sibling-comparison design does not control for sibling-specific genetic influences that could influence preterm birth and, as such, the causal role of genetic influences cannot be ruled out. However, studies have suggested a negligible role of the foetus' genotype in determining GA (Svensson et al., Reference Svensson, Sandin, Cnattingius, Reilly, Pawitan, Hultman and Lichtenstein2009). Further, whilst our adolescent sample offers a unique perspective, it would be informative to have follow-up assessments when all participants have reached adulthood.

In conclusion, our findings provide novel insight into the potential causal pathways to cognitive-neurophysiological impairments and increased ADHD symptoms in adolescents born preterm. By distinguishing impairments that are consistent with a causal inference of preterm birth from those that are instead linked to background characteristics of families with a preterm-born child, these results suggest that interventions need to target both preterm-birth specific and family-level risk factors to minimise impairments in those at risk. If replicated, clinical implications include raising the awareness about the increased risk of cognitive and brain impairments in preterm-born children; and understanding the increased risk of IQ and cognitive/brain impairments are likely causally related to preterm birth itself, whereas executive function impairments are via different causal mechanisms associated with families at risk.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719001211

Author ORCIDs

Sarah-Naomi James, 0000-0002-3800-4416.

Acknowledgements

The Study of Preterm birth and Inattention (SPIN) was supported by a generous grant from Action Medical Research (grant reference GN2080). Dr Sarah-Naomi James, Dr Anna-Sophie Rommel and Dr Giorgia Michelini were supported by the Medical Research Council studentships (G1242813, G9817803). PA is supported by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for mental health NIHR/MRC (14/23/17). The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report.

We thank all participants and their family for their time and effort as well as those individuals whose work made this research possible: Dr Celeste Cheung, Hannah Sims, Stacey Eyers, Rachel Sparrow. Special thanks to Jeffrey Dalton from the Neuroimaging department for providing the SCanalyse program.

Author contributions

Sarah-Naomi James: Acquisition of data, statistical analysis, interpretation of data and drafting of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Anna-Sophie Rommel: Acquisition of data, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Fruhling Rijsdijk: Statistical advice and critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Giorgia Michelini: Acquisition of data, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Gráinne McLoughlin: Interpretation of data and critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Daniel Brandeis: Interpretation of data and critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Tobias Banaschewski: Interpretation of data and critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Philip Asherson: Study concept and design, acquisition of data, critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Jonna Kuntsi: Study concept and design, acquisition of data, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for intellectual content.

Conflict of interest

Professor Asherson has acted in an advisory role for Shire, Janssen-Cilag, Eli Lilly and Flynn Pharma. He has received education or research grants from Shire, Janssen-Cilag and Eli-Lilly. He has given talks at educational events sponsored by the above companies. Professor Kuntsi has given talks at educational events sponsored by Medice; all funds are received by King's College London and used for studies of ADHD. Dr Banaschewski served in an advisory or consultancy role for Actelion, Hexal Pharma, Lilly, Lundbeck, Medice, Novartis, Shire. He received conference support or speaker's fee by Lilly, Medice, Novartis and Shire. He has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Shire & Viforpharma. He received royalties from Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, CIP Medien, Oxford University Press. The present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships. The other authors report no conflicts of interest.

References

Albrecht, B, Brandeis, D, Uebel, H, Heinrich, H, Mueller, UC, Hasselhorn, M, Steinhausen, H-C, Rothenberger, A and Banaschewski, T (2008) Action monitoring in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, their nonaffected siblings, and normal control subjects: evidence for an endophenotype. Biological Psychiatry 64, 615625.10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.12.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreou, P, Neale, BM, Chen, W, Christiansen, H, Gabriels, I, Heise, A, Meidad, S, Muller, UC, Uebel, H, Banaschewski, T, Manor, I, Oades, R, Roeyers, H, Rothenberger, A, Sham, P, Steinhausen, H-C, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2007) Reaction time performance in ADHD: improvement under fast-incentive condition and familial effects. Psychological Medicine 37, 17031715.10.1017/S0033291707000815CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ball, G, Aljabar, P, Zebari, S, Tusor, N, Arichi, T, Merchant, N, Robinson, EC, Ogundipe, E, Rueckert, D, Edwards, AD and Counsell, SJ (2014) Rich-club organization of the newborn human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111, 74567461.10.1073/pnas.1324118111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, RA and Murphy, KR (2006) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Clinical Workbook, 3rd Edn. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Bhutta, AT, Cleves, MA, Casey, PH, Cradock, MM and Anand, KJS (2002) Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association 288, 728737.10.1001/jama.288.6.728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blencowe, H, Cousens, S, Oestergaard, MZ, Chou, D, Moller, A-B, Narwal, R, Adler, A, Vera Garcia, C, Rohde, S, Say, L and Lawn, JEJ (2012) National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: a systematic analysis and implications. The Lancet 379, 21622172.10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60820-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouchard, TJ, Lykken, DT, McGue, M, Segal, NL and Tellegen, A (1990) Sources of human psychological differences – the Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science 250, 223228.10.1126/science.2218526CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brogan, E, Cragg, L, Gilmore, C, Marlow, N, Simms, V and Johnson, S (2014) Inattention in very preterm children: implications for screening and detection. Archives of Disease in Childhood 99, 834839.10.1136/archdischild-2013-305532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, CHM, Rijsdijk, F, McLoughlin, G, Brandeis, D, Banaschewski, T, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2016) Cognitive and neurophysiological markers of ADHD persistence and remission. The British Journal of Psychiatry 208, 548555.10.1192/bjp.bp.114.145185CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conners, CK, Sitarenios, G, Parker, JDA and Epstein, JN (1998) The Revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 26, 257268.10.1023/A:1022602400621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doehnert, M, Brandeis, D, Imhof, K, Drechsler, R and Steinhausen, H-C (2010) Mapping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from childhood to adolescence – no neurophysiologic evidence for a developmental lag of attention but some for inhibition. Biological Psychiatry 67, 608616.10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Onofrio, BM, Class, QA, Rickert, ME, Larsson, H, Långström, N and Lichtenstein, P (2013) Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: a population-based quasi-experimental study. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 12311240.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'onofrio, B, Class, Q, Lahey, B and Larsson, H (2014) Testing the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis for psychopathology using family-based, quasi-experimental designs. Child Development Perspectives 8, 151157.10.1111/cdep.12078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donovan, SJ and Susser, E (2011) Commentary: advent of sibling designs. International Journal of Epidemiology 40, 345349.10.1093/ije/dyr057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, RL, Cliver, SP, Mulvihill, FX, Hickey, CA, Hoffman, HJ, Klerman, LV and Johnson, MJ (1996) Medical, psychosocial, and behavioral risk factors do not explain the increased risk for low birth weight among black women. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 175, 13171324.10.1016/S0002-9378(96)70048-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldenberg, RL, Culhane, JF, Iams, JD and Romero, R (2008) Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. The Lancet 371, 7584.10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60074-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, D, Beer, C, Wolke, D and Johnson, S (2012) Supporting the schooling of very preterm children: education professionals’ opinions and information needs. Archives of Disease in Childhood 97, A353A353.Google Scholar
James, S-N, Rommel, A-S, Cheung, C, McLoughlin, G, Brandeis, D, Banaschewski, T, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2017) Association of preterm birth with ADHD-like cognitive impairments and additional subtle impairments in attention and arousal malleability. Psychological Medicine 48, 14841494.10.1017/S0033291717002963CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S, Wolke, D, Hennessy, E and Marlow, N (2011) Educational outcomes in extremely preterm children: neuropsychological correlates and predictors of attainment. Developmental Neuropsychology 36, 7495.10.1080/87565641.2011.540541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S, Hollis, C, Marlow, N, Simms, V and Wolke, D (2014) Screening for childhood mental health disorders using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: the validity of multi-informant reports. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 56, 453459.10.1111/dmcn.12360CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, TP, Makeig, S, Humphries, C, Lee, TW, McKeown, MJ, Iragui, V and Sejnowski, TJ (2000) Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology 37, 163178.10.1111/1469-8986.3720163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr-Wilson, CO, Mackay, DF, Smith, GCS and Pell, JP (2012) Meta-analysis of the association between preterm delivery and intelligence. Journal of Public Health 34, 209216.10.1093/pubmed/fdr024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kooij, JJS and Francken, MH (2007) Diagnostic Interview for ADHD (DIVA) in adults. www.divacentre.eu.Google Scholar
Kuntsi, J, Andreou, P, Ma, J, Börger, Na and van der Meere, JJ (2005) Testing assumptions for endophenotype studies in ADHD: reliability and validity of tasks in a general population sample. BMC Psychiatry 5, 40.10.1186/1471-244X-5-40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, BB and D'Onofrio, BM (2010) All in the family: comparing siblings to test causal hypotheses regarding environmental influences on behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19, 319323.10.1177/0963721410383977CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, ES, Yeatman, JD, Luna, B and Feldman, HM (2011) Specific language and reading skills in school-aged children and adolescents are associated with prematurity after controlling for IQ. Neuropsychologia 49, 906913.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLoughlin, G, Albrecht, B, Banaschewski, T, Rothenberger, A, Brandeis, D, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2009) Performance monitoring is altered in adult ADHD: a familial event-related potential investigation. Neuropsychologia 47, 31343142.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLoughlin, G, Palmer, JA, Rijsdijk, F and Makeig, S (2014) Genetic overlap between evoked frontocentral theta-band phase variability, reaction time variability, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a twin study. Biological Psychiatry 75, 238247.10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, JE, Loo, SK and Lee, SS (2016) Neurocognitive functioning mediates the prospective association of birth weight with youth ADHD symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 47, 727736.10.1080/15374416.2016.1183498CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moster, D, Lie, RT and Markestad, T (2008) Long-term medical and social consequences of preterm birth. The New England Journal of Medicine 359, 262273.10.1056/NEJMoa0706475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neuhaus, JM and McCulloch, CE (2006) Separating between- and within-cluster covariate effects by using conditional and partitioning methods. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B 68, 859872.10.1111/j.1467-9868.2006.00570.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, J and Muglia, LJ (2008) Genetic contributions to preterm birth: implications from epidemiological and genetic association studies. Annals of Medicine 40, 167195.10.1080/07853890701806181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potgieter, S, Vervisch, J and Lagae, L (2003) Event related potentials during attention tasks in VLBW children with and without attention deficit disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology 114, 18411849.10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00198-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rommel, A-S, James, S-N, McLoughlin, G, Brandeis, D, Banaschewski, T, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2017) Association of preterm birth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like and wider-ranging neurophysiological impairments of attention and inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 56, 4050.10.1016/j.jaac.2016.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rommel, A-S, James, S-N, McLoughlin, G, Michelini, G, Banaschewski, T, Brandeis, D, Asherson, P and Kuntsi, J (2019) Impairments in error processing and their association with ADHD symptoms in individuals born preterm. PLoS ONE 14, e0214864.10.1371/journal.pone.0214864CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoglund, C, Chen, Q, D'Onofrio, BM, Lichtenstein, P and Larsson, H (2014) Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD in offspring. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 55, 6168.10.1111/jcpp.12124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svensson, AC, Sandin, S, Cnattingius, S, Reilly, M, Pawitan, Y, Hultman, CM and Lichtenstein, P (2009) Maternal effects for preterm birth: a genetic epidemiologic study of 630000 families. American Journal of Epidemiology 170, 13651372.10.1093/aje/kwp328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A and Rutter, M (2009) Do prenatal risk factors cause psychiatric disorder? Be wary of causal claims. The British Journal of Psychiatry 195, 100101.10.1192/bjp.bp.109.062828CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, MP, Kersbergen, KJ, de Reus, MA, Keunen, K, Kahn, RS, Groenendaal, F, de Vries, LS and Benders, MJNL (2014) The neonatal connectome during preterm brain development. Cerebral Cortex 35, 30003013.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1991) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd edn) (WISC-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999) Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace & Company.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics: means and standard deviations () for the overall sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Standardised regression coefficients of the association between preterm birth, ADHD symptoms and standardised cognitive-event-related potential measures (controlling for age and sex). Preterm-probands are compared to (A) term-born siblings in a within-sibling fixed effect model and (B) unrelated controls in a linear regression model [originally partly reported in James et al. (2017) and Rommel et al. (2017, 2019)].

Supplementary material: File

James et al. supplementary material

James et al. supplementary material 1

Download James et al. supplementary material(File)
File 47.3 KB